Introduction

The Dark Triad, comprising psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and narcissism, represents a set of socially aversive and subclinical maladaptive personality characteristics. Psychopathy is characterized by high thrill-seeking, callousness, interpersonal antagonism, manipulation, and anti-social behavioral style (Hare and Neumann 2008). Machiavellianism is defined by self-interest and tendencies toward deceptiveness, exploitation and manipulation of others, cynical perspective on life and interpersonal relationships (Christie and Geis 1970). Finally, individuals high in Narcissism tend to focus largely on themselves, are characterized by self-absorption, dominance, and feelings of entitlement and grandiosity, as well as devaluation of others (Emmons 1987). There has been an asymptotic rise in papers investigating the utility of these traits (for a review, see Furnham et al. 2013). Recent papers have found that the Dark Triad traits are differentially informative in predicting workplace behaviors, interpersonal behaviors, mating behaviors, antisocial behaviors, aggression, and financial misbehaviors (e.g., Jones and Paulhus 2010; Lee and Ashton 2005; Malesza and Ostaszewski 2016a, b).

Measures of the Dark three Constructs

Most studies used three separate assessment tools for each of the Dark Triad traits, typically the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale, the Machiavellian Scale, and the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (for a review, see Furnham et al. 2013). Consequently, traditional measures of the Dark Triad together require approximately 100 items to complete. If time is limited, using concise measures can eliminate item redundancy, save time and effort, and thus reduce participant fatigue (Gosling et al. 2003). To meet the need for a brief assessment method, two short measures of the Dark Triad were developed: Short Dark Triad (Jones and Paulhus 2014) and the Dirty Dozen (Jonason and Webster 2010). The first measure comprises 27 items, whereas the second instrument includes 12 items. However, the second tool – the Dirty Dozen – is controversial. Although some researchers have found it to be useful (Jonason and Luévano 2013; Rauthmann, 2012), the instrument has been critiqued in several recent reports (Jones and Paulhus 2014; Lee et al. 2013; Maples et al. 2014; Miller et al. 2012). The main limitation is the psychometric cost of using this short measure. Particularly, compared with standard long multi-item measures of the Dark Triad, the Dirty Dozen converges less strongly with other Dark Triad measures and shows weaker convergent validity with other external variables (e.g., the Big Five, aggression, and self-esteem level; Maples et al. 2014; Miller et al. 2012).

Short Dark Triad Measure

A brief proxy measure – the Short Dark Triad questionnaire (SD3; Jones and Paulhus 2014) – is a self-report questionnaire developed to assess three dimensions of the Dark Triad personality model. First, to develop the SD3 measure, after revision of the seminal literature, Jones and Paulhus (2014) created a large item pool that was subjected to further analyses. Next, the authors reduced the number of items as much as possible without missing the conceptual facets of each triad variable. The result was a 27-item instrument. Using the Principal Components Analysis, after studying several rotated structures ranging from three to six factors, three composites, consistent with the formulation of the Dark Triad model, were formed. Moreover, previously reported psychometric properties of the original SD3 revealed acceptable internal consistency for every dimension and convergent validity with the external variables consistent with hypotheses (Furnham et al. 2013; Jones and Paulhus 2014; Lee and Ashton 2005; Paulhus and Williams 2002). Jones and Paulhus (2014) concluded that “…the SD3 subscales provide useful proxies for the established Dark Triad measures they were meant to replace” (p. 37).

Overview of the Present Study

In the present report, we aimed to adapt the SD3 inventory in a sample of German adults. The second purpose of this study was to determine whether the original structure of the SD3 measure described in terms of the three-factor model by Jones and Paulhus (2014) could be replicated with a German version by Confirmatory Factor Analyses and to examine the psychometric properties of the adaptation of this instrument. Thus, our first task focused on the translation process of the original items from the measure. The process is precisely described. Next, analyses of the measure structure through Confirmatory Factor Analyses with Maximum Likelihood estimation method were performed. We examined the reliability of the SD3 proxy. We continued with examining if the measure possesses acceptable construct validity by comparing it with longer established measures of the Dark Triad. Finally, we examined patterns of convergent and discriminant relationships with other relevant variables of personality that have been proven important in prior research, specifically the Big Five, self-esteem and aggression levels. Developing the same measure for different countries has the advantage of establishing the same metric for all countries analysed (Furr & Bacharack, 2008). This characteristic would largely facilitate cross-cultural research with the Dark Triad personality model. With that said, if a particular measure retains sound psychometric characteristics when used in different cultures, then that measure has several things to recommend it (Hambleton 2001). First, it would provide a single measure that was potentially useful to researchers and practitioners in multiple cultures. Second, such a research should confirm its psychometric characteristics when used outside of the culture in which it was originally developed.

Study 1: Factor Structure of the Adapted Short Dark Triad Measure

The aim of the first study was to provide information about the factor structure of the adapted SD3 measure. For the factorial structure, a 3-factor structure, coherent with the model and the factor analysis of the original version, was found (Jones and Paulhus 2014; Paulhus and Williams 2002) (i.e., each dimension loads specifically on its respective dimensions, with inter-correlated dimensions).

Participants

The first sample was collected from the internet using an online version of the SD3 measure. The sample was composed of 598 adults (347 female and 251 male) of all ages ranging from 18 to 67 years (M = 27.34, SD = 7.50), and approximately 40% of them were aged between 19 and 27. The majority of the participants had a higher education: 327 were university graduates, and 195 had a high school education, whereas 76 had a lower level of education. 178 participants were students at the time of data collection. The non-student respondents were from various professions.

Procedure

Participation was voluntary and anonymous. Subjects were not compensated in any way. Data were collected in an online study administered via a tool for online surveys: SosciSurvey facility. Individuals were recruited through advertisements posted on social media (i.e., Facebook). All participants provided a consent which was obtained online after a detailed instruction describing main purposes and approximate duration of the study. All studies were approved by the appropriate ethics review committee prior to initiation. All individuals were offered feedback on general results of the study.

Measure

Subjects completed the translated version of the SD3 measure, which consisted of 27 items (sample item: It’s not wise to tell your secrets). It was the only instrument which participants were asked to complete. As in the original version, responses were given on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Before the individuals could send their answers over the net, the data were checked for completeness.

Translation

To ensure that the items resemble the meaning of the original English items as closely as possible, we followed a common procedure of back-translation in which a text is translated from a source into a target language, and a second interpreter independently translates the text back into the source language. Translation and back-translation of the original inventory and the new scale were provided by professional interpreters. The English version of the measure was first translated into German and then back-translated into English by two translators, according to the guidelines developed by the International Test Commission (Hambleton 2001). Afterwards, both translators compared the original version and the back-translated version for equivalence of meaning. The accuracy of the translation was evaluated by comparing the original and back-translated versions. Any discrepancy was discussed until an agreement was reached. This version was then refined by paying special attention to the use of frequently-used words, and using a correct and easy grammar in order to ensure the items were well understood for every level of education.

Before examining its psychometric properties, the German version of the SD3 was submitted to a pretest to ensure comprehensibility and clarity of items, as well as to make sure that items were worded in an understandable language for the German-speaking population (Hambleton 2001). Criteria were judged by a group of sixty-five adult individuals ranging in age from 20 to 48 years. The sample consisted of students (60%) and employees of various professions. The mean age was 27.2 years (SD = 3.05), and 73% were female. They were asked to rate each item on its clarity using a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (not clear at all) to 7 (perfectly clear) and also to circle every item or part of item that they did not understand. Using a 7-point rating scale and following Hambleton’s (2001) suggestions, items with a score 5 or below were reviewed. Based on these item-metric analyses, three items were corrected by paying special attention to the use of frequent and well-known words. In item ‘There are things you need to keep hidden from others in order to save face’, the translators evaluated a change in the expression ‘to save face’ for the expression ‘um dein Ansehen zu wahren’ (protect your reputation), thus reducing the possibility of error in interpretation. Next, in item ‘I know I am special because everyone keeps telling me’, the expression ‘ständig’ (constantly) was added, with the aim of emphasizing the occurrence. Finally, in item ‘You must get important people on your side, no matter the cost’, the judges evaluated a change in the adjective ‘important’ for the adjective ‘wichtigen’ (essential). Finally, this last version was given to the University Language Advisor for final correction. The German translation of the SD3 is displayed in Appendix.

Statistical Analyses

To examine the factor structure of the German SD3, we performed the Confirmatory Factor Analyses with the AMOS statistical package (Arbuckle 1997). The estimation method was Maximum Likelihood (ML). Note that, ML makes assumption about multivariate normality. As both skewness and kurtosis were less than or equal to 1.3 for all items, indicating that the item-distributions were similar to the rest of the items in the instrument and that the item distributions were rather symmetrical, the ML estimation was considered appropriate (see Mulaik 2007). We used several criteria of model fit (see Bollen and Long 1993). A well-fitting model should ideally have a nonsignificant χ2 statistic (p > .05). The model is considered to have a good fit if the GFI (goodness-of-fit index), TLI (Tucker–Lewis index), and CFI (comparative fit index) values are approximately .95. As the χ2 statistic tends to be inflated in small samples, the ratio χ2/df was determined, which should not be much larger than 2.0. The χ2/df is a measure of the absolute fit of the model with the data, indicating how closely the model fits compared to a perfect fit. The RMSEA (root mean square error of approximation) represents reasonable errors of approximation in the population; a value of approximately .05 or less would indicate a close fit and a value of up to .08 would represent a reasonable fit of the model. We note, however, that the choice of indices and cutoff values is a topic surrounded by considerable controversy (see, e.g., Mulaik 2007). In these analyses, two models were examined: (1) one-factor solution (all 27 items were combined into one factor); and (2) three-factor solution, which comprised three factors representing three Dark Triad traits. The choice of models was influenced by Jones and Paulhus (2014). Next, to evaluate the internal consistency, Cronbach’s α of scales were calculated. SD3 scale inter-correlations were checked with two-tailed Pearson correlations.

Results and Discussion

The three-factor model had a good fit to the data from German sample: χ2 (102 df) = 206.24, p > .05; χ2/df = 2.02; RMSEA = .04, GFI = .97, CFI = .98, TLI = .99. Second, for the purpose of comparison, a one-factor model, which presupposes that all the items pertain to the same factor, was also assessed. According to the χ2 statistic and the p-value, the model would have to be rejected (χ2 [114 df] = 485.20, p < .01; RMSEA = .08, GFI = .83, CFI = .89, TLI = .88). The value χ2/df = 4.26 obtained here also indicates an unacceptable fit. The three-factor model had a much better fit than a one-factor model of general Dark Triad, Δχ2(12) = 278.96, p < .01.

Based on the standardized regression weights, each item was linked to a single factor. All of these coefficients exceed .45, providing additional support for the efficacy of this model (see Table 1). Also, the corrected item total correlation was positive on all items, ranging from .47 to .91. This indicates that all contribute to gauge what the test measures and further in the same direction. All items were encapsulated in their respective factors as in the original distribution of the instrument (see Jones and Paulhus 2014).

Table 1 Standardized regression weights of the adapted Short Dark Triad items

Moreover, the following moderate correlations between the three SD3 composite scales were found for the examined sample: rMach.Narc = .34, rMach.Psych = .48 and rNarc.Psych = .41 (all p < .01). These results are comparable with the correlations reported by Jones and Paulhus (2014). As shown in the descriptions of the three dark traits (Paulhus and Williams 2002), there are several notable common features among these variables. All of them include the core of manipulation and exploitation (Lee et al. 2013). Thus, it can describe the significant modest correlations between the Dark Triad factors.

The internal consistencies were in a medium but acceptable range (narcissism α = .73, psychopathy α = .72, Machiavellianism α = .78). Each dark factor is a basic personality trait that covers a wide range of behavioral and emotional reactions. Multi-item scales can afford to bolster internal consistency by using several items with high content overlap (Gosling et al. 2003). In contrast, with only nine items per scale, the SD3 instead emphasized content validity considerations, resulting in lower Cronbach’s alpha. Thus, researchers wishing to correct the SD3 correlations for unreliability should base their corrections on reliability estimates that are less biased by the small number of items on each scale. Recent work (Crede et al. 2012) suggests that test-retest reliability may be more predictive than estimates of internal consistency.

Study 2: Test-Retest Reliability

Consistency over time is a defining feature of personality traits (Furr & Bacharack, 2008), and therefore, such evidence will bolster the claims about the usefulness of our measure as well as provide support for the treatment of the Dark Triad as stable personality variables in an adapted instrument. Here, the test–retest reliability of the SD3 measure was assessed during a 4-week period.

Participants

To evaluate SD3 test-retest stability, for 221 subjects of the 598 participants who completed the SD3 measure in Study 1, we obtained a second evaluation of three dark traits; individuals completed the SD3 again 4 weeks after the first administration. Volunteers (N = 221; 66% female; mean age 22.3 years; SD = 2.3) from unique IP addresses recruited via the Internet completed an online survey that informed them of the nature of the study and asked them to respond to the items described above. Again, subjects were not compensated in any way. All respondents taking part in the research provided informed consent after the nature of the study had been explained to them. All individuals were offered feedback on general results of the study after the second measurement. They did not get any feedback after Study 1 until Study 2 was completed.

Measures

Study 2 used the same 27-item SD3 measure used previously in Study 1.

Results and Discussion

Test–retest reliability of the SD3 measure and its three components was determined by correlating Session 1 with Session 2 scores (as well as their 95% confidence intervals). The test-retest values for the various scales were as follows: Machiavellianism rtt = .81 (95% CI = .80–.82; 179 individuals out of 221 demonstrated stable preferences), psychopathy rtt = .83 (95% CI = .81–.85; 183 individuals out of 221 demonstrated stable preferences), and narcissism rtt = .74 (95% CI = .73–.75; 164 individuals out of 221 demonstrated stable preferences). These correlations were uniformly high and indicate good test-retest stability of the SD3 measure.

Study 3: Construct Validation against Standard Measures

A crucial aspect of validating an adapted assessment method is a study that tests its performance against the standard assessment tools (Crede et al. 2012). Specifically, to what degree and in what way do the SD3 subscales correlate with the long-form measures of the Dark Triad. Accordingly, Study 3 included the standard Dark Triad measures—Mach IV, SRP-III, and NPI—in addition to the SD3 tool. For comparison, an alternative short proxy – the 12-item Dirty Dozen (DD) measure – was included. To assess construct validity, some basic predictions were made. The SD3 measure should behave similarly to the longer measures. Thus, the SD3 should be correlated with the longer, standard measures of the Dark Triad. Moreover, because multi-item inventories tend to be more reliable than single-item inventories (Gosling et al. 2003), correlations between the SD3 measure and long instruments of the Dark Triad should be stronger than between DD and the same three longer measures.

Participants

A total of 384 German students participated in the study (154 men and 230 women, ranging in age from 20 to 26 years, M = 23, SD = 1.8). Individuals were recruited through advertisements posted around the university campus. All subjects provided informed consent or assent after the nature of the study had been explained to them. Participants were not compensated in any way. All individuals were offered feedback on general results of the study.

Measures

The adapted Short Dark Triad 27-item scale was used. An alternative short assessment method of the Dark Triad, the Dirty Dozen measure (Küfner et al. 2014) comprising 12 items, was used (sample item: I tend to lack remorse). Responses are scored on a 9-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 9 (strongly agree). Additionally, the standard long Dark Triad measures were provided. Machiavellianism was assessed with an 18-item scale (sample item: The best way to handle people is to tell them what they want to hear; Henning and Six 2008). The answer format was a six-point Likert-type scale from 0 (not at all like me) to 5 (totally like me). Psychopathy was measured with a 30-item version of the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale-III (sample item: I can be fairly cunning if I have to be; Williams et al. 2003). Answers were given on a five-point Likert–type scale from 0 (not at all like me) to 4 (totally like me). Narcissism was assessed with the 17-item Narcissistic Personality Inventory (sample item: I am essentially a modest person; von Collani 2008) with a five-point Likert-type scale from 0 (not at all like me) to 4 (totally like me). The standard Dark Triad measures were divided into their respective facets to determine if the SD3 subscales were capturing each measure in a balanced way (see Jones and Paulhus 2014).

Procedure

All measures were paper-and-pencil questionnaires and were presented in a random order.

Results and Discussion

The relationships between the SD3 scales in relation to established long Dark Triad measures and an alternative DD tool were examined (see Table 2). In sum, all the SD3 scales showed a clear correspondence with their criterion counterparts measured by the three longer tools. Moreover, compared with the Dirty Dozen versions, all three Short Dark Triad subscales showed stronger correlations with the facets of its corresponding standard, ranging from .28 (Antisocial Behavior facet of Psychopathy Scale with the SD3 Narcissism) to .71. (Manipulation facet of Psychopathy Scale with the SD3 Psychopathy).

Table 2 Convergent correlations between traditional three measures of the Dark Triad and the Short Dark Triad and Dirty Dozen measures (top panel), and correlations among the Short Dark Triad and Big-Five, aggression, and self-esteem measures (lower panel)

These results add support for previous research suggesting that scores from the SD3 scales manifested acceptable convergent validity with longer, more established measures (Jones and Paulhus 2014; Maples et al. 2014; Miller et al. 2012). The SD3 adequately covers the elements of each Dark Triad construct. Additionally, direct comparisons with the DD instrument indicated stronger construct validity with the well-established longer tools of the SD3 than of the DD measure. These results confirm previous findings showing that the SD3 scales provide a more complete account of the underlying Dark Triad constructs (Maples et al. 2014). Notably, the DD psychopathy and narcissism scales seemed to have only scattered coverage of the longer Dark Triad measures. The stronger convergent validity with the established Dark Triad measures of the SD3 scores adds support for the claim that only 12 items of the DD measure are likely to contain only a particularly narrow aspect of the Dark Triad constructs (Maples et al. 2014).

Study 4: Convergent and Discriminant Validity

The aim of Study 4 was to provide additional evidence of the SD3 measure’s psychometric properties and its validity as a concise measure of constructs underlying the Dark Triad. It is common to use the external personality traits to describe the validity of measures (Gosling et al. 2003). The validity of the SD3 measure was assessed by correlating it with the Big Five measure, aggression questionnaire, and a global measure of self-esteem–constructs that have proven important in prior studies with the original, 91-item version of the Dark Triad assessment technique (for a review, see Furnham et al. 2013). The original SD3 measure already has been successfully used in many research contexts (for a review, see Furnham et al. 2013), providing insight into darker personality factors and suggesting that the Dark Triad model may be theoretically informative compared to other models. The conceptual formulation of the Dark Triad traits is that they share disagreeableness (Furnham et al. 2013; Jonason et al. 2009; Lee and Ashton 2005; Paulhus and Williams 2002), conscientiousness (Furnham et al. 2013; Lee and Ashton 2005), and aggression (e.g., Jones and Paulhus 2010; Bushman and Baumeister 1998), which may relate to a fast life strategy that underlies the nature of the Dark Triad (e.g., Jonason et al. 2010). The SD3 measure should be correlated negatively with agreeableness and conscientiousness from the five-factor model of the Big Five and positively with aggression. In contrast, in showing evidence of discriminant validity, it was predicted that the SD3 should be mostly uncorrelated with the measure of self-esteem level (Bushman and Baumeister 1998). Last, there is ample evidence that suggests that men score higher on all three of these traits than women (e.g., Furnham et al. 2013; Jonason et al. 2009), and therefore, we expect that men should score higher than women on the SD3 measures. These gender predictions constitute validity tests of the SD3 measure (Furr and Bacharach 2008).

Participants

The sample consisted of 481 participants (270 women and 211 men).

Participants were students from all levels (72% of the sample) and employees of various occupations (ages between 18 and 61 years; M = 28; SD = 3.1). Subjects were recruited through advertisements posted on social media (i.e., Facebook). All respondents taking part in the research provided informed consent after the nature of the study had been explained to them. They were not compensated in any way. All individuals were offered feedback on general results of the study.

Materials

Study 4 used the same 27-item Short Dark Triad instrument used previously in three studies. Participants also completed a battery of other German-speaking measures. Participants responded to a 50-item Big Five personality dimension measure based on the International Personality Item Pool (sample item: Don’t like to draw attention to myself; Goldberg et al. 2006). The instrument contains five factors (Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, and Intellect/Imagination (mean α = .84)). Aggression was measured using a 29-item version of the Aggression Questionnaire (sample item: I let my anger show when I do not get what I want; von Collani and Werner 2005) with 29 items with the four components of Physical Aggression, Verbal Aggression, Anger, and Hostility (mean α = .79). Finally, the global self-esteem level was assessed with a 10-item measure of self-esteem (sample item: At times I think I am no good at all; von Collani and Herzberg 2003; α = .69). All measures used a 5-point Likert response scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

Procedure

The study was conducted using the paper-and-pencil method. All questionnaires were presented in a random order.

Results and Discussion

We attempted to replicate the factor structure of the SD3 through the use of CFA as in Study 1. The fit indices were as follows: χ2 (64 df) = 138.17, p > .05; χ2/df = 2.16; RMSEA = .07; GFI = .98, CFI = .97, TLI = .96. The outcome of the CFA suggested that a three-factor solution yielded the best model fit. As with the data from Study 1, CFI, GFI, TLI, RMSEA, and χ2/df, respectively, indicated a good or an acceptable model fit.

Correlations were used to explore convergent and discriminant validity between the SD3 measures and the Big Five, aggression, and self-esteem assessment tools. As expected, the SD3 displayed patterns of correlations that closely matched those obtained with the standard long Dark Triad instruments in previous research (Jones and Paulhus 2014; Maples et al. 2014; data from the present study are summarized in the lower part of Table 2). This correspondence adds further construct validity to the SD3 measure based on its location in personality space (Gosling et al. 2003).

Big five

The SD3 measure and each scale were negatively associated with agreeableness. Individuals who are disagreeable in nature manifest characteristics that are antisocial, which is how these personality traits relate to the Dark Triad traits (Paulhus and Williams 2002). Additionally, the SD3 showed a significant association with conscientiousness. However, a difference emerged with the facets of SD3; the positive correlation with narcissism was due to achievement-striving and competence, whereas the negative correlations with Machiavellianism and psychopathy were caused by low dutifulness and low deliberation (Lee and Ashton 2005; Miller et al. 2012). Except for agreeableness and conscientiousness, none of the other three factors operationalized within the Big Five model correlated substantially with the three Dark Triad variables. However, in previous research narcissism significantly correlated with Intellect/Imagination and Extraversion (Lee and Ashton 2005), and in the present study it did not. As noted in previous research, Narcissism includes some traits (i.e., leadership, assertiveness, dominance, and a tendency to seek social attention) that are not the core elements within psychopathy and Machiavellianism, all of which are relevant to extraversion and intellect/imagination (Lee and Ashton 2005). However, this finding could be an issue of culture or idiosyncratic to the sample involved in the study as well.

Aggression

The SD3 showed a consistent pattern of convergent validity with measures of aggression, suggesting that people who obtain high scores on the Dark Triad may also use aggression to obtain what they want (Jonason and Webster 2010). Among the SD3 scales, Machiavellianism was positively related to physical aggression, verbal aggression, and hostility. Psychopathy, in contrast, was positively related to anger, physical and verbal aggression. Narcissism, however, was positively related only to hostility. Those high in narcissism are easy to provoke into aggression but only if the provocation consists of a significant ego-threat (Bushman and Baumeister 1998; Furnaham et al., 2013; Jones and Paulhus 2010). Thus, it can only describe the small relationship between narcissism and hostility.

Self-Esteem

Study 4 also showed the significant lack of relationship between self-esteem stability and the Dark Triad traits (for contrary results for narcissism, see Bushman and Baumeister 1998).

Sex Differences

Gender differences were examined using a t-test. After including a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (the Bonferroni correction sets the significance cut-off at adjusted alpha level/number of t-tests performed. Thus, in the present data, with 3 tests and α = .05, we would only reject a null hypothesis if the p-value is less than .016), tests for sex differences revealed men scored slightly higher than women on the scales of narcissism, t(479) = 3.90, p = .015; and psychopathy, t(479) = 9.05, p < .01; as well as the entire SD3 measure, t(479) = 14.17, p < .01. The present study replicated prior findings indicating that high scores on these variables are more prevalent among men (Furnham et al. 2013). The gender differences in the Dark Triad traits can be described by the evolutionary perspective, in which men are thought to benefit more from social exploitation (Jonason et al. 2009). Thus, consequently, men gain higher scores on personality traits related to social exploitation.

General Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to provide a well-adapted version of the Short Dark Triad measure based on the Dark Triad personality model proposed by Paulhus and Williams (2002). The additional aim was to determine whether the original structure of the SD3 measure described in terms of the three-factor model by Jones and Paulhus (2014) could be replicated with a German version. The measure displayed a robust structure and acceptable psychometric properties in the German language. Specifically, the aims of the present study were: to analyse the factor structure, internal consistency, and temporal stability of the SD3; to explore the association of the standard measure methods of the Dark Triad with the adapted SD3 inventory; and to investigate the convergent and discriminant validity of the SD3 measure.

CFA was used to evaluate if the previously reported factor structure of the SD3 was valid for our data. We used both model fit measures and χ2-difference scores in these model comparisons. Consistent with Jones and Paulhus (2014), the three-factor solution gave the best model fit. These factors were closely associated with the theoretical three dimensions (Paulhus and Williams 2002). This solution was also supported by the χ2-difference test comparing three-factor model to one-factor model. Note that the RMSEA, GFI, and χ2/df all indicated a good or acceptable model fit. Internal consistency coefficients were acceptable and comparable with those reported by Jones and Paulhus (2014). The lower internal consistencies results could be due to the threefold fewer number of items compared to longer well-established Dark Triad instruments (27 versus 91 items). Moreover, in the present study, test-retest reliability of the adapted SD3 measure remains adequate in intervals of four weeks, indicating dark personality variables to be relatively stable over time traits. Additionally, results report an acceptable convergence among the SD3 and the other well-established Dark Triad instruments, confirming the outcomes found by Jones and Paulhus (2014).

Concurrent validity of the SD3 was assessed by relating its subscales to measures of the Big Five concept, aggression, and self-esteem. The analyses of convergent validity support both the validity of the German version of the SD3 as well as Paulhus’ and Williams’ (2002) concepts of basic personality traits. Consistent with the theoretical predictions, the Dark Triad traits are mainly related to a lack of agreeableness and to conscientiousness and to a stronger degree of various types of aggression, lending support to the validity of the adapted instrument. However, our research had some limitations.

Limitations

It is evident that our samples are also severely biased. There were very few participants from groups with lower education. Additionally, the retest reliability is high, but the period of time between the first and second assessments was short (four weeks). Finally, the validation of the adapted SD3 measure was done exclusively against other self-report instruments. In establishing the validity of a psychological construct, it clearly is important to consider non-self-report data as well (Furr and Bacharach 2008). Specifically, with self-report tasks, asking for reporting participants’ behavioral tendencies in various contexts, may not always accurately reflect individuals’ behavior. Self-reported measures are more sensitive to bias and are influenced by social desirability responses.

Conclusions

Despite the frequently cited evidence that the short measures of personality are likely to have several psychometric weaknesses than scores on well-constructed longer inventories (Crede et al. 2012), the results presented in this article suggest that these beliefs are generally incorrect. Overall, the present findings suggest that the German version of the SD3 instrument can stand as a reasonable proxy for a longer Dark Triad instrument, especially if research conditions dictate a short measure to be used.