Skip to main content
Log in

The Natural Tendency for Wide and Careful Listening: Exploring the Relationship Between Open-Mindedness and Psychological Science

  • Research
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We take open-mindedness to be a component of intellectual humility, as much of the recent empirical literature regarding intellectual humility does but contrary to what some philosophers think. More particularly, we understand intellectual humility as having a self-directed component, which is concerned primarily with the regulation of confidence we have on our own epistemic goods and capacities, and an other-directed component, which is concerned primarily with one’s epistemic openness to others so to improve one’s epistemic situation. Given that the open-minded person is disposed give new ideas serious consideration, it is crucial that she both listens widely and carefully to other’s ideas. In this paper, we examine whether there is evidence to suggest that we have a natural, evolved tendency for this wide and careful listening related to open-mindedness. We conclude that there is indication of a natural tendency for wide listening, especially an in-group tendency. However, careful listening lacks more substantive empirical studies. It seems that human infants are much more inclined to be charitable and attentive to in-group cues or opinions. This is important evidence to deconstruct the idea of a natural tendency of virtuous intellectual humility that opens up the discussion for the role of social learning in cultivating and maintaining a virtuous life.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Moreover, both the self- and other-directed components are present in the implicit theories of intellectual humility that the folk have and, particularly, they view intellectually humble people as open-minded (Samuelson et al., 2015). Although many researchers agree that humility involves both components, there is more agreement with the regard to the exact nature of the self-directed one (i.e., involving an accurate view of the self) than the other-directed one (Davies & Hook, 2014; Reis et al., 2018).

  2. On such inter-dependability, see, e.g., De Brasi (2018).

  3. Notice that Priest (2017, p.476-7) understands intellectual humility as being interpersonal but rejects the self-assessment component as an essential facet of intellectual humility and thinks of it as a byproduct of it. So, for Priest, the self-directed component is not constitutive of intellectual humility. Although we disagree, this is not an issue we will engage with here.

  4. Dalmiya (2016) speaks of the intellectual virtue of “relational humility” to emphasize the other-directed component of humility.

  5. Of course, there are other forms of communicative receptivity aside from this aural one, but for simplicity-sake here the focus is on listening.

  6. As Dewey says: “While it is hospitality to new themes, facts, ideas, questions, it is not the kind of hospitality that would be indicated by hanging out a sign: “Come right in; there is nobody at home”.” (1986, p.136).

  7. Indeed, Gilbert Harman (1999 and 2000), based on social-psychological evidence, argues against the existence of character traits. John M. Doris (2002) more moderately argues that “narrow” but not global character traits can be empirically supported. Such traits have limited explanatory power, since agents do not manifest them in most or all circumstances.

  8. A virtue is understood as consisting of attitudes and dispositions of the agent which “perfect” a natural human faculty or correct for proneness to dysfunction and error in certain situations (Roberts & Wood, 2007, p.59). An intellectual virtue consists, roughly, of attitudes and dispositions for good and productive thinking.

  9. Notice that we are here assuming an effect conception of virtue (i.e., promoting good epistemic effects) as opposed to a motive one (i.e., being motivated by good epistemic desires); see, e.g., Battaly (2015). Having said that, the positive effects, as we shall see, need not be for oneself but others. So, to use Kawall’s (2002) distinction between self-regarding and other-regarding virtues, where the former tend to directly benefit one while the latter tend to directly benefit others, open-mindedness is both self- and other-regarding. We could say that open-mindedness (and so too the more complex construct of intellectual humility) is a social intellectual virtue that has positive epistemic effects for the intellectual community.

  10. For example, one party may advance a reason in favour of some claim and another one may respond by introducing a counter-reason or defeater that speaks against the claim; then the first party may introduce a defeater of the defeater or concede the original reason offered has been defeated—or weakened to certain extent—and perhaps introduce some new reason, and so on. Eventually they weight the reasons for and against to see how strong the case for the claim is.

  11. For empirical research showing the connection between intellectual humility, open-mindedness and knowledge acquisition; see Krumrei-Mancuso et al. (2020).

References

  • Alfano, M., Iurino, K., Stey, P., Robinson, B., Christen, M., Yu, F., & Lapsley, D. (2017). Development and validation of a multi-dimensional measure of intellectual humility.PloS one, 12(8), e0182950.

  • Baehr, J. (2011). The structure of open-mindedness. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 41(2), 191–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ballantyne, N. (2021). Recent work on intellectual humility: A philosopher’s perspective. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2021.1899281.

  • Battaly, H. (2015). Epistemic virtue and vice: Reliabilism, responsibilism, and personalism. Moral and intellectual virtues in western and chinese philosophy (pp. 99–120). Routledge.

  • Battaly, H. (2019). A third kind of intellectual virtue: Personalism. In H. Battaly (Ed.), Routledge handbook of virtue epistemology (pp. 115–126). London: Routledge. Behaviors. Journal of Research in Personality 40, 21–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, L., & Simpson, J. A. (2009). The power of the Situation: The impact of Milgram’s obedience studies on personality and social psychology. American Psychologist, 64(1), 12–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition, 112(2), 337–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Morris, K. J. (1983). Effects of need for cognition on message evaluation, recall, and persuasion. Journal of personality and social psychology, 45(4), 805.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (2009). Mindreading underlies metacognition. Behavioral and brain sciences, 32(2), 164–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, J. A., & Gordon, E. C. (2014). A new maneuver against the epistemic relativist. Synthese, 191, 1683–1695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coakley, C. G., Halone, K. K., & Wolvin, A. D. (1996). Perceptions of listening ability across the life-span: Implications for understanding listening competence. International Journal of Listening, 10(1), 21–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2008). The revised neo personality inventory (neo-pi-r). Sage Publications, Inc.

  • De Brasi, L. (2018). Citizenry incompetence and the Epistemic structure of Society. Unisinos Journal of Philosophy, 19(3), 201–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Brasi, L. (2020a). Argumentative deliberation and the Development of Intellectual Humility and Autonomy in the Classroom. Cogency, 12(1), 13–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Brasi, L. (2020b). Judicial decisions, intellectual virtues and the division of labour. The International Journal of Evidence & Proof, 24(2), 142–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J., & Rorty, R. (2008). The Later Works of John Dewey, Volume 8,1925–1953: 1933, Essays and How We Think.

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual review of psychology, 64, 135–168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Doris, J. M. (2002). Lack of Character: Personality and Moral Behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Harvard University Press.

  • Edwards, K., & Smith, E. E. (1996). A disconfirmation bias in the evaluation of arguments. Journal of personality and social psychology, 71(1), 5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fearon, J. D. (1998). Bargaining, enforcement, and international cooperation. International organization, 52(2), 269–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleeson, W., and Erik Noftle (2008). The end of the Person-Situation Debate: An emerging synthesis in the answer to the consistency question. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1667–1684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleeson, W. (2004). Moving personality beyond the Person-Situation Debate: The challenge and opportunity of within-person variability. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 83–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleischhauer, M., Enge, S., Brocke, B., Ullrich, J., Strobel, A., & Strobel, A. (2010). Same or different? Clarifying the relationship of need for cognition to personality and intelligence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(1), 82–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Funder, D. C. (2006). Towards a Resolution of the Personality Triad: Persons, Situations, and

  • Funder, D. C. (2009). Persons, behaviors, and situations: An agenda for personality psychology in the postwar era. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 120–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D., & Lin, J. (1998a). Numerical cognition: Age-related differences in the speed of executing biologically primary and biologically secondary processes. Experimental Aging Research, 24(2), 101–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D., & Lin, J. (1998b). Numerical cognition: Age-related differences in the speed of executing biologically primary and biologically secondary processes. Experimental Aging Research, 24(2), 101–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gehlbach, H. (2010). The social side of school: Why teachers need social psychology. Educational psychology review, 22, 349–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gehlbach, H., & Brinkworth, M. E. (2012). The social perspective taking process: Strategies and sources of evidence in taking another’s perspective. Teachers College Record, 114(1), 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gehlbach, H., Marietta, G., King, A. M., Karutz, C., Bailenson, J. N., & Dede, C. (2015). Many ways to walk a mile in another’s moccasins: Type of social perspective taking and its effect on negotiation outcomes. Computers in Human Behavior, 52, 523–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halone, K. K., Wolvin, A. D., & Coakley, C. G. (1997). Accounts of effective listening across the life-span: Expectations and experiences associated with competent listening practices. International Journal of Listening, 11(1), 15–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harman, G. (1999). Moral Philosophy meets social psychology: Virtue Ethics and the fundamental attribution error. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 99, 315–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harman, G. (2000). The nonexistence of character traits. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 100(1), 223–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Muthukrishna, M. (2021). The origins and psychology of human cooperation. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 207–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jenks, A. C. (2011). From “lists of traits” to “open-mindedness”: Emerging issues in cultural competence education. Culture Medicine and Psychiatry, 35, 209–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., & Arie, W. K. (2002). The estrangement of Social Constructionism and experimental social psychology: History of the rift and prospects for Reconciliation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 168–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpowitz, C., & Mendelberg, T. (2018). The political psychology of deliberation. In A. Bächtiger, J. Dryzek, J. Mansbridge, & M. Warren (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of deliberative democracy (pp. 535–555). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kashima, E. S., Greiner, T., Sadewo, G., Ampuni, S., Helou, L., Nguyen, V. A., & Kaspar, K. (2017). Open-and closed-mindedness in cross-cultural adaptation: The roles of mindfulness and need for cognitive closure. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 59, 31–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawall, J. (2002). Other–regarding epistemic virtues. Ratio, 15(3), 257–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, I. J. (2015). Educating for intellectual humility. Jason Baehr (Ed) Intellectual virtues and education (pp. 54–70). New York: Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., & Spelke, E. S. (2012a). Language-based social preferences among children in South Africa. Language learning and development, 8(3), 215–232.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., & Spelke, E. S. (2012b). Language-based social preferences among children in South Africa. Language learning and development, 8(3), 215–232.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., DeJesus, J., & Spelke, E. S. (2009). Accent trumps race in guiding children’s social preferences. Social cognition, 27(4), 623–634.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., & Fishman, S. (2009). The need for cognitive closure.Handbook of individual differences in social behavior,343–353.

  • Krumrei-Mancuso, E. J., & Rouse, S. V. (2016). The development and validation of the comprehensive intellectual humility scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 98(2), 209–221. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2015.1068174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krumrei-Mancuso, E. J., Haggard, M. C., LaBouff, J. P., & Rowatt, W. C. (2020). Links between intellectual humility and acquiring knowledge. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 15(2), 155–170. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2019.1579359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kwong, J. (2016). Open-mindedness as a critical virtue. Topoi, 35(2), 403–411. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-015-9317-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. (2018). The psychology of intellectual humility.John Templeton Foundation, 3.

  • Mampe, B., Friederici, A. D., Christophe, A., & Wermke, K. (2009a). Newborns’ cry melody is shaped by their native language. Current biology, 19(23), 1994–1997.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mampe, B., Friederici, A., Christophe, A., & Wermke, K. (2009b). Newborn’s cry melody is shaped by their native language. Current Biology, 19, 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., & Santos, L. R. (2016). What cognitive representations support primate theory of mind? Trends in cognitive sciences, 20(5), 375–382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzarella, D., & Pouscoulous, N. (2021a). Pragmatics and epistemic vigilance: A developmental perspective. Mind & Language, 36(3), 355–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzarella, D., & Pouscoulous, N. (2021b). Pragmatics and epistemic vigilance: A developmental perspective. Mind & Language, 36(3), 355–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGrath, R. E., & Brown, M. (2020). Using the VIA classification to advance a psychological science of virtue. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 3442. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.565953.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H. (2011). On the universality of argumentative reasoning. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 11(1–2), 85–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H. (2017a). How gullible are we? A review of the evidence from psychology and social science. Review of General Psychology, 21(2), 103–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H. (2017b). Reasoning and argumentation. The Routledge International Handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 401–414). Routledge.

  • Mercier, H. (2020). Not born yesterday. Not born yesterday. Princeton University Press.

  • Mercier, H., & Sperber, D. (2017). The enigma of reason. Harvard University Press.

  • Michelon, P., & Zacks, J. M. (2006a). Two kinds of visual perspective taking. Perception & psychophysics, 68(2), 327–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michelon, P., & Zacks, J. M. (2006b). Two kinds of visual perspective taking. Perception & psychophysics, 68, 327–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2006a). Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 603–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2006b). Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 603–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Onraet, E., Van Hiel, A., Roets, A., & Cornelis, I. (2011). The closed mind:‘Experience’and ‘cognition’aspects of openness to experience and need for closure as psychological bases for right–wing attitudes. European Journal of Personality, 25(3), 184–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, T., & Schumann, K. (2018). Intellectual humility and openness to the opposing view. Self and Identity, 17(2), 139–162. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2017.1361861.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Priest, M. (2017). Intellectual humility: An interpersonal theory.Ergo, an Open Access Journal of Philosophy, 4.

  • Pronin, E., Gilovich, T., & Ross, L. (2004). Objectivity in the eye of the beholder: Divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others. Psychological review, 111(3), 781.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reis, H., Lee, K., O’Keefe, S., & Clark, M. (2018). Perceived partner responsiveness promotes intellectual humility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 79, 21–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.05.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riggs, W. (2010). Open-mindedness. Metaphilosophy, 41(1‐2), 172–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, R. (2007). In J. Wood (Ed.), Intellectual virtues. Oxford: OUP. 340p.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Samuelson, P., Jarvinen, M., Thomas, B. P., Church, I. M., Hardy, S. A., & Barrett, J. L. (2015). Implicit theories of intellectual virtues and vices: A focus on intellectual humility. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 10(5), 389–406. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.967802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R., & Cheney, D. (1990). The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and another species’ alarm calls. Animal Behaviour, 40(4), 754–764.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soley, G., & Köseler, B. (2021a). The social meaning of common knowledge across development. Cognition, 215, 104811.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soley, G., & Köseler, B. (2021b). The social meaning of common knowledge across development. Cognition, 215, 104811.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., Clément, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., & Wilson, D. (2010a). Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25(4), 359–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., Clément, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., & Wilson, D. (2010b). Epistemic vigilance. Mind & language, 25(4), 359–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiegel, J. S. (2012a). Open-mindedness and intellectual humility. Theory and Research in Education, 10(1), 27–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiegel, J. S. (2012b). Open-mindedness and intellectual humility. Theory and Research in Education, 10(1), 27–38. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477878512437472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & Toplak, M. E. (2019). The need for intellectual diversity in psychological science: Our own studies of actively open-minded thinking as a case study. Cognition, 187, 156–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.03.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of educational psychology, 89(2), 342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P. (2000). Humility: Theoretical perspectives, empirical findings and directions for future research. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19(1), 70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. MIT press.

  • Tomasello, M. (2010). Origins of human communication. MIT press.

  • Wells, D. A. (2021). Plasticity-led evolution and human culture. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 55(4), 821–849. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-021-09607-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitcomb, D., Battaly, H., Baehr, J., & Howard-Snyder, D. (2017a). Intellectual Humility. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 94(3), 509–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitcomb, D., Battaly, H., Baehr, J., & Howard-Snyder, D. (2017b). Intellectual humility: Owning our limitations. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 94(3), https://doi.org/10.1111/phpr.12228.

  • Wright, J. C., Nadelhoffer, T., Ross, L. T., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2018). Be it ever so humble: Proposing a dual-dimension account and measurement of humility. Self and Identity, 17(1), 92–125. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2017.1327454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors state that both have contributed equally to all steps of the writing process. This research has been partly funded by the Fondecyt project #1210724 (ANID, Chile). We, the authors of this paper, declare that we have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The authors state that both have contributed equally to all steps of the writing process. G.F. and L.D.B. conceptualized, wrote and revised the manuscript text.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Leandro De Brasi.

Ethics declarations

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fortes, G., De Brasi, L. The Natural Tendency for Wide and Careful Listening: Exploring the Relationship Between Open-Mindedness and Psychological Science. Integr. psych. behav. 57, 1312–1330 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-023-09774-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-023-09774-z

Keywords

Navigation