Skip to main content
Log in

What Are Group Level Traits and How Do They Evolve?

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cultural traits are seldom atomic, are distributed over multiple social domains, and undergo differential selection. This makes it important to study the nature and evolution of these traits from a global viewpoint. This paper considers group level cultural traits—what sort of traits are there, how do they evolve, and what is the relationship between cultural traits and their representation in individual worldviews. While not providing a concise theory, important aspects of cultural traits are elaborated and directions of further research indicated. Group level traits arising from individual biological traits are distinguished from those that are intrinsic to a group. The latter are formative of individual worldviews and are emotionally salient for group members. Children are saturated with culture from birth, it provides the scaffolding for their developing worldviews. Affective links between cultural ideas, social behavior, and material elements of culture develop so that the affordances in perceived situations carry biases influencing behavior toward culturally acceptable responses. Intrinsic traits are not, however, acted on directly by group level selection; rather, this selection acts on the behavior of group members and only indirectly on intrinsic cultural ideas through social exchange processes between group members.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data and materials

Not Applicable.

Code Availability

Not Applicable.

Notes

  1. The analogy to a virus is only partial. The genetic information in a virus instructs a host cell to replicate copies of the virus, including its genetic code. Memes do not contain the information that instructs a host to replicate the meme, transmission depends on whether the host decides to repeat the meme.

  2. Also called self-other reorganization (Gabora 2021).

  3. This does not imply that cultural solutions will always be adaptive.

  4. The problems arising from this definition of group fitness have been discussed in Sect. 1.

  5. For example, recent rule changes by the National Football League in the United States in order to reduce the chance of concussion.

  6. The United Society of Believers in Christs Second Appearing, known as the Shakers, practiced celibacy. As of 2017 there were only two Shakers remaining in the world (Smithsonian Magazine January 6, 2017).

  7. As, for example, in the 1964 statement by US Supreme Court Justice Potter Stewart: “I shall not today attempt further to define the kinds of material I understand to be embraced within that shorthand description “hard-core pornography,” and perhaps I could never succeed in intelligibly doing so. But I know it when I see it…”.

  8. This is the point of the Socratic claim that virtue cannot be taught. When Socrates raised questions on the nature of justice his goal was not to define justice, it was to become just. He was attempting to provoke internalization of the indefinable “thing in itself,” the fifth level of knowledge alluded to in Plato’s Seventh Letter.

  9. A cultural ideal of cooperation has little effect if group member are not willing to support it with behavior.

  10. In a superficial example, what led to the popularity of some dance fads, such as the Charleston or the Twist, while others (the Funky Robot, the Urkel) failed to get off the ground? More seriously, why do some conspiracy theories propagate while others do not?

  11. Gabora uses the term autocatalytic food sets.

References

  • Abbott, J. T., Griffiths, T. L., & Regier, T. (2016). Focal colors across languages are representative members of color categories. PNAS, 113(40), 11178–11183

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Acerbi, A., Jacquet, Kendal, J. R., & Tehrani, J. (2017). Cultural complexity and demography: the case of folktales. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(4), 474–480

    Google Scholar 

  • Aguilar, E. (2016). Models and Methods in Cultural and Social Evolution. Ph.D. Thesis

  • Aranguren, M. (2016). Reconstructing the social constructivist view of emotions: from language to culture, including nonhuman culture. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 47(2), 164–214

    Google Scholar 

  • Asma, S. T., & Gabriel, R. (2020). The Emotional Mind: The Affective Roots of Culture and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Atran, S. (1998). Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 547–569

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ---. (2004). The Evolutionary Landscape of Religion. NY: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger, S. (2006). Cultural transmission and diffusion. In L. Nadel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. London: MacMillian

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1962). Social learning through imitation. In M.R. Jones (ed.) Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1962. Lincoln, Nebraska, 211–274

  • ---. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall

    Google Scholar 

  • Belpaeme, T., & Bleys, J. (2005). Explaining universal color categories through a constrained acquisition process. Adaptive Behavior, 13(4), 293–310

    Google Scholar 

  • Benedict, R. (1959). Patterns of Culture. Boston: Houghton Mifflin

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackmore, S. (1999). The Meme Machine. Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloch, M. (2005). Essays on Cultural Transmission. Oxford: Berg

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (2002). Habitus. In J. Hillier, & E. Rooksby (Eds.), Habitus: A Sense of Place (pp. 27–34). Burlington: Ashgate

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2011). A Cooperative Species. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R. (2018). A Different Kind of Animal. Princeton: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., Bogerhoff-Mulder, M., Durham, W. H., & Richerson, P. J. (1997). Are cultural phylogenies possible?. In P. Weingart, S. D. Mitchel, P. J. Richerson, & S. Maasen (Eds.), Human By Nature: Between Biology and the Social Sciences (pp. 355–386). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (2001). Religion Explained: The Evolutionary Origins of Religious Thought. Berkeley: University of California Press

    Google Scholar 

  • --- (2003). Are ghost concepts “intuitive,” “endemic,” and “innate?”. Journal of Cognition Robert and Culture, 3, 233–243

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Buskell, A. (2017a). What are cultural attractors? Biology & Philosophy, 32(3), 377–394

    Google Scholar 

  • --- (2017b). Cultural attractor theory and explanation. Philosophy, Theory, and Practice in Biology, 9(13), 2475–3025

    Google Scholar 

  • Buskell, A., Enquist, M., & Jannson, F. (2019). A systems approach to cultural evolution. Palgrave Communications, 5, 131–146

    Google Scholar 

  • Chudek, M., & Henrich, J. (2011). Culture-gene coevolution, norm psychology and the emergence of human prosociality. Trends in Cognitive Science, 15(5), 218–226

    Google Scholar 

  • Claidière, N., Thom Scott-Phillips, and Dan Sperber (2014). How Darwinian is Cultural Evolution? Philosophical Transations of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 20130368

    Google Scholar 

  • Cownden, D., Eriksson, K., & Strimling, P. (2017). A popular misapplication of evolutionary modeling to the study of human cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38, 421–427

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens. NY: Harcourt Brace

    Google Scholar 

  • ---. (2010). Self Comes to Mind. NY: Pantheon

    Google Scholar 

  • ---. (2018). The Strange Order of Things. NY: Pantheon

    Google Scholar 

  • De Leersnyder, J., Boiger, M., & Mesquita, B. (2013). Cultural regulation of emotion: individual, relational, and structural sources. Frontiers in Psychology 4 (Article, 55, 1–11

    Google Scholar 

  • Downey, G. (2010). ‘Practice without theory’: a neuroanthropological perspective on embodied learning. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (N.S.) 16. S22 – S40

  • Driscoll, C. (2011). Fatal attraction? Why Sperber’s attractors do not prevent cumulative cultural evolution. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 62(2), 301–322

    Google Scholar 

  • Elkins, J. (1996). The Object Stares Back. NY: Simon & Schuster

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellemers, N., & Haslam, S. A. (2011). Social identity theory. In Van P. A. M. Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology, Vol. Two (pp. 379–398). Newbury Park, CA: SAGE

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellemers, N., Paglino, S., & Barreto, M. (2013). Morality and behavioral regulation in groups: A social identity approach. European Review of Social Psychology, 24, 160–193

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans-Prichart, E. E. (1937). Witchcraft, oracles and magic among the Azande. Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, S. A. (2012). Natural selection IV. The Price equation. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 25, 1002–1019

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • ---. (2018). The Price equation program: Simple invariances unify population dynamics, thermodynamics, probability, information and inference. Entropy, 20, 978–1003

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gabora, L. (1998). Autocatalytic closure in a cognitive system: A tentative scenario for the origin of culture. Psycoloquy, 9(67)

  • --- (2004). Ideas are not replicators but minds are. Biology & Philosophy, 19, 127–143

    Google Scholar 

  • --- (2006). Self-other organization: Why early life did not evolve through natural selection. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 241, 443–450

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ---. (2011). Five clarifications about cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 11, 61–83

    Google Scholar 

  • ---. (2013a). An evolutionary framework for culture: selectionism versus communal exchange. Physics of Life Reviews, 10, 117–145

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ---(. (2013b). Cultural evolution as distributed computation. In P. Michelucci (Ed.), Handbook of Human Computation (pp. 447–462). Berlin: Springer

    Google Scholar 

  • ---( (2021). to appear How does culture evolve? In M. Gelfand, C. Chiu, and Y. Hong (eds.) 10th Volumn of the Annual Series on Advances in Culture and Psychology. NY: Oxford University Press

  • Gabora, L., & Aerts, D. (2009). A model of the emergence and evolution of integrated worldviews. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 53, 434–451

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabora, L., & Steel, M. (2017). Autocatalytic networks in cognition and the origin of culture. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 431, 87–95

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • -- (2020a). - A model of the transition to behavioral and cognitive modernity using reflexively autocatalytic networks. Journal of the Royal Society Interface 17(171), 1720200545 https://doi.org/10.1098/rsi/202025

  • ---( (2020b). Modeling a cognitive transition at the origin or cultural evolution using autocatalytic networks. Cognitive Science 44(9), e12878, https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12878

  • --- (2021). An evolutionary process without variation and selection. Journal of the Royal Society Interface 18(180), 20210334 https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2021.0334

  • Gabora, L., & Smith, C. M. (2018). Two cognitive transitions underlying the capacity for cultural evolution. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 96, 27–52

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • --- (2021). How does culture evolve?. In M. Gelfand, C. Chiu, & Y. Hong (Eds.), 10th Volume of the Annual Series on Advances in Culture and Psychology. NY: Oxford University Press

  • Gardner, A. (2020). Price’s equation made clear. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375, 2019361

    Google Scholar 

  • Gavrilets, S. (2020). The dynamics of injunctive social norms. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 2, 1–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Giesea (2017). It’s time to embrace memetic warfare. NATO ACT Open Publications 1(5)

  • Grusec, J. E. (1992). Social learning theory and developmental psychology: The legacies of Robert Sear and Albert Bandura. Developmental Psychology, 28(5), 776–786

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E. T. (1977). Beyond Culture. Garden City, NY: Anchor

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasson, U., Ghazanfar, A. A., Galantucci, B., Garrod, S., & Keysers, C. (2012). Brain-to-brain coupling: a mechanism for creating and sharing a social world. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(2), 114–121

    Google Scholar 

  • Heggli, O. A., Konvalinka, I., Cabral, J., Brattico, E., Kringelbach, M. L., & Vuust, P. (2021). Transient brain networks underlying interpersonal strategies during synchronized action. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(1–2), 19–30

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2016). The Secret of Our Success: How Culture is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Muthukrishna, M. (2021). The origins and psychology of human cooperation. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 72, 207–240

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (2016). How evolved psychological mechanisms empower cultural group selection. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 39, doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15000138

  • Ingold, T. (2001). The transmission of representations to the education of attention. In H. Whitehouse (Ed.), The Debated Mind: Evolutionary Psychology Versus Ethnography (pp. 113–153). Oxford: Berg

    Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., Kogan, A., Piff, P. K., & Saturn, S. R. (2014). The sociocultural appraisals, values, and emotions (SAVE) prosociality: Core processes from gene to meme. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 425–460

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, H. S., & Sasaki, J. Y. (2014). Cultural neuroscience: biology of the mind in cultural contexts. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 487–514

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., & Uskul, A. K. (2011). Culture, mind, and the brain: Current evidence and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 419–449

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kronfeldner, M. (2007). Is cultural evolution Lamarckian? Biology and Philosophy, 22(4), 493–512

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N. (2008). Exploring gene-culture interactions: insights from handedness, sexual selection and niche-construction case studies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 363, 3577–3589

    Google Scholar 

  • --- (2017). Darwin’s Unfinished Symphony. Princeton: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Feldman, M. W. (2001). Cultural niche construction and human evolution. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 14, 22–33

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leaf, M., & Read, D. (2012). Human thought and social organization: Anthropology on a new plane. New York: Lexington

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehtonen, J. (2020). The Price equation and the unity of social evolution theory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375, 20190362

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, H. M., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Transmission fidelity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 2171–2180

    Google Scholar 

  • Lind, J., Ghirlanda, S., & Enquist, M. (2019). Social learning through associative processes: a computational theory. Royal Society Open Science, 6, 181777

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, M. H., & Qirko, H. N. (1996). An evolutionary perspective on maladaptive traits and cultural conformity. American Journal of Human Biology, 8(5), 615–629

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ma, Y., Bang, D., Wang, C., Allen, M., Frith, C., Roepstorff, A., & Han, S. (2014). Sociocultural patterning of neural activity during self-reflection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 73–80

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mantovani, G. (2000). Exploring Borders. London: Routledge

    Google Scholar 

  • Margolis, J. (1999). Pierre Bourdieu: habitus and the logic of practice. In R. Shusterman (Ed.), Bourdieu: A Critical Reader (pp. 64–83). Oxford: Blackwell

    Google Scholar 

  • McElreath, R., & Boyd, R. (2007). Mathematical Models of Social Evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press

    Google Scholar 

  • McGeer, V. (2015). Mind-making practices: the social infrastructure of self-knowledge agency and responsibility. Philosophical Explorations: An International Journal for the Philosophy of Mind and Action, 18(2), 259–281

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H., & Sperber, D. (2017). The Enigma of Reason. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural Evolution: How Darwinian Theory can Explain Human Diversity and Synthesize the Social Sciences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press

    Google Scholar 

  • --- (2016). Cultural evolution: integrating psychology, evolution, and culture. Current Opinions in Psychology, 7, 17–22

    Google Scholar 

  • Molenberghs, P. (2013). The neuroscience of in-group bias. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 37, 1530–1536

    Google Scholar 

  • Morin, O. (2013). How portraits turn their eyes upon us: visual preference and demographic change in cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 222–229

    Google Scholar 

  • ---. (2018). Spontaneous emergence of legibility in writing systems: the case of orientation anisotropy. Cognitive Science, 42, 644–677

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, N., Manrai, A., & Manrai, L. (2014). Memes, Memetics, and Marketing: A State-of-the-Art Review and a Lifecycle Model of Meme Management in Advertising. NY: Routledge

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R., & Norenzayan, A. (2002). Culture and cognition. In S. Kitayama, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of Cultural Psychology (pp. 569–594). NY: Guilfort Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayen, A., Atran, S., Faulkner, J., & Shaller, M. (2006). Memory and mystery: the cultural selection of minimally counterintuitive narratives. Cognitive Science, 30, 531–553

    Google Scholar 

  • Okasha, S. (2005). Multilevel selection and the major transitions in evolution. Philosophy of Science, 72(5), 1013–1025

    Google Scholar 

  • ---. (2006). Evolution and the Levels of Selection. Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • --- (2016). The relation between kin and multi-level selection: An approach using causal graphs. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 67(2), 435–470

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D. (2011). Culture as situated cognition: Cultural mindsets, cultural fluency, and meaning making. European Review of Social Psychology, 22(1), 164–214

    Google Scholar 

  • Page, K. M., & Nowak, M. A. (2002). Unifying evolutionary dynamics. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 219, 93–98

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palecek, M. (2019). The evolution of ‘culture’: Juggling a concept. Anthropological Theory, 20(1), 53–76

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, N. (2005). Universals of child rearing. Anthropological Theory, 5, 477–516

    Google Scholar 

  • Read, D. (2012). How culture makes us human. Left Coast Press

  • --- (2015). Kinship terminology. International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences (pp. 61–66). Amsterdam: Elsevier

  • --- (2016). The human story: a cultural account. PsycCritiques 61(45), Article 7

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2000). Memes: universal acid or better mouse trap. (ed.) Darwinizing Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Science (pp. 143–162). Oxford: Oxford University Press. R. Aunger

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., Boyd, R., & Henrich, J. (2010). Gene-culture coevolution in the age of genomics. PNAS 107 (Supplement, 2, 8985–8992

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., Baldini, R., Bell, A. V., Demps, K., Frost, K., Hillis, V. … Zefferman, M. (2016). Cultural group selection play an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 30–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodley, C. (2016). When memes go to war: viral propaganda in the 2014 Gaza-Israel conflict.Fibreculture Journal27

  • Roepstorff, A., Niewöhner, J., & Beck, S. (2010). Enculturing brains through patterned practices. Neural Networksd, 23, 1051–1059

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch, & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and Categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

    Google Scholar 

  • Sahlins, M. (2013). What kinship is … and is not. Chicago: University of Chicago Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapolsky, R. M. (2017). Behave. NY: Penguin

    Google Scholar 

  • Schore, A. N. (2013). Bowlby’s “environment of evolutionary adaptedness”: Recent studies on the interpersonal neurobiology of attachment and emotional development. In D. Narvaes, J. Panksepp, A. N. Schore, & T. R. Gleason (Eds.), Evolution, Early Experience and Human Development: From Research to Practicxe and Policy (pp. 31–67). NY: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott-Phillips, T., Blancke, S., & Heintz, C. (2019). Methods for studying cultural attraction. Evolutionary Anthropology, 28(1), 18–20

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 1–65

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S., & Bardi, A. (2001). Value hierarchies across cultures: Taking a similarities perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 268–290

    Google Scholar 

  • Smaldino, P. E. (2014). The cultural evolution of emergent group-level traits. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 270–271

    Google Scholar 

  • --- (2019). The evolution of the social self: Multidimensionality of social identity solves the coordination problems of a society. In W. C. Wimsatt, & A. C. Love (Eds.), Beyond the meme: Articulating dynamic structures in cultural evolution. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press

  • Smith, D. (2020). Cultural group selection and human cooperation: a conceptual and empirical review. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 2 e11, 1–29

    Google Scholar 

  • Soules, M. (2015). Media, Persuasion and Propaganda. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Oxford: Blackwell

    Google Scholar 

  • --- (1998). Are folk taxonomies “memes”? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(4), 589–590

    Google Scholar 

  • --- (2000). What memes won’t do: An objection to the memetic approach to culture. In R. Aunger (Ed.), Darwinizing Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Science (pp. 163–174). Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • --- (2006). Why a deep understanding of cultural evolution is incompatible with shallow psychology. In N. Enfield, & S. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of Human Sociality (pp. 431–449). NY: Routledge

    Google Scholar 

  • ---. (2012). Cultural attractors. In J. Brockman (Ed.), This will make you smarter. NY: Harper

  • Spiro, M. E. (1987). Collective representations and mental representations in religious symbol systems. In M. E. Spiro, B. Kilborne, & L. L. Langness (Eds.), Culture and Human Nature: Theoretical Papers of Melford (pp. 161–184). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. E. Spiro

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephens, G. J., Silbert, L. J., & Hasson, U. (2010). Speaker-listener neural coupling underlies successful communication. PNAS, 107(32), 14425–14430

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2006). Memes revisited. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 57, 145–165

    Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, J. V. (2018). Learning and embodied cognition: A review and proposal. Psychology Learning & Teaching, 17(2), 128–143

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2006). Uniquely human cognition is a product of human culture. In S. C. Levinson, & P. Jaisson (Eds.), Evolution and Culture. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

  • --- (2018). Becoming Human: A Theory of Ontogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. (1972). The Analysis of Subjective Culture. NY: Wiley

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Toorn, J., Ellemers, N., & Doosje, B. (2015). The threat of moral transgression: The impact of group membership and moral opportunity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 609–622

    Google Scholar 

  • Voorhees, B., Read, D., & Gabora, L. (2018). The objectivity of moral norms is a top down cultural construct. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, 31–32

    Google Scholar 

  • --- (2020). Identity, kinship, and the evolution of cooperation. Current Anthropology, 61(2), 194–218

    Google Scholar 

  • West, S., Ashleigh, G., & Gardner, A. (2007a). Evolutionary explanations for cooperation. Current Biology, 17(16), R661–R672

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • West, S., Ashleigh, G., & Gardner, A. (2007b). Social semantics: How useful has group selection been? Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 21, 374–385

    Google Scholar 

  • West, S., & Gardner, A. (2013). Adaptation and inclusive fitness. Current Biology, 23, R577–R584

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, C. (2010). Cultural distortions of self- and reality-perception. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 17(7–8), 95–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner, P. W. (2014). Embers of society: Firelight talk among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen. PNAS, 111(39), 14027–14035

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wiggins, B. E., Bowers, G., & Bret (2015). Memes as genera: a structural analysis of the memescape. New Media & Society 17(11)

  • Wordsworth, W. (1850). In W. Wordsworth (Ed.), The Prelude, Book, or Growth of a Poet’s Mind. London: Edward Moxon

  • Zefferman, M. R., & Richerson, P. J. (2014). Many important group-level traits are institutions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 280–281

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zipes, J. (1993). The Trials and Tribulations of Little Red Riding Hood. London: Routledge. )

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

Not Applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Burton Voorhees.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest/Competing Interests

None.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Voorhees, B. What Are Group Level Traits and How Do They Evolve?. Integr. psych. behav. 57, 913–936 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-022-09689-1

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-022-09689-1

Keywords

Navigation