Skip to main content
Log in

The Dance of Attention: Toward an Aesthetic Dimension of Attention-Deficit

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

What role does the aesthetics of bodily movement play in the understanding of attention among children diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)? This article animates a phenomenological approach to attention and embodiment with a special focus on the relation between aesthetic or expressive bodily movement and behavioral awareness in children diagnosed with ADHD. However, beyond this it is argued that the aesthetic aspect of movement calls for an expansion of the phenomenological perspective. In this context Gilles Deleuze’s notion of aesthetics as a “science of the sensible” is activated and discussed in relation to the phenomenological concept of perception. Empirically the article takes point of departure in a qualitative study conducted with a group of children with attention-deficit practicing the Afro-Brazilian marital art, capoeira. Combining ethnographic and phenomenological methods, it is demonstrated that capoeira can be considered a form of aesthetic movement that offers a transition of attention-deficit into a productive force of expression that changes the notions of sensation and movement in ADHD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. There are exceptions to this overall claim. Phenomenological scholars as Alva Nöe (2000, 2012), Maxine Sheets-Johnstone (1966, 2011), Dorothée Legrand (2007; Legrand & Ravn, 2009), and Joel Krueger (2009, 2011, 2014) all write on phenomenological issues related to art, aesthetic experience or different artistic practices. However, these theorists more or less introduce art or aesthetic practice as paradigmatic or exemplary for perception in general. In Alva Nöe’s words, “what is true of the experience of the work of art is true of human experience quite generally” (Noë 2012, p. 2). In this image art or aesthetic experience is proposed as an expression of lived experience or “perception in general,” and thus remains within a representational image of art in the sense that art is described as a representation of a presupposed “natural” activity of perception – presupposing a fundamental nature behind “natural” perception. What is not in focus is an account of the genuinely creative, productive or expressive aspect of art. For a more in depth treatment of this see (Levin 2016a)

  2. In this sense the notion of an aesthetic system is associated with the anthropologist Clifford Geertz’s notion of art as a cultural system. As he argues The artist works with his audience’s capacities – capacities to see, or hear, or touch, sometimes even to taste and smell, with understanding. And though elements of these capacities are indeed innate – it usually helps not to be color blind – they are brought into actual existence by the experience of living in the midst of certain sorts of things to look at, listen to, handle, think about, cope with, and react to; […] Art and the equipment to grasp it are made in the same shop. (1976, p. 1497)

  3. The terms emic and etic are derived from the linguistic notions of phonemic and phonetic, which refers respectively to sound differences that are consciously perceived by listeners or speakers as belonging to the language, and to physical sound differences that that can be observed by a listener, but are not necessarily part of the language’s repertoire.

    In anthropology the terms emic and etic were coined by the linguist Pike (1967) and have been used to describe the contrast between the insiders’ indigenous explications in contrast to descriptions according to the observer’s outsider’s perspective.

  4. The triangular footwork and rhythmic swaying from side to side considered as one of the basic foundational elements of capoeira movement.

  5. A cartwheel

  6. Such as an acrobatic game, an aggressive fight oriented game or more playful or trickery game.

  7. However, as I have pointed out elsewhere expressive movement in Sheets-Johnstones conception does not consider the pre-individual aspect of expression, thus she reduces expression to a particular subjective body of movement. Based on introspection and kinetic free variation Sheets-Johnstone reduces self-movement to four primary experiential structures of self-movement in tensional, linear, amplitudinal, and projectional qualities.

  8. In fact, as I have described elsewhere(Levin 2017) the neuropsychological theory of ADHD relies on its own aesthetic model of the mind in the image as a conductor of a symphony orchestra.

References

  • APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Browning, B. (1995). Samba resistance in motion. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capoeira, N. (2002). Capoeira: Roots of the dance-fight-game. Berkeley: North Atlantic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deleuze, G. (1993). Fold: Leibniz and the baroque. (T. Conley, Trans.). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

  • Deleuze, G. (2004). Difference and repetition. (P. Patton, Trans.). New York: Continuum International Publishing Group.

  • Deleuze, G. (2003). Francis bacon: The logic of sensation. (D. W. Smith, Trans.). London: Continuum.

  • Deleuze, G., & Guattari, F. (1987). A thousand plateaus: Capitalism and schizophrenia. (B. Massumi, Trans.). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

  • Deleuze, G., & Guattari, F. (1994). What is philosophy? (H. Tomlinson & G. Burchell, Trans.). London & New York: Columbia University Press.

  • Downey, G. (2002). Listening to capoeira: Phenomenology, embodiment, and the materiality of music. Ethnomusicology, 46(3), 487. https://doi.org/10.2307/852720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Downey, G. (2005). Learning capoeira : Lessons in cunning from an afro-Brazilian art. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downey, G. (2014). Capoeira as an art of living: The aesthetics of a cunning existence. In K. Gilbert (Ed.), Transforming aesthetics. Champaign: Common Ground Publ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J., & Spence, C. (2000). Multisensory perception: Beyond modularity and convergence. Current Biology, 10(20), R731–R735. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00740-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1984). What is enlightenment? In P. Rabinow (Ed. & Trans.), The Foucault reader. New York: Pantheon Books.

  • Foucault, M. (1986). The Care of the Self. Volume 3 of the history of sexuality. (R. Hurley, Trans.). New York: Pantheon Books.

  • Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Clarendon Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/0199271941.001.0001.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Zahavi, D. (2007). The phenomenological mind: An introduction to philosophy of mind and cognitive science. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (1976). Art as a cultural system. MLN. Comparative Literature, 91(6), 1473–1499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gendlin, E. (1981). Focusing. New York: Bantam Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grönlund, E., Renck, B., & Weibull, J. (2005). Dance/movement therapy as an alternative treatment for young boys diagnosed as ADHD: A pilot study. American Journal of Dance Therapy, 27(2), 63–85. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10465-005-9000-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hastrup, K. (1994). Anthropological knowledge incorporated. In K. Hastrup & P. Hervik (Eds.), Social experience and anthropological knowledge (pp. 168–181). London: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203449646_chapter_12.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J. (2014). Affordances and the musically extended mind. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01003

  • Krueger, J.W. (2009). Enacting Musical Experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16(2–3), 98–123.

  • Krueger, J.W. (2011.) Doing things with music. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 10(1), 1–22.

  • Kvale, S. (1996). Interviews : An introduction to qualitative research interviewing. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lalanne, C., & Lorenceau, J. (2004). Crossmodal integration for perception and action. Journal of Physiology, Paris, 98(1–3), 265–279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2004.06.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Legrand, D. (2007). Pre-Reflective Self-Consciousness: On Being Bodily in the World. Janus Head, 9(2), 493–519.

  • Legrand, D., & Ravn, S. (2009). Perceiving subjectivity in bodily movement: The case of dancers. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8(3), 389–408. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-009-9135-5

  • Levin, K. (2013). Becoming worthy of what happens to us: Art and subjectivity in the philosophy of Gilles Deleuze. In R. Tone & J. Lang (Eds.), Art and identity: Essays on the aesthetic creation of mind (consciousness, literature & the arts) (pp. 135–167). Amsterdam: Editions Rodopi B.V.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, K. (2016a). Aesthetic movements of embodied minds: Between Merleau-Ponty and Deleuze. Continental Philosophy Review, 49(2), 181–202. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11007-016-9376-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, K. (2016b). Aesthetics of hyperactivity: A study of the role of expressive movement in ADHD and capoeira. American Journal of Dance Therapy, 38(1), 41–62. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10465-016-9211-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, K. (2017). ADHD, expression, and the incorporeal materialism of movement. Theory & Psychology., 27(6), 774–792. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354317735600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manning, E., & Massumi, B. (2014). Thought in the act : Passages in the ecology of experience. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. https://doi.org/10.5749/minnesota/9780816679669.001.0001.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (2002). Phenomenology of perception. (C. Smith, Trans.). London: Routledge.

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (2007). Indirect Language and the Voices of Silence. In T. Toadvine & L. Lawlor (Eds.), R. McCleary (Trans.), The Merleau-Ponty reader (pp. 241–282). Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

  • Noë, A. (2000). Experience and Experiment in Art. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7(8–9), 123–136.

  • Noë, A. (2012). Varieties of presence. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. https://doi.org/10.4159/harvard.9780674063013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rapp, B., & Hendel, S. K. (2003). Principles of cross-modal competition: Evidence from deficits of attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(1), 210–219. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pike, K.L. (1967). Language in Relation to a Unified Theory of the Structure of Human Behavior. Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter.

  • Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sánchez García, R. (2013). Carnal ethnography as path to embodied knowledge. In Fighting scholars: Habitus and ethnographies of martial arts and combat sports (pp. 1–19). London: Anthem Press, https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctt1gxpddq.5.

  • Sheets-Johnstone, M. (1966). The phenomenology of dance. Madison & Milwaukee: The University of Wisconsin Press.

  • Sheets-Johnstone, M. (2011). The primacy of movement (expanded). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1075/aicr.82.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, D. W. (1996). Deleuze’s theory of sensation: Overcoming the Kantian duality. In P. Patton (Ed.), Deleuze : a critical reader (pp. 29–56). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stelter, R. (2000). The transformation of body experience into language. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 31(1), 63–77. https://doi.org/10.1163/156916200746256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stelter, R. (2010). Experience-based, body-anchored qualitative research interviewing. Qualitative Health Research, 20(6), 859–867. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732310364624.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and Developmental Psychology. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J., Thompson, E. T., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wacquant, L. (2004). Body & Soul: Notebooks of an apprentice boxer. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kasper Levin.

Additional information

Capoeira is a practice that combines fighting techniques with dance elements and comprises music and ritual aspects. In this sense, capoeira surpasses the common notion of martial art as a combat or self-defense system and is notoriously difficult to classify. Some scholars have suggested the more descriptively precise category of a ritual dance-fight-game (Capoeira 2002).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Levin, K. The Dance of Attention: Toward an Aesthetic Dimension of Attention-Deficit. Integr. psych. behav. 52, 129–151 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-017-9413-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-017-9413-7

Keywords

Navigation