Skip to main content
Log in

Object Pragmatics and Language Development

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of this contribution is to investigate the advent of language in the light of the appropriation of the cultural uses of the material objects related to material culture and the constitution of their public and shared meanings linked to their uses. First, we suggest that the Object Pragmatics paradigm offers a framework which allows us to take into account the uses of objects in daily life as a site of social conventions, communication and public and shared meanings. Second, we would like to underline the key role of the adult’s mediations in the child’s ability to evolve towards linguistic development. This contribution will discuss the notion of scenario involving primarily the object, as a possible semiotic tool to support the child’s transition to language. We will finally illustrate that it is possible to take into consideration the mastery of conventional uses of the object in the child’s ability to engage in a scenario and then to move towards communication and speech development. These issues will be addressed in the context of a research project which focuses on the observation of children interacting with an adult at 16, 20 and 24 months. These longitudinal data were collected by video in a semi-experimental triadic interaction design. The triadic interaction is considered as a relevant unit for the observation and analysis of the role of material culture in speech development, suggesting the existence of new mechanisms to be taken into account in addition to the interactive conditions largely mentioned in literature.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For reasons of simplicity, later in the text, the term Language will be used for Articulated Language.

  2. Object Pragmatics consider that early development is cultural-historical before language through the appropriation by infants of canonical uses related to objects artifacts, transmitted through the adult’s signs in adult-infant-object triadic interaction. Involving two familiar objects (a “vintage” Fisher Price telephone and a sorter box), this study shows that the canonical uses of objects are gradually constructed in triadic interaction. At the age of 7 months, the object is not yet used in a canonical way. At the age of 10 months, the child begins to be able to use the object in its canonical dimensions (proto-canonical). The child appropriates the first canonical uses of objects at the age of 13 months.

  3. The works of Trevarthen on intersubjectivity have been pioneers in the field of early development emphasizing the importance of social and interactive aspects in the ability of the child to develop her communication skills. The importance of the first interaction with the adult as dialogue premises (“primary intersubjectivity” (Trevarthen and Hubley 1978; Trevarthen 1979; Bruner 1983)) has been excellently described. However, the works on secondary intersubjectivity involving triadic child-adult-object relationships bring no further explanation about the role of objects and their public or shared meaning in the building communication.

  4. This study shows that the adult employs different semiotic tools, which pave the way for language based on joint activity around the object. At this stage of our enquiry, we have analyzed two different forms: scenario and labeling sessions (Béguin 2016).

  5. The concept of scenario is also used in literature and its origin is to be found in Umberto Eco’s Lector in Fabula (Eco 1985). For a review of this concept from a literary perspective, see for example the text by Vermeesch (2004).

  6. In the theater of the absurd for example, the code, which permits to show that this is not a realistic play, is given in the first minutes, allowing the spectator to be warned about the level in which it is possible to understand the play.

  7. Mainstream perspectives in literature talking about symbolic play focus on representation, considering the child’s ability to enter into symbolic play as a new mental competence (for Piaget, pretend play is the product of semiotic function as language, deferred imitation or mental picture (e.g. Piaget 1946; Stambak and Sinclair 1990). Additional works conducted on Object Pragmatics (Rodríguez and Moro 2002; Rodríguez et al. 2014) have suggested that first symbols emerge from canonical uses of objects and therefore develop a perspective in which symbolic uses take roots in a previous ability to master conventions through object uses. This alternative view about symbol puts forward the importance of meanings related to objects in the child’s ability to emancipate itself through symbolic behaviours.

  8. The special characteristics of the objects selected for these observations can play an important role in these first results. Indeed, the objects that constitute the dining set concern an event of everyday life very familiar to the child and common knowledge about these objects whose uses are largely shared. We would not be able to achieve the same results with another kind of object, especially for the observation of a chain of symbolic uses. This consideration points out the importance of the characteristics of the objects in the child’s ability to appropriate uses.

  9. The ELAN software is a professional tool for the creation of complex annotations on video and audio resources developed by the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics in the Netherlands: http://tla.mpi.nl/tools/tla-tools/elan/

  10. These scenarii evolve in different manners: i) they are first initiated by the adult and then appropriated and initiated by the child, ii) the shared meaning regarding object uses evolves, allowing the child to share meaning at the communicative level, iii) the dominance of the semiotic system which organizes the interaction goes from material system to communicative systems.

  11. Putting the plates on the pan is what we consider as a protocanonical use. This kind of use attests to the child’s knowledge about specific characteristics of the objet and refers to the inference “if something is round then put it into a round container” organized around the social meaning of tidying up.

  12. This sharing of meanings grounded in materiality is built through communication via the adult’s mediation, emphasising the interrelationship between cognitive and communicative development in the pre-verbal stage.

References

  • Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3½-and 4½-month-old infants. Developmental psychology, 23(5), 655–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. In a. Kasher (Ed.), Pragmatics: Critical concepts (pp. 274–295). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Benigni, I., Bretherton, L., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). Cognition and communication from nine to thirteen months: Correlational findings. In E. Bates (Ed.), The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy (pp. 69–140). New-York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Béguin, M. (2016). Statut et rôle du système sémiotique matériel dans l’avènement de la communication verbale: Illustration de l’entrée progressive de l’enfant dans une activité de dénomination

  • Bruner, J. (1974). From communication to language—a psychological perspective. Cognition, 3(3), 255–287. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(74)90012-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2(1), 1–19. doi:10.1017/S0305000900000866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1982). The formats of language acquisition. American Journal of Semiotics, 1.

  • Bruner, J. (1983). Le développement de l’enfant: Savoir faire, savoir dire. Paris: PUF.

  • Bruner, J. (1987). Comment les enfants apprennent à parler (Vol. 14). Paris: Retz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dimitrova, N. (2012). Avènement et développement de la communication intentionnelle chez l’enfant entre 8 et 16 mois dans l’interaction triadique enfant-objet-adulte à partir du paradigme de la pargmatique de l’objet. Thesis presented at the University of Lausanne.

  • Dimitrova, N. (2014). L’usage de l’objet, un lieu privilégié d’émergence de la communication intentionnelle chez l’enfant. In C. Moro & N. Müller-Mirza (Eds.), Sémiotique, culture et développpement psychologique. Villeneuve d’Ascq: Presses Universitaires du Septentrion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dimitrova, N., & Moro, C. (2013). Common ground on object use associates with caregivers’ gesturese. Infant Behavior and Development, 36(4), 618–626. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.06.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dimitrova, N., Moro, C., & Mohr, C. (2015). Caregivers interpret infants’ early gestures based on shared knowledge about referents. Infant Behavior and Development, 39, 98–106. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.02.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dupertuis, V., & Moro, C. (2016). Self-directed ostensions and mediations of the adult at the age of 8-, 12- and 16 months. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science. doi:10.1007/s12124-016-9350-x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eco, U. (1985). Lector in fabula. Paris: Grasset & Fasquelle.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garvey, C. (1974). Some properties of social play. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 20(3), 163–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mélon, M.-E. (2002). Scénario. In P. Aron, A. A. V. Viala & D. D. S. J. Saint-Jacques (Eds.), Le dictionnaire du littéraire.

  • Morilhat, C. (2005). Empire du langage ou impérialisme langagier?(Pages deux ed.). Lausanne: Cahiers libres.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moro, C. (2011). Material culture, semiotics and early childhood development. In M. Kontopodis, C. Wulf & B. Fichtner (Eds.), Children, development and education: Cultural, historical, anthropological perspectives (pp. 55–70). London, New York: Springer Verlag.

  • Moro, C. (2014a). Le rôle de l’usage de l’objet dans la construction de l’attention conjointe et dans l’accès aux intentions d’autrui. In C. Moro & N. Müller-Mirza (Eds.), Sémiotique, culture et développpement psychologique. Villeneuve d’Ascq: Presses Universitaires du Septentrion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moro, C. (2014b). Le référent dans l’intersubjectivité secondaire: Un objet aussi ignoré que “l’autre face de la lune”? In C. Moro, N. Müller-Mirza, & P. Roman (Eds.), L’intersubjectivité en questions: Agégat ou nouveau concept fédérateur pour la psychologie (pp. 69–106). Lausanne: Antipodes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moro, C. (2016). To encounter, to build the world and to become a human being. Advocating for a material-cultural turn in psychology. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science. doi:10.1007/s12124-016-9356-4.

  • Moro, C., & Rodríguez, C. (1989). L’interaction triadique bébé-objet-adulte durant la première année de la vie de l’enfant. Enfance, 42(1–2), 75–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moro, C., & Rodríguez, C. (1998). Towards a pragmatical conception of the object: The construction of the uses of the objects by the baby in the prelinguistic period. In M. C. D. P. Lyra & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Child development within culturally structured environments: Construction of psychological processes in interpersonal communication (Vol. 4, pp. 53–72). Stamford: Ablex Publishing Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moro, C., & Rodríguez, C. (2005). L’objet et la construction de son usage chez le bébé. Une approche sémiotique du développement préverbal. Berne: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moro, C., Dupertuis, V., Fardel, S., & Piguet, O. (2015). Investigation of consciousness through ostensions toward oneself from the onset of the use-of-object to first language. Cognitive Development, 36, 150–160. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.09.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1977). Cognitive development and the acquisition of concepts. In R. C. Anderson, R. J. Spiro, & W. E. Montague (Eds.), Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: the emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K., & Gruendel, J. (1986). Event knowledge: Structure and function in development. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1936/1977). La naissance de l’intelligence chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel-Paris: Delachaux et Niestlé.

  • Piaget, J. (1946/1978). La formation du symbole chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel-Paris: Delachaux et Niestlé.

  • Piaget, J. (1961). Les mécanismes perceptifs: Modèles probabilistes, analyse génétique, relations avec l’intelligence. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1974). La prise de conscience. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Racine, T. P., & Carpendale, J. I. M. (2007a). The role of shared practice in joint attention. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 3–25. doi:10.1348/026151006X119756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Racine, T. P., & Carpendale, J. I. M. (2007b). Shared practices, understanding, language and joint attention. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 45–54. doi:10.1348/026151006X162613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C., & Moro, C. (2002). Objeto, comunicación y símbolo. Una mirada a los primeros usos simbólicos de los objetos. Estudios de psicología, 23(3), 323–338. doi:10.1174/021093902762224416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C., Palacios, P., Cardenas, K., & Yuste, N. (2014). Les symboles: Formes de second ou de troisième sens? In C. Moro & N. Müller-Mirza (Eds.), Sémiotique, culture et développement psychologique. Villeneuve d’Ascq: Presses Universitaires du Septentrion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossmanith, N., Costall, A., Reichelt, A. F., López, B., & Reddy, V. (2014). Jointly structuring triadic spaces of meaning and action: Book sharing from 3 months on. Frontiers in psychology, 5. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01390.

  • Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

  • Sève, L. (2014). Présentation. In M. Brossard & L. Sève (Eds.), Histoire du développement des fonctions psychiques supérieures. Paris: La Dispute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, H., Stambak, M., Lezine, I., Rayna, S., & Verba, M. (1982). Les bébés et les choses. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S. (1990). Principles of object perception. Cognitive science, 14(1), 29–56. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1401_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stambak, M., & Sinclair, H. (1990). Les jeux de fiction entre enfants de 3 ans. Paris: PUF.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1988). The role of joint attentional processes in early language development. Language Sciences, 10(1), 69–88. doi:10.1016/0388-0001(88)90006-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambrige: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torok, J.-P. (1986). Le scénario: Histoire, théorie, pratique. Paris: Henry Veyrier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 321–347). London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2003). Intersubjectivité chez le nourrisson: Recherche, théorie et application clinique. Devenir, 15(4), 309–428. doi:10.3917/dev.034.0309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 183–229). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veneziano, E. (2000). Interaction, conversation et acquisition du langage dans les trois premières années L’acquisition du langage, l’émergence du langage. vol. 1 (pp. 231–265).

  • Vermeesch, A. (2004). Poétique du scénario. Poétique, (2), 213–234. doi:10.3917/poeti.138.0213.

  • Vygotski, L.S. (1930–31/2014). Histoire du développement des fonctions psychiques supérieures (trans: F. Sève, M. Brossard & L. Sève Eds.). Paris: La Dispute.

  • Vygotski, L.S. (1934/1997). Pensée et langage. Paris: La Dispute.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marie Béguin.

Additional information

Thesis directed by Professor Christiane Moro (Lausanne University) and entitled: The child’s entry into language: from practices around object uses to linguistic practices.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Béguin, M. Object Pragmatics and Language Development. Integr. psych. behav. 50, 603–620 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-016-9361-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-016-9361-7

Keywords

Navigation