Skip to main content
Log in

Granger Causality—Statistical Analysis Under a Configural Perspective

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The concept of Granger causality can be used to examine putative causal relations between two series of scores. Based on regression models, it is asked whether one series can be considered the cause for the second series. In this article, we propose extending the pool of methods available for testing hypotheses that are compatible with Granger causation by adopting a configural perspective. This perspective allows researchers to assume that effects exist for specific categories only or for specific sectors of the data space, but not for other categories or sectors. Configural Frequency Analysis (CFA) is proposed as the method of analysis from a configural perspective. CFA base models are derived for the exploratory analysis of Granger causation. These models are specified so that they parallel the regression models used for variable-oriented analysis of hypotheses of Granger causation. An example from the development of aggression in adolescence is used. The example shows that only one pattern of change in aggressive impulses over time Granger-causes change in physical aggression against peers.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexandrowicz, R. (2008). Wieviel ist “ein bisserl”? Ein neuer Zugang zum BIC im Rahmen von Latent Class Analysen. In J. Reinecke & C. Tarnai (Eds.), Klassifikationsanalysen in theorie und praxis (pp. 141–165). Münster: Waxmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, Y., & Rousson, V. (2000). Direction dependence in a regression line. Communications in Statistics - Theory and Methods, 32, 2053–2057.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, Y., & Rousson, V. (2001). On asymmetric properties of the correlation coefficient in the regression setting. The American Statistician, 55, 51–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, Y., & Yadegari, I. (2010). On direction of dependence. Metrika, 72, 130–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. E., & Granger, C. W. J. (1987). Cointegration and error correction: representation, estimation, and testing. Econometrica, 55, 251–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finkelstein, J. W., von Eye, A., & Preece, M. A. (1994). The relationship between aggressive behavior and puberty in normal adolescents: a longitudinal study. Journal of Adolescent Health, 15, 319–326.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foster, E. M. (2012). Causal inference, identification, and plausibility. In B. Laursen, T. D. Little, & N. E. Card (Eds.), Handbook of developmental research methods (pp. 17–30). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gates, K. M., Molenaar, P. C. M., Hillary, F. G., Ram, N., & Rovine, M. J. (2010). Automatic search for fMRI connectivity mapping: an alternative to Granger causality testing using formal equivalences among SEM path modeling, VAR, and unified SEM. NeuroImage, 50, 1118–1125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Granger, C. W. J. (1969). Investigating causal relations by economietric models and cross-spectral methods. Journal of Econometrics, 36, 424–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Havránek, T., & Lienert, G. A. (1984). Local and regional versus global contingency testing. Biometrical Journal, 26, 483–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, P. (1986). Statistics and causal inference. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 81, 945–970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, B. S., & Copenhaver, M. D. (1987). An improved sequentially rejective Bonferroni test procedure. Biometrics, 43, 417–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 65–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaccard, J. (2013). Theory Construction and Causal Modeling. Workshop presented at the 2013 Annual Conference of The Society for Social Work and Research, San Diego, January 16–20.

  • Kim, D., & Kim, J.-M. (2013). Analysis of directional dependence using asymmetric copula-based regression models. Journal of Statistical Computation and Simulation. doi:10.1080/00949655.2013.779696.

  • Lienert, G. A., & Krauth, J. (1973). Die Konfigurationsfrequenzanalyse V. Kontingenzund Interaktionsstrukturanalyse multinär skalierter Merkmale. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 21, 26–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lienert, G. A., & Krauth, J. (1975). Configural frequency analysis as a statistical tool for defining types. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 35, 231–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellenbergh, G. J. (1996). Other Null model, other (anti)type. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 45, 329–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mignon, V. (2008). Économétrie. Théorie et applications. Paris: Economica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearl, J. (2012). The causal foundations of structural equation modeling. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Handbook of structural equation modeling (pp. 68–91). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K. (1934). Logik der Forschung (the logic of scientific discovery). Wien: Julius Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pornprasertmanit, S., & Little, T. D. (2012). Determining directional dependency in causal associations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 36, 313–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, D. B. (1974). Estimating causal effects of treatments in randomized and nonrandomized studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 688–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schuster, C., & von Eye, A. (2000). Using log-linear modeling to increase power in two-sample Configural Frequency Analysis. Psychologische Beiträge, 42, 273–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics, 6, 461–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sims, C. A. (1980). Macroeconomics and reality. Econometrica, 48, 1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, M. E. (1990). Effect analysis and causation in linear structural equation models. Psychometrika, 55, 495–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Eye, A. (2002). Configural Frequency Analysis—methods, models, and applications. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Eye, A. (2004). Base models for Configural Frequency Analysis. Psychology Science, 46, 150–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Eye, A., & Bergman, L. R. (2003). Research strategies in developmental psychopathology: dimensional identity and the person-oriented approach. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 553–580.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Eye, A., & Brandtstädter, J. (1997). Configural Frequency Analysis as a searching device for possible causal relationships. Methods of Psychological Research - Online, 2(2), 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Eye, A., & DeShon, R. P. (2008). Characteristics of measures of directional dependence—A Monte Carlo study. http://interstat.statjournals.net/YEAR/2008/articles/0802002.pdf Retrieved on 2/11/2013.

  • von Eye, A., & DeShon, R. P. (2012). Directional dependency in developmental research. International Journal of Behavior Development, 36, 303–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Eye, A., & Gardiner, J. C. (2004). Locating deviations from multivariate normality. Understanding Statistics, 3, 313–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Eye, A., & Gutiérrez-Peña, E. (2004). Configural Frequency Analysis—the search for extreme cells. Journal of Applied Statistics, 31, 981–997.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Eye, A., & Mair, P. (2011). On the effects of dichotomizing information. In A. A. Hernández & J. G. Hernández (Eds.), Memoria del XXV Foro Nacional de Estadística (pp. 11–19). Aguascalientes: Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Eye, A., & Mun, E.-Y. (2013). Log-linear modeling—concepts, interpretation and applications. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Eye, A., & Schuster, C. (1998). On the specification of models for Configural Frequency Analysis—sampling schemes in prediction CFA. Methods of Psychological Research - Online, 3, 55–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Eye, A., Mun, E. Y., & Mair, P. (2009). What carries a mediation process? Configural analysis of mediation. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 43, 228–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Eye, A., Mair, P., & Mun, E.-Y. (2010). Advances in Configural Frequency Analysis. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Wright, G. H. (1971). Explanation and understanding. New York: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiedermann, W., & von Eye, A. (2013). Some significance tests to determine the direction of effects in linear regression models (under editorial review).

  • Wiedermann, W., Hagmann, M., Kossmeier, M., & von Eye, A. (2013). Resampling techniques to determine direction of effects in linear regression models (under editorial review).

  • Williamson, J. (2011). Mechanistic theories of causality. Philosophy Compass, 6, 421–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, C. F. J., & Hamada, M. S. (2009). Experiments. Planning, analysis, and optimization (2nd ed.). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alexander von Eye.

Appendix A: The Comparison Matrices in the Configural Approach to Examining Granger Causality

Appendix A: The Comparison Matrices in the Configural Approach to Examining Granger Causality

In the configural approach to the analysis of Granger causation, two base models are estimated. The first can be based on the series of Y observations only, the second uses both the X and the Y series of observations. Alternatively, both models can be based on the cross-classification that is spanned by both the X and the Y series of observations. In certain applications, it may be interesting to compare the type/antitype patterns from the first and the second model. This comparison allows researchers to identify the location of associations that are explained without and with consideration of the X scores. In the text of this article, we discuss the larger table, that is, the one that includes both the X and the Y series of observations. Here, we discuss the comparison of the smaller with the larger tables. The following outcomes of the comparison of type/antitype patterns are possible.

  1. 1.

    The emerging pattern of type and antitypes is the same for the two base models. In this case, there is no support for configural, Granger-type causality of the series of Y-scores as it is (not) explained by the series of X-scores. It should be noted that this outcome is very unlikely to occur. The reason for this is that also including the X observations results in a much larger cross-classification than using only the Y observations. For a type from the smaller cross-classification to also emerge in the larger table, the pattern of Y categories that constitutes this type must, in the larger table, constitute a type for every cell that includes the Y pattern that constituted the type. Specifically, consider a series of T observations of Y. The cross-classification of these observations has c Y T cells, where c Y is the number of categories of variable Y. Now, also including the T observations of X increases the number of cells by the factor c X T, where c X is the number of categories of variable X. By implication, each pattern of categories of Y appears c X T times in the larger table. Thus, for a type to completely re-emerge in the larger cross-classification, it will have to emerge c X T times—a rather unlikely event. This applies accordingly to antitypes. The following outcome is more likely.

  2. 2.

    A type or antitype from the smaller cross-classification re-surfaces only in some of the c X T candidate cells of the larger cross-classification. This result is most interesting from a configural perspective. It suggests that, in a Granger causality sense, the series of X scores explains this type or antitype only for those patterns of X–Y observations for which types or antitypes emerge, but not for the remaining patterns. The reason for this interpretation is that the second CFA base model proposes independence between the X and the Y series of scores. Therefore, types and antitypes suggest where, in the cross-classification of the X and Y observations, there are local associations among the X and the Y observations.

  3. 3.

    Types or antitypes that emerged for the cross-classification of only the Y observations disappear entirely when the X observations are taken into account. In other words, none of the c X T candidate cells emerges as a type or antitype in the cross-classification that is spanned by the Y and the X observations. This result indicates that a local relation between the X and the Y series does not exist, for this pattern. Including the information from the X series makes this type or antitype disappear. Because of the way the base model was specified, neither main effects of the X and the Y observations nor the interactions among the X observations or the Y observations can be the reason why a type or antitype emerges. This suggests that the assumption of no relation between the Y and the X series of observations cannot be rejected, for this configuration. This interpretation is parallel to the interpretation of changes in the appearance of configural mediation types or antitypes (von Eye and Mun 2013; von Eye et al. 2009).

  4. 4.

    Configurations that did not constitute types or antitypes in the cross-classification of the Y observations constitute types or antitypes for the cross-classification of the Y with the X observations. Here, the distinction between situations in which, for a configuration of Y categories, all c X T candidate cells or only a subset of candidate cells constitute types or antitypes is not important. All we ask is whether types or antitypes emerge. If a configuration of Y and X categories constitutes a type or antitype under the second base model, the null hypothesis of no local relation between the Y and the X series of observations can be rejected. This suggests that a local relation exists that can be interpreted in the sense of Granger causation. In this article, we, therefore, focus on types and antitypes from the second base model.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

von Eye, A., Wiedermann, W. & Mun, EY. Granger Causality—Statistical Analysis Under a Configural Perspective. Integr. psych. behav. 48, 79–99 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-013-9243-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-013-9243-1

Keywords

Navigation