Abstract
Prior research suggests that naturism leads to less social physique anxiety and more positive body image, but that other forms of public nudity (e.g., casual stripping, sexting) may be harmful, particularly for women. Two cross-sectional studies built on those previous findings. Study 1 (N1 = 6670) found a positive relationship between generalised nude activity and body appreciation which was not moderated by gender. Study 2 (N2 = 331) found that both naturism and casual stripping predicted more body appreciation, a relationship mediated by less social physique anxiety. Again, these relationships were not moderated by gender. In contrast, sexting did not predict body appreciation and predicted more social physique anxiety, but only in men. These findings highlight that some types of nudity may be more beneficial or harmful than others, and that future research and policy should specify the type of nudity under consideration in order to maximise positive effects.
Introduction
In many societies there remains a certain level of stigma attached to participation in public nudity, including an assumption of perversion or sexual deviance (Booth, 1997; Monterrubio & Valencia, 2019; West & Geering, 2018). Prior research is unclear about the relationship between public nudity and psychological wellbeing. Some research seems to find a negative relationship between the two (e.g., Sherman & Hackathorn, 2020), while others find mixed results (e.g., Liong & Cheng, 2019) or an unambiguously positive relationship (West, 2018, 2021). A potential reason for these conflicting findings is the confounding of various types of public nudity, each of which might have divergent effects. This research aimed to shed some light on these conflicting findings. Specifically, this research made clear distinctions between types of public nudity, and investigated the relationships between these activities and two specific outcomes – social physique anxiety and body appreciation. In so doing, this research added nuance to prior findings on public nudity, going beyond simplified suggestions that nudity is either good or bad.
Types of Public Nudity
Naturism
The practice of nudism or naturism (de Vries, 2018) involves the behaviour of being naked in the company of others without necessarily expecting or requiring a sexual motivation (Carr-Gomm, 2012; Deschenes, 2016). Many other aspects of naturism are a matter of debate. For example, there are strong and divergent opinions about the role of sexuality in naturism (Smith, 2007; Smith & King, 2009; West & Geering, 2018). Early research focused on (though largely failed to find) negative effects of participation in naturism or naturist-like behaviours (Lewis & Janda, 1988; Okami et al., 1998). Indeed, some research found unexpected positive associations between naturism and body image (Negy & Winton, 2008; Story, 1979, 1984). More recently, researchers have deliberately investigated and found positive associations between naturism and body image. These studies used a variety of designs including correlational (West, 2018), before-and-after (West, 2018, 2020), and experimental designs (West, 2021). The results of these studies were reliably similar; naturism predicted, preceded, or caused increases in positive body image, an effect mediated by decreases in social physique anxiety (West, 2018, 2020, 2021). Though this is still a growing area, research to date indicates that naturism has positive effects.
Casual Stripping
Sherman and Hackathorn (2020) defined casual stripping as “stripping outside of the purview of being a professional paid dancer” (p. 1219), with the implication that it involved sexual intention or sexual enjoyment. In sharp contrast with findings concerning naturism, prior research seems to show that casual stripping is associated with negative psychological outcomes. For example, Sherman and Hackathorn found that casual stripping was associated with higher levels of sociosexuality and parental rejection. These findings are in general agreement with a more pervasive belief that casual stripping is bad (e.g., Forsyth, 1992, Roach, 2011). However, there are reasons to treat these findings with caution. First, this is the only quantitative study to date to investigate casual stripping, hence, replication is needed to confirm the reliability and generalisability of the findings. Second, the participant sample only included women, so it is unclear whether the effects would be similar for men. Lastly, though the study was intended to investigate casual stripping, the measure used items that could easily be interpreted as examples of naturism instead. These include [going] “skinny dipping” or [going] “skinny dipping with other people”. These activities do not necessarily involve any sexual activities or expectations. Therefore, it is worthwhile to build on the findings of Sherman and Hackathorn (2020) using a diverse sample and a more precise scale.
Sexting
Sexting has been defined as “sending sexually explicit or nude pictures of oneself through cell phones” (Liong & Cheng, 2019, p. 337). There is a reasonably large body of research on sexting. However, this research has not produced consistent results concerning its positive versus negative effects. For example, in a single study Klettke et al. (2019) found that sexting was associated with both increases (predictive of sexting) and decreases (following sexting) in body dissatisfaction. Other research found that sexting was associated with feeling more attractive, and with greater comfort with nudity, but also with higher levels of body surveillance, body shame, and body control beliefs (Liong & Cheng, 2019). Similarly, a large-scale survey (N = 9,235) of a gay dating website found that most men used nude photographs primarily to receive affirmation (Lemke & Merz, 2018). This suggests a complex relationship between sexting and body image in which it may, depending on context, be associated with either more positive or more negative body image.
Although research seems to suggest that naturism, stripping and sexting have different outcomes, to date no research has investigated these activities simultaneously. Furthermore, the variety of outcome measures used in prior research (e.g., positive body image, body dissatisfaction, body shame, body surveillance), making it difficult to compare the findings directly. Thus, this research focused on two aspects of body image – body appreciation and social physique anxiety – to investigate how these were affected by public nudity.
Body Image
Body Appreciation
Methods of increasing positive body image are of significant practical importance as positive body image predicts such aspects of well-being as self-esteem, optimism, proactive coping, safer sex intentions, sexual satisfaction, general health-related behaviours, and sexual health expectations (Avalos et al., 2005; Grower & Ward, 2018; Robbins & Reissing, 2018; Satinsky et al., 2013; West, 2018). A central variable in the understanding of positive body image is body appreciation (Tylka, 2011). It refers to several intersecting concepts including respect for one’s body, holding favourable opinions about and accepting one’s body, and resistance to externally promoted ideals of attractiveness (Avalos et al., 2005; Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015a). Body appreciation has also been found to predict gratitude (Homan & Tylka, 2018), physical activity independent of appearance related motivations (Homan & Tylka, 2014); intuitive eating (i.e., eating according to one’s physiological cues of hunger and satiation; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013); and overall life satisfaction (West, 2018, 2020). It is also negatively associated with indicators of ill-heath such as maladaptive perfectionism (Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012), body comparison (Homan & Tylka, 2018), restrained eating (Yao et al., 2021), and eating disorder symptomatology (Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013). Thus, body appreciation was the central variable of interest in this research as well.
Social Physique Anxiety
Social physique anxiety is the concern that one’s body is being judged or negatively evaluated by others (Ginis et al., 2003; Hausenblas et al., 2004). It predicts several negative outcomes such as disordered eating (Fitzsimmons-Craft et al., 2012) and lower levels of exercise (Mülazimoğlu-Balli et al., 2010). Prior research has also reliably found that physique anxiety predicts lower body appreciation (e.g., Alcaraz-Ibáñez et al., 2017; Jalalvand et al., 2012; Swami et al., 2014; West, 2021). It has also been shown that it can be reduced by brief interventions (Song et al., 2014), and has been shown to mediate the effects of nude activity on body appreciation (West, 2021). Thus, social physique anxiety was included as a potential mediator of the relationships between public nude activities and body appreciation in this research as well.
The Importance of Gender
Another question of interest was whether these findings differed for men and women. This was important for several reasons. Women (compared to men) face double standards and harsher social penalties for activities that appear sexual, and thus may consequently experience more negative emotional outcomes from nude activities than men do (Emmerink et al., 2016). Furthermore, some research has investigated the correlates of public nudity only for women (Sherman & Hackathorn, 2020). As mentioned earlier, there has been relatively little empirical research on nudity and body image, and even less has explicitly tested whether gender moderates the effects of nudity. However, the studies that have included gender as a predictor found somewhat reliable results. Men tended to have more positive body image than women did, but the associations between nudity and body image did not vary between gender (Liong & Cheng, 2019; Story, 1984; West, 2018). We thus included gender predictive variable in this research but did not expect to find that it moderated the relationship between public nudity and psychological outcomes.
Current Research
Two studies examined whether and how public nude activities were associated with body image and whether these were moderated by gender. This research was conducted in compliance with the ethical standards of the British Psychological Society and with the approval of the appropriate university ethics committee. Study 1 operationalised public nude activities with a simple ‘yes’ or ‘no’ answer to a question about having ever engaged in public nude activities and focused only on body appreciation. We hypothesised that generalised nude activity would predict higher body appreciation, but we did not expect this relationship to vary with gender.
Study 2 measured naturism, sexting, and casual stripping separately and used more nuanced measures of frequency of activity. This study also included both social physique anxiety and body appreciation as outcome measures. We further expected that more frequent engagement in naturism would be associated with higher body appreciation and that this relationship would be mediated by lower physique anxiety, but not moderated by gender. We remained agnostic about the relationships between casual stripping, sexting and the psychological outcomes.
Study 1
Methods
Participants and Recruitment
Using an Internet link advertised on the German television channel “Sat 1” participants were invited to complete a measure of their body image (i.e., a German translation of the BAS-2; (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015a) in exchange for information about their body image (i.e., their score on the BAS-2), information on how their body image scores compared to those of others, and what the correlates of body appreciation are. The link contained all necessary information and a full consent form for obtaining informed consent. Participants were informed that the research was investigating body image, but not aware that the research was investigating the link between nude activity and body image. No monetary incentive was offered for participation.
Between December 6, 2019, and April 14, 2020, 6670 German adults completed the survey; 1609 men (24.1%), 4992 women (74.8%), and 69 who identified as “other” or “prefer not to say” (1.0%). Participants’ mean age = 34.10, standard deviation = 11.77. Most participants were White (6452, 96.7%). Smaller proportions of the sample were South Asian (11, 0.2%), East Asian (6, 0.1%), Black (27, 0.4%) or “other” (174, 2.6%). Most participants (80%) completed the survey in 6 min or less. At the end of the survey all participants were thanked and fully debriefed.
Measures
The 10-item BAS-2 was translated into German by a team of 3 bilingual native German speakers and then back-translated by another bilingual English and German speaker to ensure that the original meaning was retained. All items asked participants to reflect on their responses to their bodies, e.g., “I respect my body”. The full list of items can be seen in the Appendix. Participants responded to all items on a 5-point scale (1 = Never, 2 = Seldom, 3 = Sometimes, 4 = Often, 5 = Always). In a subsequent demographics section, participants also indicated their age in years, their gender (male, female, other/prefer not to say), their ethnicity (White, Black, East Asian, South Asian, Other), and whether they had ever taken part in any public clothes-free activities (no vs. yes).
Results and Discussion
The German BAS-2
Skewness (-0.46 to 0.20) and kurtosis (−1.00 to −0.60) values were within critical limits (Kline, 2005); hence, neither the BAS-2 scale nor any item therein was transformed. Cronbach’s alphas were calculated separately for all three gender groups and were within acceptable limits in each case (α men = 0.95; α women = 0.95; α other = 0.96). Principal components analysis based on Eigenvalues greater than 1 revealed that the scale had a single component for all 3 gender groups, and that all items loaded well on that component for men (0.65 < λ < 0.90), women (0.66 < λ < 0.91), and other participants (0.73 < λ < 0.94). The mean score on the BAS-2 in this sample was 3.03 with a standard deviation of 0.97.
The Link Between Public Nude Activity and Body Image
Of all participants, 45.6% had taken part in some nude activity. This proportion was higher among men (62.6%) than among women (40.2%) or other participants (40.6%); X2 (2) = 246.41, p < 0.001. Table 1 displays the means and standard errors of body appreciation scores according to gender and participation in nude activity. To investigate whether participation in nude activity predicted higher body appreciation scores, and whether this relationship was moderated by gender, we used a 2 (Nude activity: No vs. Yes) × 3 (Gender: Male vs. Female vs. Other) between-participants analysis of variance with body appreciation scores as the dependent variable and participant age and ethnicity (White vs. Ethnic Minority) as covariates. Bonferroni-adjusted comparisons were used to further investigate interactions and in the case of comparisons between multiple groups.
Similar to prior research (Demarest & Allen, 2000; Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015a), we found a main effect of gender on body image, F (2, 6662) = 200.03, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.06. Men reported higher levels of body appreciation than did women (p < 0.001) or other participants (p < 0.001), though the latter 2 groups did not differ significantly from each other (p = 0.63). There was also a significant effect of age on body appreciation F (1, 6662) = 26.21, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.004. Older participants reported more body appreciation (r = 0.03, p = 0.015). There was, however, no significant effect of ethnicity on body appreciation F (1, 6662) = 1.11, p = 0.29, ηp2 < 0.001.
Most relevant for this study, there was also a significant effect of nude activity on body image, F (1, 6662) = 49.75, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.007. Participants who had taken part in nude activity reported higher body appreciation scores than those who had not. Furthermore, there was no significant interaction between gender and nude activity, F (2, 6662) = 1.01, p = 0.37, ηp2 < 0.001. Thus, the positive association between nude activity and body appreciation did not differ between genders (see Fig. 1).
Body Appreciation by Gender and Participation in Nude Activities Notes: (1) Participation in nude activity predicted higher body appreciation scores regardless of gender. (2) Scores ranged from 1 to 5. (3) Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. (4) Participants were 6670 German adults; 1609 men (24.1%), 4992 women (74.8%), and 69 who identified as “other” or “prefer not to say” (1.0%)
Study 2
Study 1 found that nude activity predicted positive body image and that this relationship was not moderated by gender. Study 2 looked at three types of nudity separately – naturism, sexting, and casual stripping – to investigate which ones predicted positive body image, whether the relationships were positive or negative, and which of these relationships were mediated by changes in social physique anxiety. As in Study 1, we also investigated whether these relationships were moderated by gender.
Methods
Participants and Recruitment
Using an Internet link advertised on social media channels (Facebook, Instagram) participants were invited to complete a short survey about their activities and body image in exchange for information about their body image (i.e., their score on the BAS-2), information on how their body image scores compared to those of others, and what the correlates of body appreciation are. The link contained all necessary information and a full consent form for obtaining informed consent. Participants were informed that the research was investigating body image, but not aware that the research was investigating the link between nude activity and body image. No monetary incentive was offered for participation.
Between March 14, 2021, and September 12, 2021, 376 adults completed the survey; 318 men (84.6%), 49 women (13%), and 9 who identified as “other” or “prefer not to say” (2.4%). Participants’ mean age = 46.95, standard deviation = 16.00. Most participants were White (349, 92.8%). Smaller proportions of the sample were South Asian (8, 2.1%), East Asian (2, 0.5%), Black (1, 0.3%) or “other” (16, 4.3%). Participants were also from internationally diverse backgrounds. The largest proportion of participants were British (164, 43.6%) followed by Polish (71, 18.9%), American (60, 16%) and numerous other, mostly European nationalities (81, 21.5%). At the end of the survey all participants were thanked and fully debriefed.
Measures
As in Study 1, in a demographics section, participants indicated their age in years, their gender (male, female, other/prefer not to say), their ethnicity (White, Black, East Asian, South Asian, Other), and their nationality. Frequency of participation in all nude activities was measured using the same 6-point scale with which participants indicated how often they took part in a specified activity (1 = Never, 2 = Less than Once a Month, 3 = Once a Month, 4 = Once Every 2 Weeks, 5 = Once a Week, 6 = More than Once a Week).
Naturism was measured with a 7-item scale (α = 0.85) based on Smith and King (2009). Example items included: “Going to commercial naturist environments (naturist clubs, spas, swims, or leisure centres)”, “Going nude at the beach (official or unofficial naturist beaches)” or “Going nude in your private property (e.g., nude gardening or sunbathing)”. The full list of items can be seen in the Appendix I.
Casual stripping was measured with a revised, 8-item scale (α = 0.74) from Sherman and Hackathorn (2020). Example items included: “Taking off your clothes (e.g., your shirt) at a party”, "Flashing" a body part (e.g., shown breasts) for attention”, “"Strip dancing" for a romantic partner”. The full list of items can be seen in the Appendix II.
Sexting activity was measured with a 3-item scale (α = 0.79) developed for this study. Participants responded how often they did each of the following: “Sending nude, sexual photos of yourself to someone else”, “Sending nude, sexual videos of yourself to someone else”, “Posting nude, sexually explicit images of yourself online”.
Social physique anxiety was measured with the 12-item (α = 0.84) Social Physique Anxiety Scale (Leary, 2013; West, 2020). Participants responded on a 5-point scale (1 = Not at all characteristic of me, 5 = Extremely characteristic of me). Example items include “In the presence of others, I feel apprehensive about my physique or figure.” and “I usually feel relaxed when it's obvious that others are looking at my physique or figure.” (reversed). As in Study 1, body appreciation was measured using the 10-item BAS-2 (α = 0.93).
Results and Discussion
Descriptive statistics and correlations between variables can be seen in Table 2. As can be seen in Table 2, both gender and age were associated with social physique anxiety and body appreciation, though ethnicity was not. As in prior research men (M = 2.55, SD = 0.76), reported less social physique anxiety than did women (M = 3.17, SD = 0.90), p < 0.001, but did not differ significantly from “other” participants (M = 2.73, SD = 0.89), p = 1.00. Women also did not differ significantly from “other” participants, p = 0.26. Men also reported higher levels of body appreciation (M = 3.89, SD = 0.74), than did women (M = 3.47, SD = 0.91), p = 0.001, but did not differ significantly from “other” participants (M = 3.99, SD = 0.78), p = 1.00. Women also did not differ significantly from “other” participants, p = 0.19. Older participants reported less social physique anxiety and higher levels of body appreciation.
We also checked the proportion of participants who had engaged in some sort of public nudity at least once (i.e., those who did not respond “never” to all the items of these scales). As seen in Table 3, most participants had engaged in some form of public nudity; 88.3% had engaged in naturism, 61.7% had engaged in casual stripping and 56.6% engaged in sexting at least once. Interestingly, men were more likely to have tried naturism, while women were more likely to have tried casual stripping and sexting.
Associations Between Nudity, Social Physique Anxiety, and Body Image
Multiple regression analyses found that naturism predicted less social physique anxiety (ß = -0.15, p = 0.001), as did casual stripping (ß = -0.17, p = 0.02). In contrast, sexting predicted more social physique anxiety (ß = 0.08, p = 0.03). Age (ß = -0.01, p = 0.004) and gender (ß = 0.28, p = 0.004) were also significant predictors.
Multiple regression analyses also found that naturism predicted higher levels of body appreciation (ß = 0.17, p < 0.001), though neither casual stripping (ß = 0.15, p = 0.16) nor sexting (ß = -0.02, p = 0.56) predicted body appreciation. Neither age (ß = 0.01, p = 0.06) nor gender (ß = -0.15, p = 0.16) was a significant predictor of body appreciation in these analyses.
Mediation by Social Physique Anxiety
We also tested a mediation model in which naturist activity predicted higher levels of body appreciation via lower levels of social physique anxiety using PROCESS Macros, Model 4, with 5000 bootstrap samples, 95% confidence intervals, the following variables in the mediation analysis; X = Naturism, M = Social Physique Anxiety, Y = Body Appreciation, and the following variables as covariates; casual stripping, sexting, age, and gender. We found the hypothesised mediated relationship between naturism and body image via social physique anxiety. Naturism directly predicted less social physique anxiety (b = -0.13, p < 0.001), and more body appreciation (b = 0.07, p = 0.02). Social physique anxiety also directly predicted less body appreciation (b = -0.77, p < 0.001). Finally, naturism also indirectly predicted more body appreciation via less social physique anxiety, b = 0.10, 95% C.I. = 0.05, 0.16.
Using the same analyses (PROCESS Macros Model 4, but with casual stripping as the predictor and both naturism and sexting as covariates) we found a similar mediated relationship between casual stripping and body image via social physique anxiety. Casual stripping directly predicted less social physique anxiety (b = -0.17, p = 0.02), but not body appreciation (b = -0.02, p = 0.75). Social physique anxiety also directly predicted less body appreciation (b = -0.77, p < 0.001). Finally, casual stripping also indirectly predicted more body appreciation via less social physique anxiety, b = 0.13, 95% C.I. = 0.01, 0.26.
However, these analyses (PROCESS Macros Model 4, but with sexting as the predictor and naturism and casual stripping as covariates) did not find a mediated relationship between sexting and body image via social physique anxiety. Sexting directly predicted more social physique anxiety (b = 0.08, p = 0.03), but not body appreciation (b = 0.04, p = 0.18). As before, social physique anxiety directly predicted body appreciation (b = -0.77, p < 0.001), but sexting did not indirectly predict body appreciation via social physique anxiety, b = -0.06, 95% C.I. = -0.13, 0.003.
Moderation by Gender
We tested whether gender moderated the previously established mediated relationships between naturism and body appreciation and between casual stripping and body appreciation. First, we used PROCESS Macros, Model 8, with 5000 bootstrap samples, 95% confidence intervals, the following variables in the moderated mediation analysis; X = Naturism, M = Social Physique Anxiety, Y = Body Appreciation, W = Gender, and the following variables as covariates; casual stripping, sexting, and age. These analyses again revealed a mediated relationship between naturism and body appreciation via reduced social physique anxiety but, importantly, gender did not moderate either the association between naturism and social physique anxiety (b = -0.18, p = 0.05) or the association between naturism and body appreciation (b = -0.08, p = 0.29).
We repeated the analyses using casual stripping as the predictor and naturism as one of the covariates. Similar to the previous analyses, we once again found the mediated relationship between stripping and body appreciation via reduced social physique anxiety but, importantly, gender did not moderate either the association between stripping and social physique anxiety (b = -0.30, p = 0.10) or the association between stripping and body appreciation (b = -0.05, p = 0.69).
Concerning sexting, as the previous mediation analyses did not find a mediated relationship between sexting and body appreciation, we instead tested a simpler moderated model in which sexting predicted social physique anxiety and this relationship was moderated by gender (PROCESS Macros, Model 1, with 5000 bootstrap samples, 95% confidence intervals, the following variables in the moderated mediation analysis; X = Sexting, Y = Social Physique Anxiety, W = Gender, and the following variables as covariates; casual stripping, naturism, and age). This analysis revealed a positive association between sexting and social physique anxiety (b = 0.35, p < 0.001) as well as a significant moderation by gender (b = -0.21, p = 0.02). Sexting predicted more social physique anxiety for men (b = 0.14, p < 0.001), but not for women (b = -0.07, p = 0.40).
General Discussion
These two studies replicated, clarified, and built on previous findings concerning nudity and psychological outcomes. Using a large, age-diverse and gender-diverse sample, Study 1 found that participation in public nudity generally predicted more positive body image (i.e., higher levels of body appreciation). Furthermore, the lack of moderation by gender, despite the substantial sample size in that study, is a strong indicator that this moderation may be very small or genuinely absent. The positive associations between public nudity and body image appear to apply for those who identify as men, women, or neither.
Study 2 extended these findings by investigating three specific types of public nudity simultaneously: naturism, casual stripping, and sexting. In line with prior research (West, 2021) naturism predicted more body appreciation, and that this relationship was mediated by lower levels of social physique anxiety. Seemingly in contrast with prior research (Sherman & Hackathorn, 2020), casual stripping was also associated with positive outcomes: less social physique anxiety and (indirectly) more body appreciation. As in Study 1, these positive associations were not moderated by gender. Only sexting was associated with negative outcomes and this was the only association moderated by gender: sexting predicted more social physique anxiety in men (but not in women) and had no relationship with body appreciation. Below, we discuss these findings considering their implications, strengths and limitations, and suggestions for future research.
Implications
The positive effects of naturism on body image have been already documented in previous research (e.g., West, 2018, 2020, 2021). Nonetheless, these findings add to the generalisability of the relationship between naturism and body image. In particular, Study 1 used a very large sample of German participants, and Study 2 used a sample of mixed European participants. The consistency between these findings and prior research can increase our confidence about the positive effects of naturism, and the generalisability of these effects beyond a limited range of English-speaking countries.
Perhaps more surprising, these findings appear to contradict previous research which found that casual stripping was associated with negative (not positive) psychological factors (i.e., Sherman & Hackathorn, 2020). However, we can propose some potential reasons for these divergent findings. First, as mentioned before, Sherman and Hackathorn’s measure of casual stripping included items that could have applied to other forms of public nudity, making it unclear exactly what the scale was measuring. Second, Sherman and Hackathorn investigated different variables. Specifically, they found (1) associations between casual stripping and paternal neglect (which seemed to be a precursor and not consequence of casual stripping) and (2) associations between casual stripping and sociosexuality, a variable that can only be interpreted as negative if one holds a priori beliefs about the value of a woman’s modesty and sexual selectivity. It seems quite reasonable to interpret their findings differently, in line with prior research (e.g., Lewis & Janda, 1988) that found positive associations between nudity and comfort with one’s sexuality. In that light, it is also understandable that casual stripping may be similarly associated with lower levels of social physique anxiety and higher levels of body image in both men and women. In any case, research on stripping and psychological outcomes remains scarce and somewhat unclear, with some finding associations even between professional stripping and positive psychological outcomes (see, e.g., Sweet & Tewksbury, 2000; Wood, 2000). Thus, future research could be useful to clarify what the positive and negative effects of stripping may be.
Only one finding indicated associations between some form of public nudity and negative psychological outcomes (i.e., the association between sexting and social physique anxiety) and this finding only applied to men. While these findings do not go beyond prior research showing that sexting was sometimes associated with negative outcomes (Liong & Cheng, 2019), they do add meaningfully to these current findings in two ways. First, they clarify that not all forms of public nudity have similar associations with psychological outcomes; while naturism and casual stripping were associated with positive outcomes, sexting was associated with negative outcomes. Second, they undermine the simplistic interpretation of our findings that high levels of body image and low levels of anxiety about one’s body are necessarily the causes of more involvement in public nudity. At the very least, lower levels of anxiety about one’s body is associated with less frequent sexting (though more frequent naturism and casual stripping) suggesting that a more complex relationship exists between nudity and body image.
Finally, though this was not a central aspect of the study, it is noteworthy that a large proportion of the participants in both studies had engaged in some communal nude activity (Study 1 – 46%, Study 2 – 88% naturism, 62% casual stripping, 57% sexting). This suggests that willingness to take part in nude activity may be more widespread than it is often assumed to be.
Limitations and Future Research
These studies benefit from a number of strengths including theoretical replication across two studies, a very large sample size in Study 1, participants of varied nationalities, and participants who were more diverse than the widely overused student samples common in social psychology (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010). Practically, this research also provides a valuable tool for assessing body image in German-speaking countries by using the translation of the most recent version of the BAS-2, which has several advantages over the previous versions of the Body Appreciation Scale (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015a).
However, this research also has limitations. As both studies are cross-sectional, it is not possible to infer causal effects. Thus, a potential criticism is that this research merely shows that people who like their bodies are more likely participate in nudity. While it is plausible that a causal relationship exists in that direction, neither these current data nor prior research support the simplistic interpretation that this is the only causal relationship. As mentioned, this interpretation is incompatible with the finding that less anxiety about one’s body is associated with more of some forms of public nudity (naturism and casual stripping), but less of other forms (sexting). Furthermore, prior experimental research has shown a causal effect of public nudity on social physique anxiety and body image (West, 2021).
While the participants were diverse in some ways, most participants identified as White, and sexual orientation and cisgender or transgender status was not explicitly investigated. Also, despite the large samples, some amount of self-selection likely occurred, meaning that only participants were likely already interested in body image. Similar limitations found in other research on nudity and body image (West, 2018, 2020, 2021). Nonetheless, future research should specifically investigate whether the effects of public and communal nudity differ for participants who are not White, cisgender, and heterosexual.
We note that the effect size of the relationship between nude activity and body image in Study 1 was quite small. However, this could be due to the use of a relatively crude all-or-nothing measure of participation in nude activity; participants merely indicated (“yes” vs. “no”) whether they had ever taken part in such activity. Effect sizes were notably larger in Study 2, in which we used more nuanced measures. This further suggests that even more nuanced studies that include measures of how frequently, recently, or under what conditions participants took part in such activities may yield yet larger effect sizes, as well as more information about the ways to maximise the link between nudity and body appreciation.
Finally, though this research investigated specific, positive relationships between nude activities and body appreciation, future research should also continue to investigate the possible relationships between nude activity and negative outcomes. Several potential outcomes were not included in this research, such as body shame, body surveillance, overall life satisfaction, or even self-esteem. Including such outcomes may provide a more rounded picture of the positive and negative effects of all these activities.
Conclusions
Though this area of research is still in its early stages, this study adds meaningfully to a slowly expanding body of work indicating a link between public nude activities and positive body image. While the nature of the link is far from conclusive, initial work suggests that both naturism and casual stripping are largely positive, while sexting appeared to be more negative. Resources invested into future research, particularly longitudinal and experimental research, may yield important insights concerning whether, when, why, and what types of nude activity improve body image and other psychological outcomes.
Appendix 1
Items for the German Translation of the BAS-2
Instructions: Bitte geben Sie nun an, ob die Aussagen unten niemals, selten, manchmal, oft oder immer auf Sie zutreffen.
-
1.
Ich respektiere meinen Körper.
-
2.
Ich bin mit meinem Körper im Reinen.
-
3.
Ich finde, dass mein Körper zumindest einige gute Eigenschaften hat.
-
4.
Ich habe eine positive Einstellung gegenüber meinem Körper.
-
5.
Ich achte auf die Bedürfnisse meines Körpers.
-
6.
Ich liebe meinen Körper.
-
7.
Ich weiß die unterschiedlichen und einzigartigen Eigenschaften meines Körpers zu schätzen.
-
8.
Ich verhalte mich wie jemand, der eine positive Einstellung gegenüber seines Körpers hat, zum Beispiel gehe ich mit erhobenem Haupt und lächele.
-
9.
Ich fühle mich wohl in meinem Körper.
-
10.
Ich fühle mich schön, auch wenn ich nicht dem Schönheitsideal der Medien entspreche (z.B. Models, SchauspielerInnen…).
Appendix 2
Items from the Revised, 8-Item Casual Stripping Scale
Instructions:
"Casual stripping" can be defined as removing one's clothes to allow others to see and enjoy one’s nudity in a sexual way, but not for the purpose of making money or fulfilling work obligations (e.g., as a nude dancer). This can occur in a variety of contexts including stripping for the sole pleasure of one's partner or in a group or party setting.
Please indicate how frequently (on average) you engage in these forms of "casual stripping".*
-
1.
Taking off your clothes (e.g., your shirt) at a party.
-
2.
Taking off your clothes (e.g., your shirt) for money.
-
3.
"Flashing" a body part (e.g., shown breasts) for attention.
-
4.
"Flashing" a body part (e.g., shown breasts) as a joke.
-
5.
"Flashing" a body part (e.g., shown breasts) to get what you want.
-
6.
"Flashing" a body part (e.g., shown breasts) to fit in.
-
7.
"Strip dancing" for a romantic partner.
-
8.
"Strip dancing" for two or more people at the same time.
References
Al Sabbah, H., Vereecken, C. A., Elgar, F. J., Nansel, T., Aasvee, K., Abdeen, Z., Ojala, K., Ahluwalia, N., & Maes, L. (2009). Body weight dissatisfaction and communication with parents among adolescents in 24 countries: International cross-sectional survey. BMC Public Health, 9, 52–62. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-9-52
Alcaraz-Ibanez, M., Sicilia, A., & Burgueno, R. (2017). Social physique anxiety, mental health, and exercise: Analyzing the role of basic psychological needs and psychological inflexibility. The Spanish Journal of Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2017.13
Avalos, L. C., Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. (2005). The Body Appreciation Scale: Development and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 2(3), 285–297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.06.002
Barcan, R. (2015). Nudism. In P. Whelehan & A. Bolin (Eds.), The international encyclopedia of human sexuality (pp. 1–3). Wiley-Blackwell.
Barton, B. (2017). Stripped: Inside the lives of exotic dancers. NYU Press.
Bernard, C., Degabrielle, C., Cartier, L., Phill, C., Sherwood, J., & Tyree, T. (2003). Exotic dancers: Gender differences in societal reaction, subcultural ties, and conventional support. 10(1), 1–11.
Bianchi, D., Morelli, M., Baiocco, R., & Chirumbolo, A. (2016). Psychometric properties of the Sexting Motivations Questionnaire for adolescents and young adults. Rassegna Di Psicologia, 33(3), 5–18.
Bianchi, D., Morelli, M., Baiocco, R., & Chirumbolo, A. (2017). Sexting as the mirror on the wall: Body-esteem attribution, media models, and objectified-body consciousness. Journal of Adolescence, 61, 164–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.10.006
Boden, D. M. (2007). Alienation of Sexuality in Male Erotic Dancing. Journal of Homosexuality, 53(1–2), 129–152. https://doi.org/10.1300/J082v53n01_06
Booth, D. (1997). Nudes in the sand and perverts in the dunes. Journal of Australian Studies, 21(53), 170–182. https://doi.org/10.1080/14443059709387326
Brannan, M. E., & Petrie, T. A. (2011). Psychological well-being and the body dissatisfaction-bulimic symptomatology relationship: An examination of moderators. Eating Behaviors, 12(4), 233–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2011.06.002
Carr-Gomm, P. (2012). A brief history of nakedness. Reaktion Books.
Currin, J. M., & Hubach, R. D. (2019). Motivations for Nonuniversity-Based Adults Who Sext Their Relationship Partners. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 45(4), 317–327. https://doi.org/10.1080/0092623X.2018.1526837
de Vries, B. (2018). The Right to be Publicly Naked: A Defence of Nudism. Res Publica, September 2018, 407–424. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11158-018-09406-z
DeMarco, J. R. G. (2007). Power and Control in Gay Strip Clubs. Journal of Homosexuality, 53(1–2), 111–127. https://doi.org/10.1300/J082v53n01_05
Demarest, J., & Allen, R. (2000). Body image: Gender, ethnic, and age differences. Journal of Social Psychology, 140(4), 465–472. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224540009600485
Deschenes, S. (2016). The official INF-FNI definition of naturism.
Döring, N. (2014). Consensual sexting among adolescents: Risk preven- tion through abstinence education or safer sexting? Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 8(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.5817/CP2014-1-9.
Drouin, M., Ross, J., & Tobin, E. (2015). Sexting: A new, digital vehicle for intimate partner aggression? Computers in Human Behavior, 50, 197–204.
Emmerink, P. M., van den Eijnden, R. J., Vanwesenbeeck, I., & ter Bogt, T. F. (2016). The relationship between endorsement of the sexual double standard and sexual cognitions and emotions. Sex Roles, 75(7–8), 363–376.
Fernández-Bustos, J.-G., González-Martí, I., Contreras, O., & Cuevas, R. (2015). Relationship between body image and physical self-concept in adolescent females. Revista Latinoamericana De Psicología, 47(1), 25–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0120-0534(15)30003-0
Fitzsimmons-Craft, E. E., Harney, M. B., Brownstone, L. M., Higgins, M. K., & Bardone-Cone, A. M. (2012). Examining social physique anxiety and disordered eating in college women. The roles of social comparison and body surveillance. Appetite, 59(3), 796–805
Ginis, K. A. M., Jung, M. E., & Gauvin, L. (2003). To see or not to see: Effects of exercising in mirrored environments on sedentary women’s feeling states and self-efficacy. Health Psychology, 22(4), 354–361. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.22.4.354
Graham Holmes, L., Nilssen, A. R., Cann, D., & Strassberg, D. S. (2021). A sex-positive mixed methods approach to sexting experiences among college students. Computers in Human Behavior, 115, 106619. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106619
Grower, P., & Ward, L. M. (2018). Examining the unique contribution of body appreciation to heterosexual women’s sexual agency. Body Image, 27, 138–147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.09.003
Hausenblas, H. A., Brewer, B. W., & Van Raalte, J. L. (2004). Self-presentation and exercise. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16(1), 3–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/10413200490260026
Homan, K. J., & Tylka, T. L. (2014). Appearance-based exercise motivation moderates the relationship between exercise frequency and positive body image. Body Image, 11, 101–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.01.003
Homan, K. J., & Tylka, T. L. (2018). Development and exploration of the gratitude model of body appreciation in women. Body Image, 25, 14–22.
Howard, D., Klettke, B., Ling, M., Krug, I., & Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M. (2019). Does body dissatisfaction influence sexting behaviors in daily life? Computers in Human Behavior, 101, 320–326.
Jalalvand, S. H., Yoosefy, B., & Farahani, A. (2012). Study of relationship between physical activity and self-esteem, body appreciation and social physique anxiety among high school male and female students.
Iannantuono, A. C., & Tylka, T. L. (2012). Interpersonal and intrapersonal links to body appreciation in college women: An exploratory model. Body Image, 9(2), 227–235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.01.004
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd Ed.). Guilford Press.
Kostanski, M., & Gullone, E. (1998). Adolescent body image dissatisfaction: Relationships with self-esteem, anxiety, and depression controlling for body mass. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 39(2), 255–262. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00319
Leary, M. R. (2013). Social Physique Anxiety Scale (SPAS). Measurement Instrument Database for the Social Sciences.
Lemke, R., & Merz, S. (2018). The prevalence and gratification of nude self-presentation of men who have sex with men in online-dating environments: Attracting attention, empowerment, and self-verification. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 21(1), 16–24.
Lenhart, A. (2009). Teens and sexting (Vol. 15). Pew Internet & American Life Project.
Lewis, R. J., & Janda, L. H. (1988). The relationship between adult sexual adjustment and childhood experiences regarding exposure to nudity, sleeping in the parental bed, and parental attitudes toward sexuality. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 17(4), 349–362.
Liong, M., & Cheng, G. H. L. (2019). Objectifying or Liberating? Investigation of the Effects of Sexting on Body Image. Journal of Sex Research, 56(3), 337–344. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2018.1438576
Longobardi, C., Fabris, M. A., Prino, L. E., & Settanni, M. (2021). The role of body image concerns in online sexual victimization among female adolescents: The mediating effect of risky online behaviors. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma, 14(1), 51–60.
Mestemacher, R. A., & Roberti, J. W. (2004). qualitative analysis of vocational choice: A collective case study of strippers. Deviant Behavior, 25(1), 43–65. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639620490248934
Monterrubio, C. (2019). Exploring nudist festival attendance at a Mexican beach: A tourist motivation approach. Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 36(5), 583–594. https://doi.org/10.1080/10548408.2019.1604295
Monterrubio, C., & Valencia, L. (2019). Negotiating stigmatisation of deviant behaviour: An exploration of locals’ perceptions of nude tourists. Journal of Tourism and Cultural Change, 17(6), 689–709. https://doi.org/10.1080/14766825.2018.1511723
Morelli, M., Bianchi, D., & Baiocco, R. (2016). Sexting, psychological distress and dating violence among adolescents and young adults. Psicothema, 28(2), 137–142. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2015.193
Mülazimoğlu-Balli, Ö., Koca, C., & Aşçi, F. (2010). An examination of social physique anxiety with regard to sex and level of sport involvement. Journal of Human Kinetics, 26(2010), 115–122.
Negy, C., & Winton, S. (2008). A comparison of pro- and anti-nudity college students on acceptance of self and of culturally diverse others. Journal of Sex Research, 45(3), 287–294. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490802204464
Okami, P., Olmstead, R., Abramson, P. R., & Pendleton, L. (1998). Early childhood exposure to parental nudity and scenes of parental sexuality ('primal scenes’): An 18-year longitudinal study of outcome. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 27(4), 361–384.
Parker, T. S., Blackburn, K. M., Perry, M. S., & Hawks, J. M. (2013). Sexting as an intervention: Relationship satisfaction and motivation considerations. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 41(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/01926187.2011.635134
Pedersen, C. L., Champion, A. R., Hesse, C. L., & Lewis, B. J. (2015). A Question of Deviancy: Comparing Exotic Dancers and Female University Students. Sexuality & Culture, 19(4), 800–815. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12119-015-9292-3
Roach, C. M. (2011). Stripping, sex, and popular culture. Oxford Publishing.
Robbins, A. R., & Reissing, E. D. (2018). Appearance dissatisfaction, body appreciation, and sexual health in women across adulthood. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47(3), 703–714. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-017-0982-9
Satinsky, S., Dennis, B., Reece, M., Sanders, S., & Bardzell, S. (2013). My ‘Fat Girl Complex’: A preliminary investigation of sexual health and body image in women of size. Culture, Health and Sexuality, 15(6), 710–725. https://doi.org/10.1080/13691058.2013.783236
Schweitzer, D. (2000). Striptease: The Art of Spectacle and Transgression. The Journal of Popular Culture, 34(1), 65–75. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3840.2000.3401_65.x
Scull, M. T. (2015). The Self-Concept as a Side Bet: How Stripping Enhances the Self-Views of Men who Dance for Women. Deviant Behavior, 36(11), 890–909. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639625.2014.977197
Sherman, M., & Hackathorn, J. (2020). Keeping It Casual: Stripping Behaviors in Non-sex Worker Populations. Sexuality & Culture, 24(5), 1218–1231. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12119-020-09704-w
Smith, G. (2007). Nudity can be erotic and naturists should not have to deny it. The Guardian, 14, 1–5.
Smith, G., & King, M. (2009). Naturism and sexuality: Broadening our approach to sexual wellbeing. Health and Place, 15(2), 439–446. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2008.08.002
Song, H., Kim, J., & Lee, K. M. (2014). Virtual vs. real body in exergames: Reducing social physique anxiety in exercise experiences. Computers in Human Behavior, 36, 282–285.
Story, M. D. (1979). Factors associated with more positive body self-concepts in preschool children. The Journal of Social Psychology, 108(1), 49–56. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1979.9711960
Story, M. D. (1984). Comparisons of body self-concept between social nudists and nonnudists. The Journal of Psychology: Interdiciplinary and Applied, 118(1), 99–112.
Swami, V., Frederick, D. A., Aavik, T., Alcalay, L., Allik, J., Anderson, D., & Zivcic-Becirevic, I. (2010). The attractive female body weight and female body dissatisfaction in 26 countries across 10 world regions: Results of the International Body Project I. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(3), 309–25. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167209359702
Swami, V., Miah, J., Noorani, N., & Taylor, D. (2014). Is the hijab protective? An investigation of body image and related constructs among British Muslim women. British Journal of Psychology, 105(3), 352–363.
Swami, V., Stieger, S., Haubner, T., & Voracek, M. (2008). German translation and psychometric evaluation of the Body Appreciation Scale. Body Image, 5(1), 122–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.10.002
Sweet, N., & Tewksbury, R. (2000). Entry, maintenance and departure from a career in the sex industry: Strippers’ experiences of occupational costs and rewards. 24(2), 136–161.
Temple, J. R., & Choi, H. (2014). Longitudinal association between teen sexting and sexual behavior. Pediatrics, 134(5), e1287–e1292.
Tylka, T. L. (2011). Positive psychology perspectives on body image. In T. F. Cash & L. Smolak (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of science, practice, and prevention (pp. 56–64). Guilford Press.
Tylka, T. L., & Kroon Van Diest, A. M. (2013). The Intuitive Eating Scale-2: Item refinement and psychometric evaluation with college women and men. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 60(1), 137–153. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030893
Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. L. (2015a). The Body Appreciation Scale-2: Item refinement and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 12(1), 53–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.09.006
Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. L. (2015b). What is and what is not positive body image? Conceptual foundations and construct definition. Body Image, 14, 118–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.04.001
Van Ouytsel, J., Lu, Y., Ponnet, K., Walrave, M., & Temple, J. R. (2019a). Longitudinal associations between sexting, cyberbullying, and bullying among adolescents: Cross-lagged panel analysis. Journal of Adolescence, 73, 36–41.
Van Ouytsel, J., Van Gool, E., Walrave, M., Ponnet, K., & Peeters, E. (2017). Sexting: Adolescents’ perceptions of the applications used for, motives for, and consequences of sexting. Journal of Youth Studies, 20(4), 446–470.
Van Ouytsel, J., Walrave, M., & Ponnet, K. (2019b). Sexting within adolescents’ romantic relationships: How is it related to perceptions of love and verbal conflict? Computers in Human Behavior, 97, 216–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.03.029
Vartanian, L. R. (2009). When the Body Defines the Self: Self-Concept Clarity, Internalization, and Body Image. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28(1), 94–126. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2009.28.1.94
Walker, S., Sanci, L., & Temple-Smith, M. (2013). Sexting: Young women’s and men’s views on its nature and origins. Journal of Adolescent Health, 52(6), 697–701.
West, K. (2018). Naked and unashamed: Investigations and applications of the effects of naturist activities on body image, self-esteem, and life satisfaction. J o Happiness Studies, 19(3), 677–697. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-017-9846-1
West, K. (2020). A nudity-based intervention to improve body image, self-esteem, and life satisfaction. International Journal of Happiness and Development.
West, K. (2021). I Feel Better Naked: Communal Naked Activity Increases Body Appreciation by Reducing Social Physique Anxiety. Journal of Sex Research, 00(00), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2020.1764470
West, K., & Geering, P. (2018). The Benefits of Nakedness. BBC World Service. https://www.bbc.co.uk/programmes/w3csxh9l
Wood, E. A. (2000). Working in the Fantasy Factory: The Attention Hypothesis and the Enacting of Masculine Power in Strip Clubs. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 29(1), 5–31. https://doi.org/10.1177/089124100129023800
Yao, L., Niu, G. & Sun, X. Body Image Comparisons on Social Networking Sites and Chinese Female College Students’ Restrained Eating: The Roles of Body Shame, Body Appreciation, and Body Mass Index. Sex Roles 84, 465–476 (2021). https://doi-org.gold.idm.oclc.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-020-01179-1
Acknowledgements
There are no acknowledgements.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
There are no conflicts of interest in this research.
Data Availability
These data are available upon request and will be sent (containing no personal or identifiable information) by the corresponding author if requested.
Ethical Approval Statement
This study was approved by the Psychology Department’s Research Ethics Committee at Goldsmiths, University of London.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
West, K., Kukawska, E. Good Nudes and Bad Nudes: How Naturism, Casual Stripping, and Sexting Predict Social Physique Anxiety and Body Appreciation. Sexuality & Culture 26, 2151–2170 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12119-022-09990-6
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12119-022-09990-6
Keywords
- Public nudity
- Naturism
- Sexting
- Casual stripping
- Body image
- Body appreciation