Abstract
The amygdala is a hub in brain networks that supports social life and fear processing. Compared with other apes, humans have a relatively larger lateral nucleus of the amygdala, which is consistent with both the self-domestication and the cooperative breeding hypotheses of human evolution. Here, we take a comparative approach to the evolutionary origin of the relatively larger lateral amygdala nucleus in humans. We carry out phylogenetic analysis on a sample of 17 mammalian species for which we acquired single amygdala nuclei volumetric data. Our results indicate that there has been convergent evolution toward larger lateral amygdala nuclei in both domesticated and cooperatively breeding mammals. These results suggest that changes in processing fearful stimuli to reduce fear-induced aggression, which are necessary for domesticated and cooperatively breeding species alike, tap into the same neurobiological proximate mechanism. However, humans show changes not only in processing fearful stimuli but also in proactive prosociality. Since cooperative breeding, but not domestication, is also associated with increased proactive prosociality, a prominent role of the former during human evolution is more parsimonious, whereas self-domestication may have been involved as an additional stepping stone.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
The amygdala has been described as a hub in brain networks that supports social life (Bickart et al., 2014; Wu & Hong, 2022). In humans, compared with other apes, the lateral nucleus of the amygdala is relatively larger (Barger et al., 2007) and contains more neurons (Barger et al., 2012). Recent work has demonstrated that, in humans, reduced amygdala volume correlates with a reduction in the ability to both feel fear and recognize fear in others, and with an increased prevalence of psychopathic traits (Marsh et al., 2008). Similarly, extreme altruists are characterized by a heightened ability to recognize fear, and by a larger and more active amygdala (Marsh et al., 2014). These neuroanatomical findings appear to be well in line with the self-domestication (SD) hypothesis of human evolution (Hare, 2017), which argues for selection against fear-induced aggression toward strangers given that the amygdala and particularly the lateral nucleus play a critical role in fear processing.
However, this relative increase in lateral amygdala nucleus volume in humans compared with other apes is also consistent with the cooperative breeding model (CB) of human evolution. The CB model (Hrdy & Burkart, 2020) posits that infant care by non-mothers (allomaternal care) became essential for infant survival during the Pleistocene. Indeed, our life history pattern, which includes short interbirth intervals without compromising infant size and/or brain size, depends, energetically, on the availability of helpers in raising offspring (Cerrito & Spear, 2022). Importantly, this reliance on allomaternal care requires that immatures overcome the fear of being close to conspecifics who are not their mothers, even if those conspecifics are in the possession of valuable food sources in the case of provisioning. Moreover, mothers need to overcome the fear of others approaching, handling, and carrying their infants. These two different types of fear and consequent aggressive responses (affective leading to defensive) make use of the same underlying brain networks, as verified across several species of mammals (Panksepp, 2004).
In sum, both SD and CB require overcoming fear, tolerating close proximity, and eventually eliciting care from others: in domesticated species from humans, and in cooperatively breeding ones from conspecifics. Both thus had to find ways to deal with these potentially fear-eliciting situations. We therefore predict similar neuroanatomical adaptations of the amygdala nuclei in both domesticated and cooperatively breeding species. We take a comparative approach to test whether the amygdala volumetric patterns observed in humans are more likely the result of SD or CB. Specifically, we hypothesize that a relatively larger lateral nucleus, which is the sensory interface in fear conditioning (LeDoux et al., 1990), allowed for the evolution of a heightened sensitivity in the recognition of others’ fear, expressed also, especially in cooperatively breeding species, via facial expressions (Cerrito & DeCasien, 2021). In humans, this increased ability to recognize others’ fear is thought to trigger a violence inhibition mechanism (Blair, 1995) which in turn allows for adults to care for infants that are not their own (Marsh, 2019).
To test our hypothesis, we combine neuroanatomical data with detailed data on infant care and domestication status and use phylogenetic methods to assess the correlation between relative volumes of different amygdala nuclei, domestication, and CB. Specifically, we test whether convergent evolution resulted in similar neuroanatomical adaptations in the amygdala of domesticated and cooperatively breeding species, with both groups having a larger than expected lateral nucleus than species who are neither domesticated nor cooperative breeders.
Materials and Methods
We compiled previously published volumetric data from 64 adult individuals of 17 mammalian species (of which three are domesticated and three are cooperative breeders) for the following regions: the amygdaloid complex (AC) and the basolateral nucleus (BLA) and its lateral (L), basal (B), and accessory basal (AB) nuclei (the latter three comprise the BLA). The species analyzed in the study are Sorex araneus, Pongo pygmaeus, Oryctolagus cuniculus, Cavia porcellus, Sus scrofa, Macaca nemestrina, Macaca fascicularis, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla, Rattus norvegicus, Macaca mulatta, Pan troglodytes, Hylobates lar, Sapajus apella, Vulpes vulpes, Callithrix jacchus, and Homo sapiens. In the dataset provided in the ESM for each individual of each species we report the sample size (n), sex, and age (in years). All data come from adult individuals, with a balanced representation of each sex. All volumetric data were obtained from the literature (Barger et al., 2007; Carlo et al., 2010; Chareyron et al., 2011, 2012; Równiak et al., 2022). When both left and right hemispheric values are reported, the mean was taken. Conversely, in some cases (Chareyron et al., 2011, 2012), the right or left hemisphere was randomly chosen after ascertaining the absence of lateralization (p = 0.757), and in yet others (Carlo et al., 2010) only the left is reported. The variance of volumetric measures is quite small within a species (Chareyron et al., 2011; Równiak et al., 2022); hence, data for species with even small samples sizes is expected to be representative of the taxon. All behavioral data were taken from published literature (Cerrito & Spear, 2022; Isler & van Schaik, 2012). For a definition of the measures of allomaternal care, see Isler and van Schaik (2012).
We created a binary variable aimed at expressing whether a species was a cooperative breeder. To do so we first created a compound “allocare_sum” variable as the sum of all forms of allomaternal care observed in each species: male provisioning of infants, provisioning of infants by other group members, carrying of infants by males, carrying of infants by others, allonursing, protection by male, and other forms of infant care, such as thermoregulation, babysitting, and pup retrieval (called communal work). We then made this continuous variable (which is the sum of all the single allocare variables) binary (non–cooperative breeder = 0 to 2.64; cooperative breeder = 2.64 to 5.2) using k-means clustering implemented using the “discretize” function in the R package arules (Hornik et al., 2005). Finally, we made a compound binary variable, called “elicit_care,” which takes the value of 1 for species that are either domesticated or cooperative breeders, or the value of 0 for those that are neither. Given that in some species there is no infant carrying at all (neither maternal nor allomaternal), we also created an “allocare_sum_NoCarry” variable which represents the sum of all allomaternal care behaviors except infant carrying. The breaks used to discretize this variable are: non–cooperative breeder = 0 to 2.36; cooperative breeder = 2.36 to 4.5. The resulting species identified as cooperative breeders are the same three as in the “allocare_sum” variable, and therefore the value of elicit_care for each species does not change whether one selects the “allocare_sum” or the “allocare_sum_NoCarry” value. The final and complete dataset is provided in the ESM, where volume is expressed in cubic centimeters (cm3).
We carried out all statistical analyses using R version 4.0.1 (R Core Team, 2020). We used phylogenetic generalized least squares (PGLS) regressions to assess the correlation between the variable “elicit_care” (domestication or cooperative breeding) and relative neuroanatomical data. To compare the data across species with a large range of variation in amygdala size, we regressed B, L, and AB against BLA and against the entire AC. We used the additive effect of “elicit_care” to test for significant differences between intercepts. We assessed the correlation between infant care measures on one side and relative neuroanatomical data on the other using phylogenetic generalized least squares (PGLS) regressions (Grafen, 1989). The models used a recently updated species-level mammals phylogeny (Upham et al., 2019). We used the maximum clade credibility tree calibrated using node dates and an exponential prior. To account for the high degree of skew, we log-transformed (in base e) all the volumetric, weight, and time variables.
We estimated λ for the PGLS models independently for each regression using maximum likelihood. We carried out the PGLS analyses using the “pgls” function in the R package caper (Orme et al., 2013). For each significant model we also report the effect size, expressed as percentage. To compare the effect size between different explanatory variables, we scaled the intercept difference to reflect the maximum value that the variable can take, so the intercept difference is multiplied by that value and then the difference in the natural log of the effect sizes is exponentiated to convert back to the original ratio of the effect sizes. We then proceeded to compute the AIC (Akaike, 1998) for each of the significant models and compared models with the same response variable. The complete list of models, their estimates, and the results of the model selection using AIC are available in Table 1. The model with the lowest AIC score is considered the best model, and models with an AIC score within 2 of that of the best model are also considered to receive support (Akaike, 1998).
Since the results were significant, we proceeded to conduct post-hoc tests to assess which of the constituting variables of “elicit_care” (i.e., domestication and cooperative breeding) were, individually, correlated with significant nuclei volumetric differences.
For each continuous variable we measured the phylogenetic signal using Blomberg’s K (Blomberg et al., 2003) implemented by the “phylosig” function in the R package phytools (Revell, 2012). To test significance, we used 100,000 iterations. For discrete variables we followed previously published methods (Cerrito & Spear, 2022) and estimated the phylogenetic signal using δ (Borges et al., 2019), and ran 10,000 iterations to test for significance. The value of K/δ for each of the variables analyzed is available in Table 2.
Finally, we used the Wheatsheaf Index (Arbuckle et al., 2014) to test for convergent evolution of nuclei volumes in cooperatively breeding and domesticated species. This index allows us to explicitly test hypotheses regarding the evolutionary convergence of quantitative traits by incorporating both phenotypic similarities and phylogenetic relatedness. We implemented this using the “test.windex” function in the R package windex (Arbuckle & Minter, 2015), which performs a statistical test for convergence. We defined as focal species the ones that are either cooperative breeders or domesticated ones (as per data in the ESM) and used 10,000 iterations to test for significance.
Results
Our findings suggest that both domesticated and cooperatively breeding species share adaptations in the amygdala that are fundamentally involved in processing fear-eliciting stimuli (Manassero et al., 2018; Ostroff et al., 2010). First, both cooperative breeding and domestication are associated with a significant increase of the volume of the lateral nucleus relative to BLA (+ 35% in both cases) and a relative decrease in the basal one (− 24% and − 26%, respectively; Fig. 1). These results are robust and very similar when regressing L and B against AC: a 46% relative increase in L for both groups, a 25% relative decrease in B for cooperatively breeding species, but no significant effect for domesticated ones. Further, we find that the model with the compound variable “elicit_care” as explanatory variable is the best one (according to the AIC value) in explaining both the increase of the lateral nucleus and the decrease of the basal one. No significant effect is found for the accessory basal nucleus.
The complete list of all the models tested, their estimates, and results are available in Table 1; the phylogenetic signal for each of the variables used is reported in Table 2.
Second, to test for convergence, we calculate the Wheatsheaf Index (W). The 10,000 iterations (Fig. 2) support the hypothesis of convergent evolution in cooperative breeding and domesticated species: (i) relative (to BLA) lateral nucleus volume (W = 5.38; p = 0.04; 95% CI = 3.42, 10.49; p = 0.04); (ii) relative (to BLA) basal nucleus volume (W = 5.49; p = 0.03; 95% CI = 3.65, 10.31; p = 0.03). We thus find robust evidence for a relatively larger lateral nucleus in cooperatively breeding and domesticated species that is the result of convergent evolution.
Discussion
Our results suggest that reduced reactive aggressiveness, mediated by a relatively larger lateral nucleus, constitutes a shared adaptation in cooperatively breeding and domesticated species. Both processes, domestication and cooperative breeding, apparently tap into the same proximate mechanism: a neurobiological adaptation of the lateral amygdala, likely relating to fear processing and reactive aggressiveness, which is adaptive in both cases. For domesticated animals, a reduction of fear-induced aggressiveness toward humans provides adaptive advantages (e.g., in terms of food sharing for commensal domestication), as in the case of dogs. Such a relationship is asymmetrical, and the roles do not change over time. Conversely, in cooperatively breeding species the selective pressure for increased social tolerance is entirely different as it is directed toward conspecifics, it confers the adaptive advantage of socially distributing the energetic cost of raising offspring, and it is susceptible to change over time with helpers becoming breeders themselves such that a given individual can take up different roles in the cooperative relationship as it matures.
Hence, what could likely be selected for in both CB and domesticated species is the reactive response to fear.
Further inquiries, with increased samples sizes, regarding the role played by the basal nucleus are warranted by our preliminary results showing that this nucleus (relative to total amygdala volume) is reduced only in cooperatively breeding species and not in domesticated ones. The basal nucleus is implicated, together with the nucleus accumbens, in the avoidance response (Ramirez et al., 2015). A relatively smaller nucleus could potentially correlate with a higher threshold that must be met by a stimulus before eliciting avoidance, thus favoring an approach response. This could be instrumental for mothers to allow others to care for their offspring, and for offspring, that individuals other than their mothers may care for them.
What are the implications of these findings for human evolution? The human social niche not only required changes in the processing of fearful stimuli and a reduction in fear-induced aggression, but also an increase in proactive prosociality—in other words, an other-regarding concern for the welfare of others (Burkart et al., 2007). Whereas reduced fear-induced aggression can result in increased social tolerance, proactive prosociality goes beyond that by actively motivating behaviors that benefit others. A reduction in reactive aggressiveness, potentially mediated by the derived amygdala volumetric patterns found in the present research, is thus only a prerequisite or stepping stone for the emergence of proactive prosociality. Whereas most domesticated species are not particularly prosocial, with dogs even losing this feature during the domestication process (Range & Marshall-Pescini, 2022), empirical evidence across a large number of species shows that cooperative breeding is associated with proactive prosociality (Burkart et al., 2014; Horn et al., 2020). Furthermore, recent research shows that our less reactively aggressive close relatives who arguably underwent self-domestication, the bonobos, nonetheless show low levels of prosociality when tested with the same paradigms (Verspeek et al., 2022). Our results thus indicate that the presence of reduced reactive aggressiveness in humans may be equally linked to self-domestication or cooperative breeding, but proactive prosociality requires additional selection pressures that are most likely related to cooperative breeding. Our results thus contribute to disentangling the roles of self-domestication and cooperative breeding during human evolution (Range & Marshall-Pescini, 2022; Sánchez-Villagra & van Schaik, 2019).
Data Accessibility
All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this published article and the attached ESM.
References
Akaike, H. (1998). Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. Selected papers of Hirotugu Akaike (pp. 199–213). Springer.
Arbuckle, K., & Minter, A. (2015). Windex: Analyzing convergent evolution using the Wheatsheaf index in R. Evolutionary Bioinformatics, 11, EBO–S20968. https://doi.org/10.4137/EBO.S20968.
Arbuckle, K., Bennett, C. M., & Speed, M. P. (2014). A simple measure of the strength of convergent evolution. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 5(7), 685–693. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12195.
Barger, N., Stefanacci, L., & Semendeferi, K. (2007). A comparative volumetric analysis of the amygdaloid complex and basolateral division in the human and ape brain. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 134(3), 392–403. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.20684.
Barger, N., Stefanacci, L., Schumann, C. M., Sherwood, C. C., Annese, J., Allman, J. M., Buckwalter, J. A., Hof, P. R., & Semendeferi, K. (2012). Neuronal populations in the basolateral nuclei of the amygdala are differentially increased in humans compared with apes: A stereological study. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 520(13), 3035–3054. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.23118.
Bickart, K. C., Dickerson, B. C., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2014). The amygdala as a hub in brain networks that support social life. Neuropsychologia, 63, 235–248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.013.
Blair, R. J. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57(1), 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(95)00676-P.
Blomberg, S. P., Garland Jr, T., & Ives, A. R. (2003). Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: Behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution, 57(4), 717–745. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00285.x.
Borges, R., Machado, J. P., Gomes, C., Rocha, A. P., & Antunes, A. (2019). Measuring phylogenetic signal between categorical traits and phylogenies. Bioinformatics, 35(11), 1862–1869. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/bty800.
Burkart, J. M., Fehr, E., Efferson, C., & van Schaik, C. P. (2007). Other-regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets provision food altruistically. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(50), 19762–19766. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0710310104.
Burkart, J. M., Allon, O., Amici, F., Fichtel, C., Finkenwirth, C., Heschl, A., Huber, J., Isler, K., Kosonen, Z. K., & Martins, E. (2014). The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation. Nature Communications, 5(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms5747.
Carlo, C. N., Stefanacci, L., Semendeferi, K., & Stevens, C. F. (2010). Comparative analyses of the neuron numbers and volumes of the amygdaloid complex in old and new world primates. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 518(8), 1176–1198. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.22264.
Cerrito, P., & DeCasien, A. R. (2021). The expression of care: Alloparental care frequency predicts neural control of facial muscles in primates. Evolution, 75(7), 1727–1737. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.14275.
Cerrito, P., & Spear, J. K. (2022). A milk-sharing economy allows placental mammals to overcome their metabolic limits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 119(10), e2114674119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2114674119.
Chareyron, L. J., Lavenex, B., Amaral, P., D. G., & Lavenex, P. (2011). Stereological analysis of the rat and monkey amygdala. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 519(16), 3218–3239. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.22677.
Chareyron, L. J., Lavenex, P. B., Amaral, D. G., & Lavenex, P. (2012). Postnatal development of the amygdala: A stereological study in macaque monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 520(9), 1965–1984. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.23023.
R Core Team. (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing, version 4.0.1. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-Project.org/.
Grafen, A. (1989). The phylogenetic regression. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 326(1233), 119–157. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1989.0106.
Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68(1), 155–186. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044201.
Horn, L., Bugnyar, T., Griesser, M., Hengl, M., Izawa, E. I., Oortwijn, T., Rössler, C., Scheer, C., Schiestl, M., & Suyama, M. (2020). Sex-specific effects of cooperative breeding and colonial nesting on prosociality in corvids. eLife, 9, e58139. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.58139.
Hornik, K., Grün, B., & Hahsler, M. (2005). Arules-A computational environment for mining association rules and frequent item sets. Journal of Statistical Software, 14(15), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v014.i15.
Hrdy, S. B., & Burkart, J. M. (2020). The emergence of emotionally modern humans: Implications for language and learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375(1803), 20190499. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0499.
Isler, K., & van Schaik, C. P. (2012). Allomaternal care, life history and brain size evolution in mammals. Journal of Human Evolution, 63(1), 52–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2012.03.009.
LeDoux, J. E., Cicchetti, P., Xagoraris, A., & Romanski, L. M. (1990). The lateral amygdaloid nucleus: Sensory interface of the amygdala in fear conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience, 10(4), 1062–1069. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-04-01062.
Manassero, E., Renna, A., Milano, L., & Sacchetti, B. (2018). Lateral and basal amygdala account for opposite behavioral responses during the long-term expression of fearful Memories. Scientific Reports, 8(1), https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-19074-3.
Marsh, A. A. (2019). The caring continuum: Evolved hormonal and proximal mechanisms explain prosocial and antisocial extremes. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 347–371. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103010.
Marsh, A. A., Finger, E. C., Mitchell, D. G., Reid, M. E., Sims, C., Kosson, D. S., Towbin, K. E., Leibenluft, E., Pine, D. S., & Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(6), 712–720. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07071145.
Marsh, A. A., Stoycos, S. A., Brethel-Haurwitz, K. M., Robinson, P., VanMeter, J. W., & Cardinale, E. M. (2014). Neural and cognitive characteristics of extraordinary altruists. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(42), 15036–15041. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1408440111.
Orme, D., Freckleton, R., Thomas, G., Petzoldt, T., Fritz, S., Isaac, N., & Pearse, W. (2013). The caper package: Comparative analysis of phylogenetics and evolution in R. R Package, version 0.5. https://caper.r-forge.r-project.org/.
Ostroff, L. E., Cain, C. K., Bedont, J., Monfils, M. H., & LeDoux, J. E. (2010). Fear and safety learning differentially affect synapse size and dendritic translation in the lateral amygdala. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(20), 9418–9423. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0913384107.
Panksepp, J. (2004). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press.
Ramirez, F., Moscarello, J. M., LeDoux, J. E., & Sears, R. M. (2015). Active avoidance requires a serial basal amygdala to nucleus accumbens shell circuit. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(8), 3470–3477. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1331-14.2015.
Range, F., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2022). Comparing wolves and dogs: Current status and implications for human ‘self-domestication’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(4), 337–349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2022.01.003.
Revell, L. J. (2012). phytools: An R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 3(2), 217–223. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210X.2011.00169.x.
Równiak, M., Bogus-Nowakowska, K., Kalinowski, D., & Kozłowska, A. (2022). The evolutionary trajectories of the individual amygdala nuclei in the common shrew, guinea pig, rabbit, fox and pig: A consequence of embryological fate and mosaic‐like evolution. Journal of Anatomy, 240(3), 489–502. https://doi.org/10.1111/joa.13571.
Sánchez-Villagra, M. R., & van Schaik, C. P. (2019). Evaluating the self‐domestication hypothesis of human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues News and Reviews, 28(3), 133–143. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21777.
Upham, N. S., Esselstyn, J. A., & Jetz, W. (2019). Inferring the mammal tree: Species-level sets of phylogenies for questions in ecology, evolution, and conservation. PLoS Biology, 17(12), e3000494. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000494.
Verspeek, J., van Leeuwen, E. J., Laméris, D. W., Staes, N., & Stevens, J. M. (2022). Adult bonobos show no prosociality in both prosocial choice task and group service paradigm. PeerJ, 10, e12849. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.12849.
Wickham, H., & W. Chang. (2016). Package ‘ggplot2’: Create elegant data visualisations using the grammar of graphics, Version 3.4.3. https://ggplot2.tidyverse.org, https://github.com/tidyverse/ggplot2.
Wu, Y. E., & Hong, W. (2022). Neural basis of prosocial behavior. Trends in Neurosciences, 45(10), 749–762. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2022.06.008.
Acknowledgements
We are extremely thankful to Carel van Schaik for providing constrictive feedback and suggestions. We would also like to thank Jeffrey K. Spear for generously sharing his R code, which was used to estimate ẟ.
Funding
Open access funding provided by Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich. Financial support for this project was provided by the Junior Fellowship of the Collegium Helveticum (ETH Zürich, Switzerland) and a consolidator grant from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (grant agreement No 101,001,295).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
PC conceived the study and carried out data collection and analysis. PC and JMB designed the study, interpreted the results, and wrote the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics Approvals
This research did not use samples or materials requiring ethical approval.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no financial or non-financial competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic Supplementary Material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Cerrito, P., Burkart, J.M. Human Amygdala Volumetric Patterns Convergently Evolved in Cooperatively Breeding and Domesticated Species. Hum Nat 34, 501–511 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-023-09461-3
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-023-09461-3