Abstract
Purpose of Review
Anxiety symptoms increase during the peri-menstrual phase of the menstrual cycle in people with anxiety disorders. Whether this reflects a heightened variant of normal menstrual-related changes in psychological states experienced by healthy (i.e. non-anxious) people is unknown. Moreover, menstrual-related change in anxiety symptoms is a poorly understood phenomenon, highlighting a need for pre-clinical models to aid mechanistic discovery. Here, we review recent evidence for menstrual effects on anxiety-like features in healthy humans as a counterpart to recent reviews that have focused on clinically anxious populations. We appraise the utility of rodent models to identify mechanisms of menstrual effects on anxiety and offer suggestions to harmonise methodological practices across species to advance knowledge in this field.
Recent Findings
Consistent with reports in clinical populations, some evidence indicates anxiety symptoms increase during the peri-menstrual period in healthy people, although null results have been reported, and these effects are heterogeneous across studies and individuals. Studies in rats show robust increases in anxiety during analogous phases of the oestrous cycle.
Summary
Studies in female rats are useful to identify the evolutionarily conserved biological mechanisms of menstrual-related changes in anxiety. Future experimental approaches in rats should model the heterogeneity observed in human studies to increase alignment across species and advance understanding of the individual factors that increase the propensity to experience menstrual-related changes in anxiety.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
The female menstrual cycle (Fig. 1A) modulates the severity and frequency of anxiety symptoms in a subset of menstruating people with a range of diagnostic subtypes, including generalised anxiety disorder (GAD), social anxiety disorder, panic disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and obsessive–compulsive disorder, as summarised in recent reviews [1, 2, 3•].
Notwithstanding notable methodological limitations in many of these studies, there is relative consensus that anxious symptoms in this subset of people worsen peri-menstrually, i.e. the weeks prior to and during menstruation [3•], a phenomenon referred to as ‘premenstrual exacerbation’ (PME). PME is observed across different anxiety subtypes and symptom dimensions, including physiological [13], cognitive [14] and affective [15,16,17], and is not a product of comorbid reproductive endocrine conditions like premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD). In most studies, PME is evident in a subset and not the full complement of anxious symptoms measured. Moreover, a minority of studies [18] find no evidence for menstrual changes in any symptom. Evidence supports substantial individual variation in PME [3•], akin to that reported for PMDD and perimenopausal mood disorder [19]. For example, based on retrospective reports, estimates of PME prevalence amongst menstruating people with anxiety or related disorders are 41–79% in panic disorder [20, 21], 45% in social anxiety disorder [17] and 48.5% in obsessive–compulsive disorder [22]. Whether menstrual fluctuations in anxiety symptoms are specific to people with anxiety disorders or are a heightened variant of normal changes in psychological states experienced over the menstrual cycle in healthy (i.e. non-anxious) people is an outstanding question. Moreover, the mechanisms underlying peri-menstrual exacerbation of anxiety symptoms are poorly understood (in anxious and non-anxious populations alike) and likely involve complex interplays between biological and psychological processes. Thus, pre-clinical animal models of menstrual changes in anxiety are required to enable the use of techniques that can identify biological mechanisms with greater precision and greater ability to interrogate cause-and-effect at a greater pace and scale than can be achieved in human studies. Promisingly, comparable changes in anxiety-like behaviour over the rat reproductive cycle (the ‘oestrous’ cycle) are well documented [23]. In this article, we review the evidence for menstrual changes in anxiety symptoms in healthy humans (i.e. those not meeting diagnostic criteria for a mental health condition, including PMDD) to provide a counterpart to the recent reviews on menstrual-related symptom changes in humans with anxiety disorders [2, 3•]. We also review the evidence for oestrous-mediated changes in anxiety-like behaviour in rats and compare these findings against those obtained in humans. We focus on rats because of the inconsistent oestrous-associated hormonal and behavioural changes reported in past literature in mice, possibly driven by strain differences and greater oestrous variability in mice [24,25,26,27,28,29]. We highlight both the complexities and the potential gains in investigating menstrual fluctuations in anxiety via a cross-species translational framework, and we offer recommendations for future research to harmonise methodological practices across species to improve our understanding of this phenomena in healthy and clinical human populations.
Menstrual Fluctuations in Anxiety in Healthy (Non-anxious) Humans
Most research on menstrual changes in psychological symptoms in healthy people have focused on mood-related symptoms [30] or used global scales that average scores across distinct psychological states (mood and anxiety [31, 32•, 33, 34]), resulting in a minority of research that has expressly measured and reported statistical comparisons for changes in anxiety across the menstrual cycle in healthy people. These studies differ substantially with respect to anxiety dimension assessed, e.g. trait or state anxiety using validated measures [13, 18, 35,36,37] or single items [38], cognitive features of anxiety (repetitive negative thinking [14], perceptions of control [33, 34], anxiety sensitivity [39]), discrete emotions (nervousness/irritability [40]), sub-classes of anxiety (attachment anxiety [38], phobic anxiety [16], panic symptoms [13, 41, 42]) and fatigue [40, 43]. Moreover, most studies have assessed symptoms during pre-selected menstrual phases (e.g. mid-luteal versus early follicular) rather than over a full cycle, and the precise phases examined differ between studies. These factors make it difficult to compare findings across studies. Moreover, consistent with commentaries on menstrual cycle research more broadly [4, 44], many studies are methodologically limited. For example, most studies are underpowered (with sample sizes fewer than 10 in several studies), and menstrual phases are poorly defined and unreliably measured (e.g. via day count in the absence of tests to confirm ovulation). Likewise, measures assessing anxiety are often crude (e.g. a single item), and no study has measured the full range of physiological, affective, cognitive and behavioural dimensions that characterise anxiety. Finally, very few studies have directly compared anxious versus non-anxious populations, which is needed to determine whether the presence, quality or magnitude of cyclic changes in anxiety symptoms differs as a function of mental health status.
Despite the overall limitations of the literature outlined above, some studies support the existence of menstrual-related fluctuations in anxiety symptoms in healthy people. An early study [45] reported evidence for elevated premenstrual anxiety relative to ovulation in a thematic analysis of free association speech samples. Consistent with these findings, a later study [35] reported increased premenstrual state anxiety and obsessive–compulsive symptoms relative to the follicular phase, and these effects were independent of the severity of premenstrual physical symptoms reported (e.g. bloating, headache). A recent study [46•] in high-functioning trauma-exposed individuals, the majority of whom did not meet PTSD criteria, assessed PTSD symptoms daily across the follicular phase, as oestradiol levels were increasing, confirmed via salivary assays. They found that overall PTSD symptoms, negative cognitions, and arousal, decreased over this time interval. Additionally, oestradiol levels assessed during a lab visit negatively correlated with PTSD symptoms, and people in a high oestradiol menstrual phase (periovulatory, late-follicular) showed less sympathetic nervous system activity and more cortisol activity in response to a trauma reminder relative to those in a low oestradiol menstrual phase (early-follicular, early- and late-luteal). However, null results have also been reported in recent studies. One study [40] reported no menstrual-related changes in nervousness, irritability, or fatigue, assessed daily over at least two menstrual cycles, and another [36] reported no changes in state anxiety during the early- and mid-follicular and mid-luteal phases. Another study [38] reported no changes in attachment anxiety or avoidance over the follicular, ovulatory and luteal phases, but did find that as progesterone increased relative to each person’s average progesterone levels, anxiety also increased. Other studies [33, 34] found no changes in perceived control from the follicular to premenstrual phases. Studies using between-groups comparisons have reported mixed results [37, 38, 47], but a major limitation of between-group designs is the inability to control for individual differences in anxiety levels that could mask within-person variation.
Of the studies that have directly compared menstrual effects in anxious versus non-anxious people, some have reported no evidence for menstrual changes in self-reported anxiety symptoms, or panic symptoms, in non-anxious people [13, 18, 42], despite these studies finding some evidence for cyclic changes in anxiety symptoms in people with panic disorder. Likewise, a more recent study [48] reported no menstrual changes in panic symptoms in people without panic disorder, despite finding a premenstrual increase in panic symptoms in people with panic disorder and a menses-related increase in physical symptoms (e.g. bloating, headache) in both groups. We recently reported that non-anxious people exhibited increased mental fatigue from the early follicular to the mid-luteal phase, comparable to the high levels of fatigue exhibited by people with GAD at both menstrual phases [43]. In a separate study [14], we found no evidence for menstrual fluctuations in repetitive negative thinking and negative effect amongst non-anxious people, despite finding a mid-luteal increase in these symptoms in people with GAD. However, within-person fluctuations in progesterone were related to these symptoms in non-anxious people, but not those with GAD. Specifically, these symptoms were elevated during times of reduced progesterone levels relative to each person’s average, consistent with progesterone’s role in the synthesis of anxiolytic neurosteroids. Finally, studies on PTSD have reported an increase in phobic anxiety in the early-follicular phase relative to the mid-luteal phase in trauma-exposed people with PTSD, but not those without PTSD [16], with no cyclic changes in either group on anxiety sensitivity [39].
Other studies have examined individual difference factors that moderate menstrual fluctuations of anxiety symptoms in healthy people. Nillni et al. [41] compared changes in panic symptoms in healthy people who were high or low in anxiety sensitivity (fear of anxiety and anxiety-related sensations) at different menstrual phases. Cognitive responses to a CO2 challenge (e.g. fear of losing control) were higher in the premenstrual phase relative to the follicular phase, only in people high in anxiety sensitivity. There was no impact of the menstrual cycle in either group on physical or self-reported panic symptoms. A recent study [49] reported that whereas people higher in health anxiety reported increases in perceived stress from the follicular phase to the week prior to menses, the opposite pattern was noted in those lower in health anxiety. Notably, health anxiety did not moderate premenstrual symptoms, illustrating menstrual changes in anxiety symptoms seem to exist independent of premenstrual symptom severity.
Combined, evidence to date for menstrual-related fluctuations in anxiety in healthy people appears more equivocal than that in populations with anxiety disorders. However, of the studies that do report significant changes, in general (with some exceptions), it is the peri-menstrual phase that is associated with anxiety symptom exacerbation, consistent with the pattern of PME reported in clinical populations.
Methodological Considerations in Studies of Menstrual-related Anxiety Change in Healthy Humans
The work by Nillni and colleagues highlights the importance of the moderating role of individual difference factors, such as anxiety sensitivity and health anxiety. These factors may be widely dispersed in a healthy population, as opposed to a clinical population which may cluster more uniformly around one point of these dimensions (e.g. high anxiety sensitivity). The wider dispersion of these factors in healthy populations could lead to greater variation in the magnitude, presence, or direction of menstrual cycle fluctuation and may partly account for null results or inconsistencies between studies in healthy people. Relatedly, menstrual fluctuation in anxiety symptoms is likely a heterogeneous phenomenon experienced by a subset of those who menstruate, and there may be distinct sub-patterns of cyclic changes in symptoms. This is exemplified in work by Kiesner [32•], who tracked changes in anxiety and depression in a community sample of 213 people (including those with and without psychiatric conditions and menstrual-related difficulties) across two menstrual cycles. A single score of anxiety and depression was calculated by collapsing responses to two items querying anxiety and four items querying depression in the last 24 h. Although the study is somewhat limited by the use of an unvalidated measure of anxiety, the outcomes highlight the importance of considering individual trajectories of symptom change over the cycle. Specifically, cluster analysis revealed four patterns of anxiety/depression change across the menstrual cycle, with one group reporting no cyclic change, another group reporting a midcycle symptom exacerbation (around ovulation), and the remaining two groups both reporting a ‘classic’ premenstrual symptom exacerbation, with one of the groups exhibiting a larger magnitude of change than the other. Combined, work by Nillni and Kiesner suggests that an advanced understanding of the phenomenology of menstrual fluctuation in anxiety symptoms in healthy people is likely to be made by using sufficiently sized samples to capture an array of individual variations and to use statistical techniques (e.g. latent group modelling) to identify subgroups exhibiting different patterns of symptom change. Conversely, groups could be selected for more focused investigations by pre-selecting individuals for heightened or reduced levels of moderators implicated in menstrual effects on anxiety (e.g. anxiety sensitivity, health anxiety).
Other methodological considerations include the importance of measuring for hormonal contraceptive use, like the oral contraceptive pill, which introduces high and relatively steady levels of synthetic oestradiol and progestins. The oral contraceptive pill chronically suppresses ovulation and inhibits ovarian production of oestradiol and progesterone. Several studies did not assess hormonal contraceptive use (e.g. [48]), which significantly compromises the internal validity of the study because changes in anxiety symptoms observed over a monthly timeframe in people taking hormonal contraceptives cannot be attributed to the impact of the menstrual cycle. Moreover, an underutilised but possibly useful approach is to compare changes in anxiety symptoms between cycling people and those taking the oral contraceptive pill. Such a design would enable hypotheses regarding the mechanisms of menstrual-related symptom change to be assessed. For example, if cyclic changes in hormones mediate symptom change, then the magnitude of symptom variance over time should be reduced in those taking the oral contraceptive pill (who have steady levels of hormones) relative to cycling people. A conceptually similar idea is to compare symptom change in cycling people during ovulatory versus anovulatory cycles, which differ in their hormonal profiles (e.g. anovulatory cycles have lower progesterone) and thus provide a comparison condition to enable the assessment of hypotheses regarding mechanisms of menstrual fluctuations in anxiety. For example, a within-person analysis in a longitudinal study [50] revealed a greater peri-menstrual increase in self-reported anxiety for ovulatory versus anovulatory cycles. Data from anovulatory cycles could easily be examined via within-person analyses over multiple cycles or between-person analyses over a single cycle.
Oestrous Fluctuations in Anxiety-like Behaviour in Female Rats
Symptoms of anxiety disorders can be modelled in rats using unlearned fear paradigms. The most common unlearned fear tasks capitalise on the animal’s innate fear of ethologically relevant stimuli (e.g. elevated or open spaces, bright lights, predator odour) and therefore do not require training to evoke behavioural fear responses (e.g. avoidance, freezing), termed ‘anxiety-like behaviour’. These approach-avoidance conflict tasks include the elevated plus maze, open field test and light–dark box. Other unlearned fear tasks that do not depend on approach-avoidance conflict include those measuring active avoidance behaviour (e.g. shock probe burying test, marble burying test), startle response (e.g. acoustic startle, light-enhanced startle) or social behaviour (e.g. ultrasonic vocalisations, social interaction tests).
The impact of the oestrous cycle (Fig. 1B) on anxiety-like behaviour in female rats is conceptually consistent with findings in clinically anxious and healthy women. That is, female rats exhibit higher anxiety-like behaviour when tested during oestrous phases of low sex hormones (metestrus or diestrus) compared to oestrous phases of high sex hormones (proestrus). These effects are highly robust across various rat strains, testing paradigms, lighting conditions and experimental designs and have not diminished over time, with several recent studies reporting similar results [51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65, 66•, 67•, 68, 69•]. A minority report null effects [70,71,72,73,74,75]. The analogous findings cross-species suggest that changes in anxiety coincident with cycling hormones are likely linked to an evolutionarily conserved mechanism in female rats and women. Given that rats do not menstruate, it is unlikely that physical symptoms associated with menses in women (e.g. pain, physical discomfort) can completely account for peri-menstrual anxiety, notwithstanding the likely impact of social and cultural beliefs about menstruation [76]. For example, one possibility is that heightened anxiety during the premenstrual phase is a by-product of heightened threat perception following ovulation that facilitates a successful pregnancy [77], which may be more evident in hormonally sensitive individuals. Another possibility is that changes in anxiety coincident with cycling hormones across the reproductive cycle prepare rats and humans to respond appropriately to extended and elevated hormonal changes across the peripartum period to ensure offspring survival.
Potential biological mechanisms underlying hormonal-associated fluctuations in anxiety that have been examined in female rats and humans include oestradiol and the oestrogen receptors [78, 79], the progesterone metabolite allopregnanolone and its actions on the GABAergic system [80, 81] and the neurotransmitter serotonin [82]. However, the mechanisms remain understudied, and the mechanisms contributing to individual variance in menstrual fluctuations in anxiety are unknown and are difficult, time-intensive and costly to examine in humans. Studies in female rats offer a controlled laboratory setting to investigate the potential neurobiological and endocrinological mechanisms that may underlie menstrual-related changes in anxiety symptoms in humans with greater scale and pace. To help improve the translational value of female rat findings, in the following sections, we offer some considerations for future research.
Defining and Aligning Oestrous and Menstrual Cycle Phases
Although there are broad similarities between the human and rat reproductive cycles (Fig. 1), there are also differences. In addition to differences in cycle length, the timing of ovulation relative to the progesterone surge differs between rats and humans, with ovulation occurring prior to the progesterone surge in humans and after the progesterone surge in rats. In both species, oestradiol surges prior to ovulation, but whereas humans experience a secondary rise in oestradiol during the post-ovulation luteal phase, this is absent in rats. Both species exhibit a post-ovulation rise in progesterone, but this appears to be stronger in humans than in rats. Finally, unlike humans, rats do not menstruate. As such, although the impact of relative differences in sex hormone levels on various behavioural outcomes can be compared in humans and rats, it is important to be mindful of cross-species differences when making inferences about humans based on rats – A point that is rarely acknowledged in studies on oestrous effects on anxiety-like behaviour.
We suggest that translational researchers justify their selection of the oestrous phase being examined and the time of day of testing based on the research question and indicate how it aligns with the human menstrual cycle. For example, if a researcher is interested in declining oestradiol and progesterone levels as experienced during the premenstrual phase in humans, this may correspond to the oestrus phase in rats. However, this may confound the impact of declining sex hormones with the period of sexual receptivity, so testing during metestrus or diestrus may be more appropriate. Some researchers use the afternoon of diestrus in female rats as an analogue to the premenstrual phase in humans, given that during diestrus, there is a withdrawal from a mini peak in progesterone which is then proceeded by a surge in oestradiol [73, 83,84,85]. Moreover, while some studies examine distinct oestrous phases [52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62, 68, 70,71,72,73], others combine phases with similar hormonal profiles, with proestrus and oestrus as a ‘high sex hormones’ group [51, 63,64,65, 66•, 67•, 69•, 74, 75] despite oestradiol and progesterone declining during oestrus and potentially diluting the anxiolytic effect of proestrus. Studying oestrous phases that are clearly defined with respect to hormonal state and are aligned to the human cycle will improve the translatability of rat findings to humans.
Using Repeated Measures to Examine Individual Differences in Anxiety Across the Oestrous Cycle
Whereas most rodent studies on oestrous effects adopt a between-subject design, most human studies on menstrual effects adopt a within-subject design, which, as noted, is better suited to detect menstrual-related changes in anxiety. Behaviour on unlearned fear tests in rats, such as the EPM, is commonly evaluated using a single trial to avoid behavioural changes that may arise due to non-associative learning processes (e.g. sensitization or habituation). This concern stems from the observation of a ‘one-trial tolerance’ to the anxiolytic-like effects of benzodiazepines on the EPM, wherein the anxiolytic effects are diminished upon a second exposure to the EPM [86, 87]. However, there is little evidence to suggest that sensitisation or habituation to anxiety tests occurs under non-drug-induced states. Rather, a recent study in male rats found that behaviour on the EPM was comparable over three trials, with deviations from the initial test only emerging in the fourth trial [88]. Unpublished data from our lab has found a similar lack of sensitisation or habituation in female rats over two testing trials on the EPM and LDB (Fig. 2). As such, repeated testing on unlearned fear tasks may be a valid approach for assessing oestrous effects on anxiety-like behaviour in female rats, requiring only two trials (e.g. once each during metestrus and proestrus), counterbalancing oestrous phase at the first test.
Examining oestrous effects on anxiety-like behaviour using a within-subjects design is advantageous because it allows researchers to identify underlying mechanisms that account for individual differences in oestrous-related exacerbation in anxiety-related behaviour, with the aim of translating these findings to understand the biological source of the heterogeneity of menstrual effects on anxiety in humans. Indeed, individual differences in oestrous effects on anxiety-like behaviour have been demonstrated on a marble burying task, such that only 30% out of 85 female rats exhibited cycle-dependent burying behaviour, with higher marble burying during metestrus versus proestrus [60]. Future studies could measure both anxiety-like behaviour (e.g. via EPM) and hormone/neurosteroid levels (e.g. blood collection via catheter) at multiple oestrous phases in the same rat. This experimental approach would enable researchers to identify whether absolute levels of hormones, changes in hormones and/or the ratio of hormones are associated with variability in oestrous effects on anxiety-like behaviour. Studies in rats could also identify the neurobiological processes underlying heightened sensitivity to hormonal fluctuations in the absence of differential hormone/neurosteroid levels. For example, unlike in humans, studies in rats could easily examine brain changes (e.g. neuronal activity via cfos expression, gene and protein expression) to typical hormonal changes across the oestrous cycle in rats identified as ‘hormone sensitive’ (i.e. rats that exhibit cyclic-dependent anxiety-like behaviour). This is important given recent demonstrations that cell cultures from people with versus without PMDD exhibit differential changes in gene expression following treatment with oestradiol and progesterone [89]. These findings highlight the need to examine cellular responsivity to hormones beyond the crude assessment of absolute changes in hormones, which is something that can be measured with greater ease in rats. The knowledge gained from such experiments may provide insights into the endocrinological and neurobiological characteristics of the subset of humans with and without anxiety disorders who experience menstrual fluctuations in anxiety.
Modelling Multiple Features of Anxiety in Rats and Aligning Experimental Tasks Across Species
Just as anxiety symptoms in humans are comprised of affective, behavioural, physiological, and cognitive dimensions that can be assessed using different procedures, tests of unlearned fear in rats capture different facets of anxiety-like behaviour [90]. For example, some tasks measure active responses (burying behaviour, compulsive grooming), whereas others measure passive responses (freezing, avoidance). However, most studies measure anxiety-like behaviour on a single approach-avoidance task. While these tasks have high face validity given that avoidance is a key characteristic of anxiety disorders, the caveat of these tasks is that it is difficult to disentangle differences in emotionality and general locomotor activity, an issue which appears to be particularly relevant for female rats [91]. It has been suggested that researchers can address this post-hoc by calculating a ratio score of anxiety-like behaviours divided by locomotive measures (e.g. the ‘anxiety index’ [92, 93]). Another solution is to use a battery of tests, including active tasks, such as marble burying behaviour, acoustic startle response, light-potentiated startle response or social interaction, which would produce a more comprehensive picture of oestrous effects on anxiety [51, 56, 61, 62, 66•].
An advantage of using measures such as the acoustic startle response in female animals is that this test can also be examined in humans. Indeed, some studies report higher startle response during the late-luteal phase compared to the early follicular phase in humans [94, 95], although other studies report no changes [96, 97]. Another task that can be examined across species is the light-potentiated startle response in female rats and the dark-potentiated startle response in humans [98], yet no studies have examined the effect of the reproductive cycle on these tasks. Humans can also be tested on an EPM using virtual reality while also measuring physiological responses (e.g. heart rate, breathing) and subjective measures of anxiety [99, 100•]. Examining common tasks across species may improve the translatability of results. In addition, such findings will add to a growing body of research showing that cyclic sex hormones impact models of learned anxiety-related features in rats and humans, such as fear extinction (model of fear inhibition [101]) and pre-pulse inhibition of the startle response (model of sensorimotor gating [102]). Moreover, whereas rat studies assess anxiety-like behaviour under provoked (inherently threatening) situations, the majority of studies on menstrual-related effects on anxiety in healthy humans measure changes in basal (i.e. unprovoked) symptoms, with some notable exceptions [13, 46•]. More robust effects of the menstrual cycle could be unmasked in healthy people using tasks designed to provoke symptoms (e.g. behavioural approach tests with spiders, social stress paradigms); this is particularly relevant in healthy people, given that floor effects could contribute to null results.
Testing Non-human Animals Selectively Bred for High Anxiety to Align with Clinically Anxious Humans
To the best of our knowledge, all research on oestrous effects on anxiety-like behaviour have been conducted using normally bred adult female rats, with one exception [65] that examined Wistar Kyoto rats, a strain associated with heightened stress sensitivity. Selective breeding for extremes in trait anxiety has produced rodent strains with inborn low or high anxiety-like behaviour [103, 104]. Whereas the literature on oestrous effects on anxiety in normally bred rats may offer insights into menstrual-related changes in anxiety in healthy people, future studies testing oestrous effects on anxiety-like behaviour in female rodents selectively bred for high anxiety-like behaviour may be a promising animal model for examining the genetic, neurobiological, and endocrinological causes of menstrual-related anxiety fluctuations in clinically anxious women. Additionally, examining oestrous effects on anxiety-like behaviour in both normally bred and high-anxiety rodents may be a useful analogue to examine potential differences in menstrual cycle effects on anxiety between healthy and clinically anxious human populations.
Conclusions
Fluctuations in anxiety across the menstrual cycle are evident in both clinical and healthy human populations, although evidence to date indicates a more robust effect in clinical populations, and substantial heterogeneity is evident in both populations. Analogous fluctuations in anxiety-like behaviour across the oestrous cycle in rats point to an evolutionarily conserved biological mechanism that at least in part underlies these effects. Cross-species translational frameworks can be better leveraged to investigate and understand this effect. Future research should endeavour to improve the alignment of cross-species models through increasing the reliability and comparability of the hormonal phases that are examined, employing within-subject designs, and further investigating which individual factors may contribute to the incidence and pattern of menstrual-related fluctuations in anxiety across species.
References
Papers of particular interest, published recently, have been highlighted as: • Of importance
Handy AB, Greenfield SF, Yonkers KA, Payne LA. Psychiatric symptoms across the menstrual cycle in adult women: a comprehensive review. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2022;30:100–17.
Nillni YI, Rasmusson AM, Paul EL, Pineles SL. The impact of the menstrual cycle and underlying hormones in anxiety and PTSD: what do we know and where do we go from here? Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2021;23:8.
• Green SA, Graham BM. Symptom fluctuation over the menstrual cycle in anxiety disorders, PTSD, and OCD: a systematic review. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2022;25:71–85. A recent systematic review of menstrual fluctuations in anxiety symptoms in people with anxiety disorders, PTSD and OCD.
Schmalenberger KM, Tauseef HA, Barone JC, Owens SA, Lieberman L, Jarczok MN, et al. How to study the menstrual cycle: practical tools and recommendations. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2021;123: 104895.
Hampson E. A brief guide to the menstrual cycle and oral contraceptive use for researchers in behavioral endocrinology. Horm Behav. 2020.
Shea AA, Vitzthum VJ. The extent and causes of natural variation in menstrual cycles: integrating empirically-based models of ovarian cycling into research on women’s health. Drug Discovery Today: Disease Models. 2020;41–9.
Smith MS, Freeman ME, Neill JD. The control of progesterone secretion during the estrous cycle and early pseudopregnancy in the rat: prolactin, gonadotropin and steroid levels associated with rescue of the Corpus luteum of pseudopregnancy. Endocrinology. 1975;96:219–26.
Hubscher CH, Brooks DL, Johnson JR. A quantitative method for assessing stages of the rat estrous cycle. Biotech Histochem. 2005;80:79–87.
Becker JB, Arnold AP, Berkley KJ, Blaustein JD, Eckel LA, Hampson E, et al. Strategies and methods for research on sex differences in brain and behavior. Endocrinology. 2005;146:1650–73.
Butcher RL, Collins WE, Fugo NW. Plasma concentration of LH, FSH, prolactin, progesterone and estradiol-17beta throughout the 4-day estrous cycle of the rat. Endocrinology. 1974;94:1704–8.
Abraham GE, Odell WD, Swerdloff RS, Hopper K. Simultaneous radioimmunoassay of plasma FSH, LH, progesterone, 17-hydroxyprogesterone, and estradiol-17 beta during the menstrual cycle. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1972;34:312–8.
Draper CF, Duisters K, Weger B, Chakrabarti A, Harms AC, Brennan L, et al. Menstrual cycle rhythmicity: metabolic patterns in healthy women. Sci Rep. 2018;8.
Sigmon ST, Dorhofer DM, Rohan KJ, Hotovy LA, Boulard NE, Fink CM. Psychophysiological, somatic, and affective changes across the menstrual cycle in women with panic disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68:425–31.
Li SH, Denson TF, Graham BM. Women with generalized anxiety disorder show increased repetitive negative thinking during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle. Clin Psychol Sci. 2020;8:1037–45.
Kaspi SP, Otto MW, Pollack MH, Eppinger S, Rosenbaum JF. Premenstrual exacerbation of symptoms in women with panic disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 1994;8:131–8.
Nillni YI, Pineles SL, Patton SC, Rouse MH, Sawyer AT, Rasmusson AM. Menstrual cycle effects on psychological symptoms in women with PTSD. J Trauma Stress. 2015;28:1–7.
van Veen JF, Jonker BW, van Vliet IM, Zitman FG. The effects of female reproductive hormones in generalized social anxiety disorder. Int J Psychiatry Med. 2009;39:283–95.
Stein MB, Schmidt PJ, Rubinow DR, Uhde TW. Panic disorder and the menstrual cycle: panic disorder patients, healthy control subjects, and patients with premenstrual syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 1989;146.
Rubinow DR, Schmidt PJ. Is there a role for reproductive steroids in the etiology and treatment of affective disorders? Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2018;20:187–96.
Cook BL, Noyes R, Garvey MJ, et al. Anxiety and the menstrual cycle in panic disorder. J Affect Disord. 1990;19:221–6.
Kaspi SP, Otto MW, Pollack MH, et al. Premenstrual exacerbation of symptoms in women with panic disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 1994;8:131–8.
Vulink NCC, Denys D, Onie Bus L, Westenberg HGM. Female hormones affect symptom severity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2006;21:171–5.
Lovick TA, Zangrossi H Jr. Effect of estrous cycle on behavior of females in rodent tests of anxiety. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12: 711065.
Walmer DK, Wrona MA, Hughes CL, Nelson KG. Lactoferrin expression in the mouse reproductive tract during the natural estrous cycle: correlation with circulating estradiol and progesterone. Endocrinology. 1992;131:1458–66.
McLean AC, Valenzuela N, Fai S, Bennett SA. Performing vaginal lavage, crystal violet staining, and vaginal cytological evaluation for mouse estrous cycle staging identification. J Vis Exp. 2012;e4389.
Walf AA, Koonce C, Manley K, Frye CA. Proestrous compared to diestrous wildtype, but not estrogen receptor beta knockout, mice have better performance in the spontaneous alternation and object recognition tasks and reduced anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus and mirror maze. Behav Brain Res. 2009;196:254–60.
Koonce CJ, Walf AA, Frye CA. Type 1 5α-reductase may be required for estrous cycle changes in affective behaviors of female mice. Behav Brain Res. 2012;226:376–80.
Meziane H, Ouagazzal AM, Aubert L, Wietrzych M, Krezel W. Estrous cycle effects on behavior of C57BL/6J and BALB/cByJ female mice: implications for phenotyping strategies. Genes Brain Behav. 2007;6:192–200.
Corpéchot C, Collins BE, Carey MP, Tsouros A, Robel P, Fry JP. Brain neurosteroids during the mouse oestrous cycle. Brain Res. 1997;766:276–80.
Laessle RG, Tuschl RJ, Schweiger U, Pirke KM. Mood changes and physical complaints during the normal menstrual cycle in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1990;15:131–8.
Craner JR, Sigmon ST, Young MA. Self-focused attention and symptoms across menstrual cycle phases in women with and without premenstrual disorders. Cogn Ther Res. 2016;40:118–27.
• Kiesner J. One woman’s low is another woman’s high: paradoxical effects of the menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:68–76. One of few studies to characterise sub-patterns of menstrual changes in affective symptoms in a large community sample including those with and without psychiatric and menstrual-related conditions.
Mahon JN, Rohan KJ, Nillni YI, Zvolensky MJ. The role of perceived control over anxiety in prospective symptom reports across the menstrual cycle. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2015;18:239–46.
Manikandan S, Nillni YI, Zvolensky MJ, Rohan KJ, Carkeek KR, Leyro TM. The role of emotion regulation in the experience of menstrual symptoms and perceived control over anxiety-related events across the menstrual cycle. Arch Women’s Ment Health. 2016;19:1109–17.
Gonda X, Telek T, Juhász G, Lazary J, Vargha A, Bagdy G. Patterns of mood changes throughout the reproductive cycle in healthy women without premenstrual dysphoric disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2008;32:1782–8.
Özer Kaya D, Toprak ÇŞ. Fluctuations of state anxiety, spinal structure, and postural stability across the menstrual cycle in active women. Turk J Med Sci. 2016;46:977–84.
Walder DJ, Statucka M, Daly MP, Axen K, Haber M. Biological sex and menstrual cycle phase modulation of cortisol levels and psychiatric symptoms in a non-clinical sample of young adults. Psychiatry Res. 2012;197:314–21.
Reynolds TA, Makhanova A, Marcinkowska UM, Jasienska G, McNulty JK, Eckel LA, et al. Progesterone and women’s anxiety across the menstrual cycle. Horm Behav. 2018;102:34–40.
Nillni YI, Arditte Hall KA, Langdon KJ, Pineles SL. Examination of the stability of the anxiety sensitivity index across the menstrual cycle in trauma-exposed women with and without PTSD. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2020;33:115–21.
Lorenz TK, Gesselman AN, Vitzthum VJ. Variance in mood symptoms across menstrual cycles: implications for premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Womens Reprod Health. 2017;4:77–88.
Nillni YI, Rohan KJ, Zvolensky MJ. The role of menstrual cycle phase and anxiety sensitivity in catastrophic misinterpretation of physical symptoms during a CO(2) challenge. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2012;15:413–22.
Perna G, Brambilla F, Arancio C, Bellodi L. Menstrual cycle-related sensitivity to 35% CO2 in panic patients. Biol Psychiatry. 1995;37:528–32.
Li SH, Lloyd AR, Graham BM. Physical and mental fatigue across the menstrual cycle in women with and without generalised anxiety disorder. Horm Behav. 2020;118: 104667.
Blake KR, Dixson BJW, O’Dean SM, Denson TF. Standardized protocols for characterizing women’s fertility: a data-driven approach. Horm Behav. 2016;81:74–83.
Ivey ME, Bardwick JM. Patterns of affective fluctuation in the menstrual cycle. Psychosom Med. 1968;30:336–345.
• Rieder JK, Kleshchova O, Weierich MR. Trauma characteristics moderate the relation between estradiol and trauma-related symptoms. J Trauma Stress. 2022;35:246–56. Assessed the relationship between menstrual phase, oestradiol levels and trauma symptoms in high-functioning trauma-exposed people, including use of ecological momentary assessment and biological stress reactivity.
Lane T, Francis A. Premenstrual symptomatology, locus of control, anxiety and depression in women with normal menstrual cycles. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2003;6:127–38.
Haigh EAP, Craner JR, Sigmon ST, Yoon KL, Thorpe GL. Symptom attributions across the menstrual cycle in women with panic disorder. Journal of Rational - Emotive and Cognitive - Behavior Therapy. 2018;36:320–32.
Shayani DR, Arditte Hall KA, Isley BC, Rohan KJ, Zvolensky MJ, Nillni YI. The role of health anxiety in the experience of perceived stress across the menstrual cycle. Anxiety Stress Coping. Routledge; 2020;33:706–15.
Harvey AT, Hitchcock CL, Prior JC. Ovulation disturbances and mood across the menstrual cycles of healthy women. J Psychosom Obstet Gynaecol. 2009;30:207–14.
Rodríguez-Landa JF, Guillén-Ruiz G, Hernández-López F, Cueto-Escobedo J, Rivadeneyra-Domínguez E, Bernal-Morales B, et al. Chrysin reduces anxiety-like behavior through actions on GABA(A) receptors during metestrus-diestrus in the rat. Behav Brain Res. 2021;397: 112952.
Fernandez-Guasti A, Picazo O. The actions of diazepam and serotonergic anxiolytics vary according to the gender and the estrous cycle phase. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1990;37:77–81.
Bitran D, Dowd JA. Ovarian steroids modify the behavioral and neurochemical responses of the central benzodiazepine receptor. Psychopharmacology. 1996;125:65–73.
Mora S, Dussaubat N, Diaz-Veliz G. Effects of the estrous cycle and ovarian hormones on behavioral indices of anxiety in female rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1996;21:609–20.
Díaz-Véliz G, Alarcón T, Espinoza C, Dussaubat N, Mora S. Ketanserin and anxiety levels: influence of gender, estrous cycle, ovariectomy and ovarian hormones in female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;58:637–42.
Frye CA, Petralia SM, Rhodes ME. Estrous cycle and sex differences in performance on anxiety tasks coincide with increases in hippocampal progesterone and 3alpha,5alpha-THP. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2000;67:587–96.
Marcondes FK, Miguel KJ, Melo LL, Spadari-Bratfisch RC. Estrous cycle influences the response of female rats in the elevated plus-maze test. Physiol Behav. 2001;74:435–40.
Frye CA, Walf AA. Changes in progesterone metabolites in the hippocampus can modulate open field and forced swim test behavior of proestrous rats. Horm Behav. 2002;41:306–15.
Zuluaga MJ, Agrati D, Pereira M, Uriarte N, Fernandez-Guasti A, Ferreira A. Experimental anxiety in the black and white model in cycling, pregnant and lactating rats. Physiol Behav. 2005;84:279–86.
Schneider T, Popik P. Attenuation of estrous cycle-dependent marble burying in female rats by acute treatment with progesterone and antidepressants. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:651–9.
Walf AA, Frye CA. Administration of estrogen receptor beta-specific selective estrogen receptor modulators to the hippocampus decrease anxiety and depressive behavior of ovariectomized rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86:407–14.
Frye CA, Rhodes ME. Infusions of 3alpha,5alpha-THP to the VTA enhance exploratory, anti-anxiety, social, and sexual behavior and increase levels of 3alpha,5alpha-THP in midbrain, hippocampus, diencephalon, and cortex of female rats. Behav Brain Res. 2008;187:88–99.
Molina-Hernandez M, Tellez-Alcantara NP, Olivera-Lopez JI, Jaramillo MT. Estrous cycle variation in anxiolytic-like effects of topiramate in Wistar rats in two animal models of anxiety-like behavior. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2013;103:631–6.
Sayin A, Derinoz O, Yuksel N, Sahin S, Bolay H. The effects of the estrus cycle and citalopram on anxiety-like behaviors and c-fos expression in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2014;124:180–7.
D’Souza D, Sadananda M. Estrous cycle phase-dependent changes in anxiety- and depression-like profiles in the late adolescent Wistar-Kyoto rat. Ann Neurosci ed. 2017;24:136–45.
• Guillén-Ruiz G, Cueto-Escobedo J, Hernández-López F, Rivera-Aburto LE, Herrera-Huerta EV, Rodríguez-Landa JF. Estrous cycle modulates the anxiogenic effects of caffeine in the elevated plus maze and light/dark box in female rats. Behav Brain Res. 2021;413:113469. Tests the same female rats in a battery of unlearned fear tests (EPM, LDB) and a control locomotor test to show that the oestrous cycle impacts anxiety-like behaviour.
• Miller CK, Halbing AA, Patisaul HB, Meitzen J. Interactions of the estrous cycle, novelty, and light on female and male rat open field locomotor and anxiety-related behaviors. Physiol Behav. 2021;228: 113203. Uses a within-subjects design to examine oestrous effects on the OFT in female rats.
Pentkowski NS, Litvin Y, Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ. Effects of estrus cycle stage on defensive behavior in female Long-Evans hooded rats. Physiol Behav. 2018;194:41–7.
• Pidoplichko VI, Aroniadou-Anderjaska V, Figueiredo TH, Wilbraham C, Braga MFM. Increased inhibitory activity in the basolateral amygdala and decreased anxiety during estrus: a potential role for ASIC1a channels. Brain Res. 2021;1770: 147628. Tests different female rats in various unlearned fear tasks (OFT, LDB, acoustic startle response) and shows that the oestrous cycle impacts anxiety-like behaviour on all tasks.
Nomikos GG, Spyraki C. Influence of oestrogen on spontaneous and diazepam-induced exploration of rats in an elevated plus maze. Neuropharmacology. 1988;27:691–6.
Byrnes EM, Bridges RS. Reproductive experience alters anxiety-like behavior in the female rat. Horm Behav. 2006;50:70–6.
Hiroi R, Neumaier JF. Differential effects of ovarian steroids on anxiety versus fear as measured by open field test and fear-potentiated startle. Behav Brain Res. 2006;166:93–100.
Soares-Rachetti Vde P, de Sousa Pinto IA, Santos RO, Andre E, Gavioli EC, Lovick T. Short term, low dose fluoxetine blocks estrous cycle-linked changes in responsiveness to diazepam in female rats. J Psychopharmacol. 2016;30:1062–8.
Scholl JL, Afzal A, Fox LC, Watt MJ, Forster GL. Sex differences in anxiety-like behaviors in rats. Physiol Behav. 2019;211.
Börchers S, Krieger JP, Asker M, Maric I, Skibicka KP. Commonly-used rodent tests of anxiety-like behavior lack predictive validity for human sex differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2022;141: 105733.
Klebanov PK, Jemmott JB. Effects of expectations and bodily sensations on self-reports of premenstrual symptoms. Psychol Women Q. 1992;16:289–310.
Maner JK, Miller SL. Hormones and social monitoring: menstrual cycle shifts in progesterone underlie women’s sensitivity to social information. Evol Hum Behav. 2014;35:9–16.
Walf AA, Frye CA. A review and update of mechanisms of estrogen in the hippocampus and amygdala for anxiety and depression behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:1097–111.
Borrow AP, Handa RJ. Estrogen Receptors Modulation of Anxiety-Like Behavior. Vitam Horm. 2017;103:27–52.
Gilfarb RA, Leuner B. GABA System modifications during periods of hormonal flux across the female lifespan. Front Behav Neurosci. 2022;16: 802530.
Schiller CE, Schmidt PJ, Rubinow DR. Allopregnanolone as a mediator of affective switching in reproductive mood disorders. Psychopharmacology. 2014;231:3557–67.
Li SH, Graham BM. Why are women so vulnerable to anxiety, trauma-related and stress-related disorders? The potential role of sex hormones. Lancet Psychiat. 2017;4:73–82.
Devall AJ, Santos JM, Fry JP, Honour JW, Brandão ML, Lovick TA. Elevation of brain allopregnanolone rather than 5-HT release by short term, low dose fluoxetine treatment prevents the estrous cycle-linked increase in stress sensitivity in female rats. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;25:113–23.
Machado Figueiredo R, de Carvalho MC, Brandão ML, Lovick TA. Short-term, low-dose fluoxetine prevents oestrous cycle-linked increase in anxiety-like behaviour in female rats. J Psychopharmacol. 2019;33:548–57.
Lovick TA, Griffiths JL, Dunn SM, Martin IL. Changes in GABA(A) receptor subunit expression in the midbrain during the oestrous cycle in Wistar rats. Neuroscience. 2005;131:397–405.
File SE. One-trial tolerance to the anxiolytic effects of chlordiazepoxide in the plus-maze. Psychopharmacology. 1990;100:281–2.
Rodgers RJ, Lee C, Shepherd JK. Effects of diazepam on behavioural and antinociceptive responses to the elevated plus-maze in male mice depend upon treatment regimen and prior maze experience. Psychopharmacology. 1992;106:102–10.
Schrader AJ, Taylor RM, Lowery-Gionta EG, Moore NLT. Repeated elevated plus maze trials as a measure for tracking within-subjects behavioral performance in rats (Rattus norvegicus). PLoS ONE. 2018;13: e0207804.
Dubey N, Hoffman JF, Schuebel K, Yuan Q, Martinez PE, Nieman LK, et al. The ESC/E(Z) complex, an effector of response to ovarian steroids, manifests an intrinsic difference in cells from women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2017;22:1172–84.
Ramos A. Animal models of anxiety: do I need multiple tests? Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2008;29:493–8.
Fernandes C, González MI, Wilson CA, File SE. Factor analysis shows that female rat behaviour is characterized primarily by activity, male rats are driven by sex and anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1999;64:731–8.
Cohen H, Matar MA, Joseph Z. Animal models of post-traumatic stress disorder. Curr Protoc Neurosci. 2013;Chapter 9:Unit 9.45.
Walf AA, Frye CA. The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of anxiety-related behavior in rodents. Nat Protoc. 2007;2:322–8.
Armbruster D, Grage T, Kirschbaum C, Strobel A. Processing emotions: effects of menstrual cycle phase and premenstrual symptoms on the startle reflex, facial EMG and heart rate. Behav Brain Res. 2018;351:178–87.
Armbruster D, Strobel A, Kirschbaum C, Brocke B. The impact of sex and menstrual cycle on the acoustic startle response. Behav Brain Res. 2014;274:326–33.
Epperson CN, Pittman B, Czarkowski KA, et al. Luteal-phase accentuation of acoustic startle response in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:2190–8.
Hantsoo L, Grillon C, Sammel M, et al. Response to sertraline is associated with reduction in anxiety-potentiated startle in premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Psychopharmacology. 2021;238:2985–97.
Grillon C. Models and mechanisms of anxiety: evidence from startle studies. Psychopharmacology. 2008;199:421–37.
Biedermann S v, Biedermann DG, Wenzlaff F, Kurjak T, Nouri S, Auer MK, et al. An elevated plus-maze in mixed reality for studying human anxiety-related behavior. BMC Biol. 2017;15:125.
• Nouri S, Biedermann S v, Joue G, Auer MK, Sommer T, Fuss J. Effects of circulating estradiol on physiological, behavioural, and subjective correlates of anxiety: a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2022;138:105682. Tests humans on a virtual reality EPM to examine the impact of oestradiol on physiological, behavioural and subjective measures of anxiety.
Velasco ER, Florido A, Milad MR, Andero R. Sex differences in fear extinction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;103:81–108.
Hantsoo L, Golden CEM, Kornfield S, et al. Startling differences: using the acoustic startle response to study sex differences and neurosteroids in affective disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2018;20:40.
Bosch OJ. Maternal nurturing is dependent on her innate anxiety: the behavioral roles of brain oxytocin and vasopressin. Horm Behav. 2011;59:202–12.
Booher WC, Hall LA, Thomas AL, Merhroff EA, Reyes Martínez GJ, Scanlon KE, et al. Anxiety-related defensive behavioral responses in mice selectively bred for high and low activity. Genes Brain Behav. 2021;20: e12730.
Acknowledgements
Preparation of this manuscript was supported by an Australian Research Council Discovery Project (DP220101339) to BMG.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by CAUL and its Member Institutions.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
This article is part of the Topical Collection on Reproductive Psychiatry and Women's Health
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Pestana, J.E., Islam, N., Van der Eyk, N.L. et al. What Pre-clinical Rat Models Can Tell Us About Anxiety Across the Menstrual Cycle in Healthy and Clinically Anxious Humans. Curr Psychiatry Rep 24, 697–707 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-022-01376-7
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-022-01376-7