Skip to main content
Log in

Training reading fluency among poor readers of German: many ways to the goal

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Outcomes of two training programs aimed at improving reading speed for 39 German-speaking poor readers in grades 2 and 4 were evaluated. During a 6-week training period, a specific target for children in a computer group was to improve reading of word-initial consonant clusters by practice in associating an orthographic unit with a corresponding phonological unit. Children in a paired reading group read books with an adult tutor. The results showed that, in reading words in which the computer-trained sublexical items were included, both groups exhibited similar improvement. A post hoc analysis suggested that computer training was associated with better reading skills with respect to the trained sublexical items; however, this improvement did not show large generalization effects to the words with the sublexical items. The paired reading group showed a more rapid gain in global word reading fluency than the computer group. Neither of the groups improved their pseudoword reading.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The reading speed of two children was not as slow as the inclusion criterion (one child had percentiles below the 45th and 55th, and the other one had a reading speed of below the 25th in only one subtest), but they made a considerable number of errors and were included in the participant group.

References

  • Baker, S., Gersten, R., & Keating, T. (2000). When less may be more: A 2-year longitudinal evaluation of a volunteer tutoring program requiring minimal training. Reading Research Quarterly, 35, 494–519. doi:10.1598/RRQ.35.4.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berends, I., & Reitsma, P. (2006). Addressing semantics promotes the development of reading fluency. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 247–265. doi:10.1017/S0142716406060279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M., & Treiman, R. (1990). Phonological awareness and spelling in normal children and dyslexics: the case of initial consonant clusters. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 156–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G. (1993). Text reading and rereading: determinants of fluency beyond word recognition. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 133–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bulheller, S., & Häcker, H. (2002). Coloured progressive matrices—Deutsche Bearbeitung und Norminierung [German adaptation and norms]. Frankfurt: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Compton, D. L., Olson, R. K., DeFries, J. C., & Pennington, B. F. (2002). Comparing the relations among two different versions of alphanumeric rapid automatized naming and word reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 343–368. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0604_03.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2002). Effects of phonological abilities and linguistic comprehension on the development of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 51–77. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0601_03.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Filippo, G., de Luca, M., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Lexicality and stimulus length effects in Italian dyslexics: Role of the overadditivity effect. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 141–149. doi:10.1080/09297040500346571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erlbaum, B., Vaughan, S., Hughes, M. T., & Moody, S. M. (2000). How effective are the one-to-one tutoring programs in reading for the elementary students at risk for reading failure? A meta-analysis of the intervention research. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 605–619. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harm, M. W., McCandliss, B., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2003). Modeling the successes and failures of interventions for disabled readers. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 155–182. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0702_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hintikka, S., Aro, M., & Lyytinen, H. (2005). Computerized training of the correspondences between phonological and orthographic units. Written Language and Literacy, 8, 155–178. doi:10.1075/wll.8.2.07hin.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hintikka, S., Landerl, K., Aro, M., & Lyytinen, H. (2008). Training reading fluency: is it important to practice reading aloud and is generalization possible? Annals of Dyslexia, 58, 59–79. doi:10.1007/s11881-008-0012-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holopainen, L., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Predicting delay in reading achievement in a highly transparent language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 401–413. doi:10.1177/002221940103400502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Judica, A., De Luca, M., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2002). Training of developmental surface dyslexia improves reading performance and shortens eye fixation duration in reading. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12, 177–198. doi:10.1080/09602010244000002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kame’enui, E. J., & Simmons, D. C. (Eds.) (2001). The role of fluency in reading competence, assessment, and instruction: fluency at the intersection of accuracy and speed. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 203–210, special issue. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_1

  • Klicpera, C., & Schabmann, A. (1993). Do German-speaking children have a chance to overcome reading and spelling difficulties? A longitudinal survey from the second until the eighth grade. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8, 307–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 5, 831–843. doi:10.1038/nrn1533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, M. R., & Stahl, S. A. (2003). Fluency: a review of developmental and remedial practices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 3–21. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(74)90015-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., & Moser, E. (2006). Lesepartner: evaluierung eines 1:1 Tutoring Systems zur Verbesserung der Leseleistungen. Heilpädagogische Forschung, 32, 27–38. Paired reading: evaluation of a 1:1 tutoring system to improve reading skills.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., & Willburger, E. (2008). Der Ein-Minuten Leseflüssigkeitstest—ein Verfahren zur Diagnose der Leistung im Wort- und Pseudowortlesen [One-minute reading fluency test: A method to evaluate word and pseudoword reading performance]. In W. Lenhard, W. Schneider, & H. Marx (Eds.), Tests und trends 2008. Jahrbuch der pädagogisch-psychologischen Diagnostik. Diagnose und Förderung von Leseleistungen und Leseverständnis. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An eight-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 150–161. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315–334. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00005-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Moser, E. (2006). Salzburger Lese- und Rechtschreibtest [Salzburg Reading and Spelling Test] (2nd ed.). Bern: Hans Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landi, N., Perfetti, C. A., Bolger, D. J., Dunlap, S., & Foorman, B. R. (2006). The role of discourse context in developing word form representations: A paradoxical relation between reading and learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 114–133. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.12.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lemoine, H. E., Levy, B. A., & Hutchinson, A. (1993). Increasing the naming speed of poor readers: Representations formed across repetitions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 297–328. doi:10.1006/jecp.1993.1018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B. A. (2001). Moving the bottom: Improving reading fluency. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain (pp. 357–379). Timonium, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B. A., Bourassa, D. C., & Horn, C. (1999). Fast and slow namers: Benefits of segmentation and whole word training. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 115–138. doi:10.1006/jecp.1999.2497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W., & Steinbach, K. A. (1997). The effectiveness of remedial programs for reading disabled children of different ages: Does the benefit decrease for older children? Learning Disability Quarterly, 20, 189–210. doi:10.2307/1511308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W., Warren-Chaplin, P. M., Ransby, M. J., & Borden, S. L. (1990). Training word recognition skills of reading disabled children: treatment and transfer effects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 769–780. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.4.769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, H., Ronimus, M., Alanko, A., Poikkeus, A.-M., & Taanila, M. (2007). Early identification of dyslexia and the use of computer game-based practice to support reading acquisition. Nordic Psychology, 59, 109–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magnan, A., Ecalle, J., Veuillet, E., & Collet, L. (2004). The effects of an audio-visual training program in dyslexic children. Dyslexia (Chichester, England), 10, 131–140. doi:10.1002/dys.270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martens, V. E. G., & de Jong, P. F. (2006). The effect of visual word features on the acquisition of orthographic knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 337–356. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.11.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin-Chang, S. L., & Levy, B. A. (2005). Fluency transfer: Differential gains in reading speed and accuracy following isolated word and context training. Reading and Writing, 18, 343–376. doi:10.1007/s11145-005-0668-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin-Chang, S. L., Levy, B. A., & O’Neil, S. (2007). Word acquisition, retention, and transfer: Findings from contextual and isolated word training. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 37–56. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2006.08.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayringer, H., & Wimmer, H. (2003). SLS 1–4: Das Salzburger Lese-Screening für die Klassenstufen 1–4 [Salzburg reading-screening for grade 1–4]. Bern: Verlag Hans Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, R., & Lyon, E. (1979). “Paired Reading”: A preliminary report on a technique for parental tuition of reading-retarded children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 20, 151–160. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1979.tb00495.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, R. E., White, A., & Swanson, H. L. (2007). Repeated reading versus continuous reading: influences on reading fluency and comprehension. Exceptional Children, 74, 31–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 145–173). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma, P. (1983). Printed word learning in beginning readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36, 321–339. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(83)90036-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W., Hülsmann, J., Seidler, T., & Remschmidt, H. (2001). Das Marburger Rechtschreib-Training—Ergebnisse einer Kurzzeit-Intervention. Zeitschrift fur Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 29, 7–15. Marburg spelling training: Results of a short-term intervention. doi:10.1024//1422-4917.29.1.7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 95–129. doi:10.1006/jecp.1998.2481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shany, M. T., & Biemiller, A. (1995). Assisted reading practice: Effects on performance for poor readers in Grades 3 and 4. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 382–395. doi:10.2307/747622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2005). Dyslexia (specific reading disability). Biological Psychiatry, 1301–1309. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.043

  • Snowling, M. J. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Research, 43, 219–234. doi:10.1007/BF00309831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spinelli, D., de Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Mancini, M., Martelli, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2005). Length effect in word naming latencies: Role of reading experience and reading deficit. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 217–235. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2702_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–406. doi:10.1598/RRQ.21.4.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stock, C., Marx, P., & Schneider, W. (2003). Basiskompetenzen für Lese- und Rechtschreibleistungen, BAKO 1–4 [Basic competencies for reading and writing skills]. Göttingen: Beltz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tewes, U., Rossmann, P., & Schallberger, U. (Eds.) (2000). HAWIK-III: Hamburg-Wechsler—Intelligenztest für Kinder—Dritte Auflage [Hamburg–Wechsler—Intelligence test for children—third edition]. Bern: Huber.

  • Thaler, V., Ebner, E. M., Wimmer, H., & Landerl, K. (2004). Training reading fluency in dysfluent readers with high reading accuracy: word specific effects but low transfer to untrained words. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 89–113. doi:10.1007/s11881-004-0005-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J., & Davis, C. (1996). Individual difference variables that predict response to training in phonological awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 1–21. doi:10.1006/jecp.1996.0040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J., Rashotte, C., & Alexander, A. W. (2001). Principles of fluency instruction in reading: Relationships with established empirical outcomes. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain (pp. 333–355). Timonium, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Rose, E., Lindamood, P., Conway, T., et al. (1999). Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities: group and individual responses to instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 579–593. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.4.579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1991). Children’s spelling errors on syllable-initial consonant clusters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 346–360. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.83.3.346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Leij, A., & van Daal, V. H. P. (1999). Automatization aspects of dyslexia: Speed limitations in word identification, sensitivity to increasing task demands, and orthographic compensation. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 417–428. doi:10.1177/002221949903200507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. M., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 45, 2–40. doi:10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wentink, W. M. H., van Bon, W. H. J., & Schreuder, R. (1997). Training poor readers’ phonological decoding skills: Evidence for syllable bound processing. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 9, 163–192. doi:10.1023/A:1007921805360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Mayringer, H. (2002). Dysfluent reading in the absence of spelling difficulties: A specific disability in regular orthographies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 272–277. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (1998). Poor reading: A deficit in skill automatization or a phonological deficit? Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 321–340. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0204_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexia. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 211–239. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yap, R., & van der Leij, A. (1993). Word processing in dyslexics: An automatic decoding deficit? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 261–279. doi:10.1007/BF01027391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquistion, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., My-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169–193. doi:10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00139-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Judica, A., Orlandi, M., & Spinelli, D. (1999). Markers of developmental surface dyslexia in a language (Italian) with high grapheme–phoneme correspondence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 191–216. doi:10.1017/S0142716499002027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This paper is part of the first author’s doctoral thesis. This study was supported by the Finnish Center of Excellence Program of the Academy of Finland (44858 and 213486), the Finnish Graduate School of Psychology, Niilo Mäki Foundation, and Finland’s Slot Machine Association. The paired reading training was funded by the Austrian Ministry of Education. We tender our thanks to Minna Torppa for valuable comments on the first version of the manuscript and to Michael Freeman for polishing the language. We would also like to thank the schools and children who participated in the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sini Huemer (née Hintikka).

Appendix

Appendix

Training material of the computer group:

fla

fle

fli

flo

flu

flä

flö

flü

flau

flei

fleu

kra

kre

kri

kro

kru

krä

krö

krü

krau

krei

kreu

schla

schle

schli

schlo

schlu

schlä

schlö

schlü

schlau

schlei

schleu

stra

stre

stri

stro

stru

strä

strö

strü

strau

strei

streu

Words with the consonant clusters:

Flamme

Flöte

Kraft

Kranz

Schlag

schließen

strahlen

Strick

flau

Fluss

Kragen

Krähe

Schlauch

schlimm

Straße

Stroh

Fleck

flüchten

Kran

Kreide

schlecken

Schlitten

Strauch

strömen

Fliegen

flüstern

krank

Krug

Schleife

Schloss

streiten

 

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Huemer (née Hintikka), S., Landerl, K., Aro, M. et al. Training reading fluency among poor readers of German: many ways to the goal. Ann. of Dyslexia 58, 115–137 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-008-0017-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-008-0017-2

Keywords

Navigation