Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Second language learning and reading with the additional load of dyslexia

  • Part III Cross-Linguistic Aspects Of Literacy Development And Dyslexia
  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Students speaking a language at home, which is different from the instructional at home, often face serious literacy problems in their additional language. This article examines the various factors involved in these difficulties and proposes some procedures for determining whether the reading problems are primarily due to linguistic factors, sociocultural factors, or specific learning problems such as dyslexia. The discussion of linguistic factors includes a closer examination of the concept of language proficiency. Dimensions of individual differences in the ability of second language (L2) acquisition are specified. Particular emphasis is given to the role of verbal working memory in L2 acquisition and to neuropsychological aspects. A popular target for recent research has been the orthographic structure of the language, and a general assumption has been that transparent orthographies, such as Italian or Finnish, are easier for a learner to deal with than deep orthographies. The methodological problems involved in the study of this issue are discussed. Some recent large-scale comparative surveys of reading literacy have not been able to demonstrate the impact of orthographic structures, as other, more powerful explanations of achievement variations have masked the possibly small orthographic effects. These explanatory factors are to a large extent related to social and cultural conditions in homes, communities, and schools. A tentative model is proposed where the various factors involved in L2 reading are specified, providing guidelines for assessment strategies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Agloti, S., Beltramello, A., Girardi, F., & Fabbro, F. (1996). Neurolinguistic and follow-up study of an unusual pattern of recovery from bilingual subcortical aphasia. Brain, 119, 1551–1564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anglin, J. M. (1989). Vocabulary growth and the knowing-learning distinction. Reading Canada, 7, 142–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • August, D., & Hakuta, K. (Eds.). (1997). Improving schooling for language-minority children: A research agenda. Washington DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bull, T. (1985). Lesing og barns talemål. Oslo: Novus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll. J. (1981). Twenty-five years of research on foreign language aptitude. In K. C. Diller (Ed.), Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude (pp. 83–118). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (1992). Human cognitive abilities. A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collier, V. P. (1988). The effect of age on acquisition of a second language for school. Forum, 2, Winter 1987/88.

  • Corson, D. (1995). Using English words. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1984). Bilingualism and special education: Issues in assessment and pedagogy. Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diaz, R. M. (1983). The impact of bilingualism on cognitive development. In E. W. Gordon (Ed.), Review of research in education (Vol.10, pp. 23–54). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elley, W. B. (Ed.). (1994). The IEA study of reading literacy: Achievement and instruction in thirty-two school systems. Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitzgerald, J. (1995). English-as-a-second-language learners’ cognitive reading processes: A review of research in the United States. Review of Educational Research, 65, 145–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francis, W. S. (1999). Cognitive integration of language and memory in bilinguals: Semantic representations. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 193–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederickson, N., & Frith, U. (1998). Identifying dyslexia in bilingual children: A phonological approach with inner London Sylheti speakers. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 4, 119–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganschow, L., Sparks, R., & Schneider, E. (1995). Learning a foreign language: Challenges for students with language learning difficulties. Dyslexia, 1, 75–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, R.C. (1985). Social psychology and second language learning: The role of attitudes and motivation. London: Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, S., Reyes, M., & Varnhagen, C. (1984). Understanding fables in first and second languages. NABE Journal, 3, 35–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (2000). Phonological representations, reading development and dyslexia: Towards a cross-linguistic theoretical framework. Dyslexia. An International Journal of Research and Practice, 6, 133–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hakuta, K. (1987). Degree of bilingualism and cognitive ability in mainland Puerto Rican children. Child Development, 58, 1372–1388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Høien, T., & Lundberg, I. (2000). Dyslexia. From theory to intervention. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoosain, R. (1992). Different cerebral lateralization of Chinese-English bilingual functions. In R. J. Harris (Ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals (pp. 561–571). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johansson, E. (1987). Literacy campaigns in Sweden. In R. J. Arnove & H. Graff (Eds.), National literacy campaigns. Historical and comparative perspectives (pp. 65–98). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, H. S., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K. M., & Hirsch, J. (1997). Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388, 171–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, D., Zatorre, R. J., Milneer, B., & Meyer, E. (1994). Left putaminal activation when speaking a second language: Evidence from PET. NeuroReport, 5(17), 2295–2297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: a German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K. (1999). What can we learn from dyslexia in Chinese? In I. Lundberg, F. E. Tönnessen & I. Austad (Eds.), Dyslexia: Advances in theory and practice (pp. 117–139). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E. F. (1975). Leading questions and eye witness report. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 560–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I. (1991). Reading as an individual and social skill. In I. Lundberg & T. Hoien (Eds.), Literacy in a world of change. Stavanger: Center for Reading Research/UNESCO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I. (1999). Towards a sharper definition of dyslexia. In I. Lundberg, F.E. Tönnessen, & I. Austad (Eds.), Dyslexia: Advances in theory and practice (pp. 9–29). Dordrecht, NL: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, B. (1985). Second-language acquisition in childhood: Vol.2. School-age children. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller Guron, L. (2001). Multilingual students - Multilingual sources of reading difficulty. Poster presented at the BDA international conference in York, England, April 2001.

  • Miller-Guron, L., & Lundberg, I. (1999). Dyslexia and second language reading: A second bite at the apple. Reading and Writing. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 41–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller Guron, L., & Lundberg, I. (2002). Identifying dyslexia in multilingual students: Can core phonological deficits be identified through the majority language? Journal of Reading Behaviour (in press).

  • Obler, L., Zatorre, R. J., Galloway, L., & Vaid, J. (1982). Cerebral lateralization in bilinguals: methodological issues. Brain and Language, 15, 40–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OECD (2000). Literacy in the information age. Final report of the international adult literacy survey. Paris: OECD.

    Google Scholar 

  • OECD (2001). Knowledge and skills for life - First results from PISA 2000. Paris: OECD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ojemann, G. A. (1989). Some brain mechanisms for reading. In C von Euler, I. Lundberg, & G. Lennerstrand (Eds.), Brain and reading (pp. 47–59). Hampshire, UK: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Österberg, T. (1961). Bilingualism and the first school language. Uppsala: Almqvist & Wiksell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1995). Verbal short-term memory and vocabulary learning in polyglots. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 98–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paulesu, E., McCrory, E., Fazio, F., Menoncello, L., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S. F., Cotelli, M., Cossu, G., Corte, F., Lorusso, M., Pesenti, S., Gallagher, A., Perani, D., Price, C., Frith, C. D., and Frith, U. (2000). A cultural effect on brain function. Nature, Neuroscience, 3, 91–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peer, L., & Reid, G. (Eds.). (2000). Multilingualism, literacy and dyslexia. A challenge for educators. London: Fulton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romaine, S. (1995). Bilingualism. Second edition. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory, and foreign language learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 21–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, J. S., Garcia, L. A., & Robles, B. E. (1997). Cross-language postevent misinformation effects across languages in Spanish-English bilinguals. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 889–899.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. (1987). Beyond conversation: Second language learners’ acquisition of description and explanation. In J. Lantolf & A. Labarca (Eds.), Research in second language learning: Focus on the classroom (pp. 3–16). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., & Ganschow, L. (1991). Foreign language learning difficulties: Affective or native language aptitude differences? Modern Language Journal, 75, 3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Svensson, I., Lundberg, I., & Jacobson, C. (2001). The prevalence of reading and spelling difficulties among inmates of institutions for compulsory care of juvenile delinquents. Dyslexia. An International Journal of Research and Practice, 7, 62–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taube. K. (1996). Invandrarbarns läsfärdighet. (Reading skills among immigrant children in Stockholm). Stockholm: Skolförvaltningen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, D. A., Spratt, J. E., & Ezzaki, A. (1989). Does learning to read in a second language always put the child at a disadvantage?: Some evidence from Morocco. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 31–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiggen, G. (1992). Rettskrivingsstudier 2. Kvalitativ og kvantitativ analyse av rettskrivingsavvik hos östnorske barneskoleelever. Vol 1–2. Oslo: Novus Forlag.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ingvar Lundberg.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lundberg, I. Second language learning and reading with the additional load of dyslexia. Ann. of Dyslexia 52, 165–187 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-002-0011-z

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-002-0011-z

Keywords

Navigation