Our goal for Study 1 is to place the CSII concept within a conceptual framework and test it with an empirical survey. We present the theoretical background, conceptual model, hypotheses, and results next.
Theoretical background
The most widely used measure of CSII is that developed by Bearden et al. (1989, 1990), which distinguishes between susceptibility to informational influences (SII) and susceptibility to normative influences (SNI). The former reflects a person’s tendency to accept information from others as credible evidence about reality (Deutsch and Gerard 1955). Informational influence results from actively requesting information from knowledgeable others or passively observing others (Park and Lessig 1977). It operates through the process of internalization, which occurs if information from others increases an individual’s knowledge about some aspect of the environment. Informational influence is driven by a desire to form accurate interpretations about reality in order to make more informed decisions and behave correctly (Cialdini and Goldstein 2004). SNI reflects an individual’s tendency to comply with the positive expectations of others (Deutsch and Gerard 1955: 629) and is driven by an individual’s desire to enhance one’s self-image by association with a reference group or because individuals want to achieve rewards or avoid punishments mediated by others.
Previous studies relate the CSII scale to several consumer factors, including demographics, general psychographic traits, domain-specific knowledge, personal values, and situational factors (Batra et al. 2001; Bearden et al. 1989, 1990; Bearden and Rose 1990; Clark and Goldsmith 2005; D’Rozario and Choudhury 2000; Lascu et al. 1995; Mangleburg et al. 2004). The aim of Study 1 is not to provide an exhaustive list of potential correlates of investors’ susceptibility to interpersonal influence, but rather to place this concept within a theoretical framework by incorporating its primary antecedents and consequences.
Conceptual model
As we indicate in Fig. 1, Study 1 uses a hierarchical trait model in which general traits and dispositions causally precede more situation-specific traits, which then influence a larger set of secondary traits and actions (cf. Batra et al. 2001). In our conceptual model, susceptibility to interpersonal influence acts as a situation-specific trait, because it represents a “weak trait” (McGuire 1968: 1132) with low intercorrelations across situations and behaviors (Batra et al. 2001). A person’s influenceability often appears as a consequence of personality variables, such as self-esteem or social anxiety (Batra et al. 2001; Bearden et al. 1990; McGuire 1968). Anxiety originating from a lack of knowledge or perceptions of psychological or social risk therefore drives a person’s influenceability. Moreover, personal values, such as social needs, are assumed to be important antecedents or causes of situation-specific traits such as someone’s susceptibility to interpersonal influence (Batra et al. 2001). Therefore, we argue that consumers’ general traits and dispositions such as their investment-related knowledge, perceptions of social and psychological risk, and strength of socially oriented needs determine the more specific and situation-dependent trait of susceptibility to interpersonal influences, which leads to even more specific outcomes or behaviors, such as transaction frequency.
The dimensions of CSII in an investment context thus can be explained by motivations to increase the accuracy of investment decisions by building knowledge through social interactions, decrease the potential for social embarrassment or psychological discomfort by learning about and conforming to socially accepted behavior, and fulfill social needs by creating and maintaining valuable relationships.
Hypotheses
Antecedents of consumers’ susceptibility to interpersonal influence
Domain-specific knowledge
In an investment context, domain-specific knowledge relates to consumers’ familiarity with and expertise in making investment decisions (cf. Alba and Hutchinson 1987). The amount of domain-specific knowledge determines product decisions (Alba and Hutchinson 1987) and negatively influences susceptibility to both informational and normative interpersonal influence (Furse et al. 1984; Gilly et al. 1998; Mangleburg et al. 2004).
Individuals with more knowledge tend to be more confident about making correct decisions and demonstrate less interest in others’ information and opinions (Bearden et al. 1990; Clark and Goldsmith 2006; Kahle 1995; Locander and Hermann 1979). That is, knowledgeable consumers depend less on others to obtain relevant product information, but they also depend less on others with respect to the normative aspects of decision making. They tend to be more self-confident and have higher self-esteem, which relates negatively to the need for social approval (Cox and Bauer 1964). Hence, more knowledgeable consumers should be less susceptible to both informational and normative interpersonal influences.
Conversely, individuals with less knowledge are more susceptible to informational and normative interpersonal influences. On the one hand, those with less knowledge may doubt their ability to make good decisions and perceive higher risks (Alba and Hutchinson 1987), so to reduce this risk, they may feel more compelled to ask knowledgeable others for advice and rely more strongly on this advice compared with those consumers who know more and perceive less risk (Festinger 1954; Furse et al. 1984; Gilly et al. 1998; Mangleburg et al. 2004; Mitchell and McGoldrick 1996). On the other hand, individuals with less knowledge lack a sense of personal adequacy and may suffer low self-confidence and self-esteem, which often makes them excessively fearful of social disapproval and strongly motivated to conform to others’ demands or suggestions (Janis 1954; Cox and Bauer 1964). Their compliance can be interpreted as a defensive form of behavior that permits these consumers to agree with everyone in an attempt to guarantee that nobody will be displeased with them (Janis 1955).
-
H1.
The amount of consumers’ investment-related knowledge is negatively associated with their susceptibility to informational influences when making investment decisions.
-
H2.
The amount of consumers’ investment-related knowledge is negatively associated with their susceptibility to normative influences when making investment decisions.
Psychological and social risk
In addition to product or performance risk, consumers may also experience social and psychological risk that discourages them from engaging in behaviors that are not accepted by their reference group or that conflict with their personality or self-image (Cialdini and Goldstein 2004; Sirgy 1982, 1985). Beyond the possibility of losing money (product or performance risk), consumers may be anxious that their peers will not accept their investment choices or that they will embarrass themselves in public or “not fit in” with specific reference groups (social risk). Moreover, consumers run the risk of experiencing psychological discomfort or frustration if their actions are not consistent with their self-concept or self-image (psychological risk). In general, status-conscious consumers perceive greater psycho-social risks, because they are more concerned about their self-image and worried about not fitting in, which makes them more likely to be affected by interpersonal influences (O’Cass and McEwen 2004). Research into the concept of purchase pals indicates that teens frequently shop together and adjust their decisions according to the information and opinions of relevant others to reduce not only their functional (product or performance) risks but also the perceived psycho-social risks associated with purchases (Kiecker and Hartman 1993).
A recent study by Yim et al. (2007) investigates the role of multiple referents on the evaluations of services. They suggest that the evaluation of services depends on the approval of others, and in particular on how well the service fits with the self-image. They argue that higher levels of self-image congruity (i.e., services that more closely match one’s self-image) are associated with lower levels of psychological discomfort or risk (cf. Sirgy 1985) and less consideration for attractive alternatives. In a similar vein, Kleijnen et al. (2005) find that consumers with low self-image congruence are more susceptible to their surroundings than are consumers with high self-image congruence when adopting service innovations.
This discussion implies that consumers who perceive psycho-social risks may actively seek and rely on information from and the opinions of social others to reduce their risk. That is, consumers’ perception of psycho-social risks should relate to SII and SNI. First, consumers who perceive greater psycho-social risks tend to be more susceptible to informational influence, because they are motivated to build their knowledge about socially accepted behaviors and the consequences of their actions for their self-concept (Cialdini and Goldstein 2004). These consumers ask relevant others so that they may learn about the correct way to behave and avoid future social embarrassment or psychological discomfort. Therefore, psycho-social risks are positively associated with the informational dimension of interpersonal influence, operating through internalization (Burnkrant and Cousineau 1975). Second, consumers who perceive greater psycho-social risks want to fit in and seek the approval of social others by currently complying with their social norms. As such, psycho-social risks should be positively associated with the normative dimension of interpersonal influence, operating through processes of identification (maintaining a positive self-concept) and compliance (achieving social rewards) (Burnkrant and Cousineau 1975).
-
H3.
Consumers’ level of perceived psycho-social risk is positively associated with their susceptibility to informational influences when making investment decisions.
-
H4.
Consumers’ level of perceived psycho-social risk is positively associated with their susceptibility to normative influences when making investment decisions.
Social needs
Consumers engage in social interactions not only to reduce perceived purchase risk but also to fulfill their social needs. Humans are fundamentally motivated to create and maintain meaningful and rewarding social relationships (Maslow 1954). The strength of social needs differs across individuals, and just as personal values do, they guide individuals’ behavior by affecting the criteria used to evaluate actions, people, and events. Personal or human values include internal values (e.g., self-fulfillment, sense of accomplishment, self-respect) and social or external values (e.g., being well-respected, having warm relationships with others) (Batra et al. 2001). Values are motivational in nature and provide important antecedents of situation-specific predispositions, such as CSII (cf. Batra et al. 2001). Consumers’ importance of social values is positively related to SNI (Batra et al. 2001; Kropp et al. 1999).
On the basis of these arguments, we expect consumers with stronger social needs to be more susceptible to informational and normative interpersonal influences when they invest than are consumers who have less explicit social needs. Consumers with stronger social needs tend to be more open to and derive greater value from social interactions, such as investment-related interpersonal information exchanges (SII). Moreover, they prefer buying stocks that they expect their reference group to approve of or that enable them to identify with others and thus create and maintain rewarding relationships (SNI).
-
H5.
The strength of consumers’ social needs is positively associated with their susceptibility to informational influences when making investment decisions.
-
H6.
The strength of consumers’ social needs is positively associated with their susceptibility to normative influences when making investment decisions.
Consequences of consumers’ susceptibility to interpersonal influence
This study examines the effect of consumers’ susceptibility to interpersonal influence on their yearly number of investment transactions, which is an important variable for consumers and policymakers alike, because overtrading generally results in poor performance through the accumulation of transaction costs (Odean and Barber 2000). Furthermore, through commissions, consumers’ transaction frequency directly affects banks’ and brokerage firms’ profitability.
Existing literature presents mixed and inconclusive results regarding the possible effect of CSII on transaction frequency (Mangleburg et al. 2004). Prior research finds a negative relationship between CSII and willingness to adopt new products, which may relate to hesitation to trade. Individuals who are more susceptible to interpersonal influences are less willing to make an adoption decision until a majority of relevant others also supports the new concept (Clark and Goldsmith 2006; Steenkamp and Gielens 2003). Conversely, we can assume a positive relationship between CSII and transaction frequency, because consumers who are susceptible to interpersonal influence are more receptive to environmental cues and more likely to act on this social information. By either conforming to or deviating from this new information, they may trade more frequently.
On the basis of social comparison theory (Festinger 1954), we hypothesize a negative relationship between SII and transaction frequency. That is, consumers susceptible to informational influences may—after requesting and receiving information from other, more knowledgeable people—receive reinforcement of their belief that they have insufficient knowledge to make well-informed investment decisions or obtain contradictory information. They can limit the impact of wrong decisions by minimizing their transactions, and they might be informed by others that a buy-and-hold strategy is best.
We hypothesize a positive relationship between SNI and transaction frequency, because consumers who are susceptible to normative influence are more likely to get carried away by others’ opinions and may transact to reinforce their social bonds or comply with others’ expectations. Then, SNI may lead to chameleon-like changes in response to any new source of persuasive influence (Janis 1955), which suggests more transactions.
-
H7.
Consumers’ susceptibility to informational influence is negatively associated with the number of investment transactions they make.
-
H8.
Consumers’ susceptibility to normative influence is positively associated with the number of investment transactions they make.
Finally, we expect a direct relationship between consumers’ level of investment-related knowledge and the number of investment transactions. More knowledgeable consumers have more expertise and are more familiar with investing (cf. Alba and Hutchinson 1987), which likely is associated with lower perceptions of risk, easier access to financial markets, more involvement with the product category, and a higher likelihood to transact.
-
H9.
Consumers’ level of investment-related knowledge is positively associated with the number of investment transactions they make.
Research approach: Study 1
Method
We used a survey approach to collect data from individual investors and test the hypotheses of our conceptual model (Fig. 1). We developed and tested an online questionnaire among 78 undergraduate and graduate students. After revising the questionnaire wording and layout, we asked a panel of five academics to confirm whether the items closely resembled the intended constructs. Next, we collected the empirical data through an online questionnaire that targeted visitors to four investment-related Web sites, selected because they attempt to attract respondents with various levels of experience and backgrounds. The call to participate provided a summary of the purpose of the study and a link to the online questionnaire. We carefully checked the final sample for duplicates using respondents’ IP address and contact details. Furthermore, we told respondents that their responses would remain anonymous and that all data would be treated confidentially.
Sample
The net sample consisted of 287 investors with an average age of 53 years (SD = 13). Twelve percent of the respondents are women, and more than two-thirds have at least a college degree. These respondents note a considerable length of investing experience (M = 16 years, SD = 11) and, on average, transact 77 times per year (SD = 122, median = 30). Almost all respondents (98%) invest for their own accounts. The dominant purchasing channels that these investors use are as follows: 56% online brokers, 36% banks, 4% direct telephone order lines, and 4% expert advice. The average portfolio size is €207,000 (approximately $310,500) with a median of €70,000 (approximately $105,000). Among the sample, 20% belong to an investment club.
To investigate potential selection bias, we compared these sample characteristics with the characteristics of the general population of investors with direct investments in the Dutch stock market (VEB 2002). Our respondents are slightly older (53 years compared with 48 years) and more likely to be men (88% compared with 71%). The modal portfolio size, however, equals that of the general investment population, at €50,000. Also, the median portfolio size closely corresponds with estimates from the Dutch National Bank (2006) that show an average portfolio size per investing household of €70,000. The respondents’ transaction frequency also matches another Dutch sample (Bauer et al. 2007). In conclusion, the sample appears similar to the overall population of Dutch investors with regard to selected background characteristics, though we also note that our sample consists of fairly experienced investors.
Measures and research instrument
All items use five- or seven-point Likert scales to measure the relevant constructs (Table 1). Respondents’ susceptibility to interpersonal influence employs the CSII scale proposed by Bearden et al. (1989, 1990). The SII measure includes all four items of the original scale, whereas the SNI measure for our study drops one of the original eight items because it explicitly refers to purchasing the latest fashion. This item was potentially distracting for our respondents and irrelevant to the investment context.
Table 1 Measurement model results of Study 1
Following Bloch et al. (1989), we measure domain-specific knowledge with self-reported measures. The two items measuring psychological and social risk come directly from work by Kaplan et al. (1974). To measure respondents’ strength of social needs, we use items from Cheek and Buss’s (1981) sociability study.
We employ standard psychometric procedures to test the reliability and validity of the scales (Netemeyer et al. 2003; Nunnally and Bernstein 1994). After performing reliability analyses, we test the validity of the constructs with confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) using AMOS 7 (see Table 1). As a result, we remove two items from the SNI scale that indicated low loadings (<0.50).
The final measurement model using maximum likelihood estimation demonstrates acceptable fit (χ
2/df = 1.68, GFI = 0.94, CFI = 0.96, RFI = 0.89, TLI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.049).Footnote 1 We also find evidence of convergent validity and unidimensionality, because each item loads significantly (p < 0.001) on its assigned factor and reveals insignificant cross-loadings. Next, the average variance extracted (AVE) of all constructs was greater than 0.50 with the exception of the SII scale, which falls marginally below the required level (Fornell and Larcker 1981). To establish discriminant validity, we first note that the intercorrelations between the latent factors (± two standard errors) do not include unity (Anderson and Gerbing 1988) (Table 2). Furthermore, the AVE of each latent construct is greater than the squared correlations between any set of two constructs (Fornell and Larcker 1981). Finally, the composite reliabilities range from 0.74 to 0.88, which indicate high levels of construct reliability (Bagozzi and Yi 1988). These analyses demonstrate that our scales have sufficient levels of reliability, unidimensionality, convergent validity, and discriminant validity.
Table 2 Construct correlations and AVE of Study 1
Results
We depict the structural model of Study 1 and the significance of its relationships in Fig. 2. The data confirm all hypotheses (H1–H9). The structural model fits the data well (χ
2/df = 2.00, GFI = 0.92, CFI = 0.94, RFI = 0.87, TLI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.059) and explains a reasonable amount of the variance in SNI (R
2 = 36.6%) and SII (R
2 = 38.1%). The antecedents account for 9.9% of the variance of consumers’ transaction frequency. The maximum variable inflation factor (VIF) for each independent variable in a set of regression analyses is 1.35, which suggests some but not strong multicollinearity.Footnote 2
Consistent with previous studies in other contexts (Gilly et al. 1998; Mangleburg et al. 2004; Park and Lessig 1977), we find that consumers’ level of domain-specific knowledge is negatively associated with both SII (H1: β
= −0.19, p
= 0.005) and SNI (H2: β
= −0.16, p
= 0.010) in an investment context. The results also support the proposed positive associations between psycho-social risk and SII (H3: β
= 0.25, p < 0.001) and SNI (H4: β
= 0.37, p < 0.001). Furthermore, consumers’ strength of social needs appears strongly positively related to both SII (H5: β
= 0.40, p < 0.001) and SNI (H6: β
= 0.51, p < 0.001).
Since the dimensions of SII and SNI are conceptually different and operate through different mechanisms with different goals (e.g., Cialdini and Trost 1998), the relative strength of the antecedents may differ across these dimensions. In particular, SII relates more strongly to personal values, such as self-fulfillment and self-respect, because it is driven by the motivations to learn and increase the accuracy of investment decisions, whereas SNI appeals to social values, such as having warm relationships with others and achieving a sense of belonging, which are inherently linked to the motivations to fulfill socially oriented needs (Batra et al. 2001). Therefore, we expect knowledge to relate more strongly to SII than to SNI, and social needs should relate more strongly to SNI than to SII. We used the procedure suggested by Chin (2000) to test the differences between these relationships by analyzing the differences in the strength of the unstandardized path coefficients. The results confirm our expectations. Knowledge has a stronger influence on SII than on SNI \(\left( {B_{{\text{know}} \to {\text{SII}}} = - 0.22\,{\text{vs}}{\text{.}}\,B_{{\text{know}} \to {\text{SNI}}} = - 0.13,t = 16.4,p <0.001} \right)\), and social needs have a more pronounced effect on SNI than on SII \(\left( {B_{{\text{socneeds}} \to {\text{SNI}}} = 0.48\,{\text{vs}}{\text{.}}\,B_{{\text{socneeds}} \to {\text{SII}}} = 0.26,t = 39.3,p <0.001} \right)\).
We also find support that SII is negatively associated (H7: β = −0.22, p = 0.002) and SNI is moderately positively associated (H8: β = 0.15, p = 0.030) with the number of transactions. Because these results contradict findings by Mangleburg et al. (2004), who find that SII positively and SNI negatively influences teens’ shopping frequency with friends, we call for replication studies that can help generalize these results.
Finally, we find that knowledge (H9: β = 0.22, p < 0.001) is positively associated with transaction frequency.