Abstract
The two hemispheres of the human brain differ functionally and structurally. Despite over a century of research, the extent to which brain asymmetry is influenced by sex, handedness, age, and genetic factors is still controversial. Here we present the largest ever analysis of subcortical brain asymmetries, in a harmonized multi-site study using meta-analysis methods. Volumetric asymmetry of seven subcortical structures was assessed in 15,847 MRI scans from 52 datasets worldwide. There were sex differences in the asymmetry of the globus pallidus and putamen. Heritability estimates, derived from 1170 subjects belonging to 71 extended pedigrees, revealed that additive genetic factors influenced the asymmetry of these two structures and that of the hippocampus and thalamus. Handedness had no detectable effect on subcortical asymmetries, even in this unprecedented sample size, but the asymmetry of the putamen varied with age. Genetic drivers of asymmetry in the hippocampus, thalamus and basal ganglia may affect variability in human cognition, including susceptibility to psychiatric disorders.
Introduction
Left–right differentiation of the central nervous system (CNS) results in anatomical, functional, and behavioral asymmetries in many organisms (Ocklenburg and Gunturkun 2012). Humans are no exception: functions including language, visuospatial cognition, and hand–motor control are asymmetrically organized between hemispheres in a typical human brain (Haaland and Harrington 1996; Mellet et al. 2014). At the population level, these asymmetries show clear directional biases, or lateralizations (Bryden 1982). Handedness is the most overt example: around 90 % of people have a right-hand preference, a strong bias not seen in other species including our closest evolutionary relatives, the apes (Hopkins et al. 2011).
Functional and structural lateralization of the human brain may be influenced by left-right differences in gene expression (Francks 2015), as recently demonstrated in language-related regions of the adult superior temporal cortex (Karlebach and Francks 2015). Even so, lateralization varies markedly across individuals. Women and men show average differences in asymmetry, as well. Men show, on average, more pronounced asymmetries in superior temporal language regions of the cerebral cortex than women, based on brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data from over 3000 people (Guadalupe et al. 2015). Genes involved in steroid hormone biology may affect the degree of lateralization in both men and women (Guadalupe et al. 2015). Another trait linked to cerebral lateralization is handedness (Willems et al. 2014): the largest study of cerebral cortical structural differences by handedness showed weak associations with changes in surface area of the left precentral sulcus (Guadalupe et al. 2014a), consistent with prior reports (Amunts et al. 1996; Foundas et al. 1998). Left-handers have a slightly higher incidence of atypical functional hemispheric language dominance (Mazoyer et al. 2014). Alterations of cerebral cortical lateralization have also been linked to cognitive and psychiatric disorders , including language-related impairments (Altarelli et al. 2014; Herbert et al. 2005), autism (Eyler et al. 2012; Herbert et al. 2005), schizophrenia (SCZ; Oertel-Knochel et al. 2012), and substance-use disorders (Balconi and Finocchiaro 2015).
In contrast to the cerebral cortex, lateralizations of human subcortical structures and the hippocampus have not been well studied, nor the factors that might affect their individual differences or roles in lateralized cognition. Most investigations have been in clinical contexts, where differences between cases and controls in asymmetry patterns of subcortical structures have been linked to various neuropsychiatric disorders. For example, abnormal asymmetries in the basal ganglia, particularly of the globus pallidus and caudate nucleus, have been observed in cases of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Hynd et al. 1993), and in developmental stuttering and Tourette’s syndrome (TS; Foundas et al. 2013; Singer et al. 1993). Abnormal asymmetry of the striatum has been linked to prenatal alcohol or methamphetamine exposure (Roos et al. 2014; Willford et al. 2010). Changes in thalamic asymmetry have been found in cases of TS (Lee et al. 2006) and SCZ (Zhou et al. 2003). Regarding limbic system structures, studies of major depression (Xia et al. 2004), obsessive-compulsive disorder (Szeszko et al. 1999), SCZ (Niemann et al. 2000), anorexia nervosa (Titova et al. 2013), and age-related memory impairment (Soininen et al. 1994) have shown abnormal asymmetries of the hippocampus, which in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy also included the amygdala (Cendes et al. 1993). Abnormal asymmetries of the amygdala have also been reported in SCZ (Niu et al. 2004) and in cocaine addiction (Makris et al. 2004). Some of these disorders differ in their prevalence between sexes and by handedness (Castellanos et al. 2001; DeLisi et al. 2002; Niemann et al. 2000). Interestingly, sex differences in subcortical asymmetries have been suggested to have an etiological role in TS (Zimmerman et al. 2000) and SCZ (Niu et al. 2004). These findings suggest that, in addition to the more salient cerebral cortical asymmetries, asymmetries of the subcortical nuclei also play a role in brain health and disease.
Despite these intriguing initial findings with respect to disease states, decades of research have failed to answer definitively how brain asymmetries in the healthy population are linked to basic biological factors such as age, sex, and handedness. This is partly because many brain asymmetries and their normal variability are subtle, and difficult to measure reliably in small studies (tens to low hundreds of subjects are typical). Regarding sexual dimorphisms, a sex difference in asymmetry of the amygdala has been reported (Niu et al. 2004), while no sex difference was detected in another study (Szabo et al. 2001). For striatal asymmetry, no significant sex differences were observed by three studies (Abedelahi et al. 2013; Giedd et al. 1996; Wyciszkiewicz and Pawlak 2014), although a sex difference in putamen asymmetry was suggested to affect TS etiology (Zimmerman et al. 2000). Sexual dimorphism in thalamic asymmetry has been recently reported (Kang et al. 2015) but not replicated. Asymmetry of striatal nuclei changes with age (Abedelahi et al. 2013; Yamashita et al. 2011), but prior studies of subcortical structures have tended to look at age and asymmetry as separate aspects of study (Caviness et al. 1996; Giedd et al. 1996). Left-handedness has not been robustly investigated in relation to subcortical asymmetries, as there are so few left-handers in most datasets (Foundas et al. 1998; Kloppel et al. 2007). Likewise, in clinical studies, possible effects of sex, age, and handedness have not often been investigated, either as a result of restricted inclusion criteria, or otherwise not considering these factors in their analyses (e.g. Kang et al. 2015; Yamashita et al. 2011).
The present study was the first by the Lateralization working-group embedded within the ENIGMA (Enhancing Imaging Genetics through Meta-Analysis) Consortium (Thompson et al. 2014). Our goal was to detect effects of sex, handedness, and age on the normal variability in subcortical asymmetries, through a harmonized multi-site study using meta-analysis methods, based on 52 healthy control and population-based datasets which comprised a total of 15,847 participants. All brain magnetic resonance (MR) images were analyzed using a single, uniform protocol, despite inevitable heterogeneity in image acquisition (Hibar et al. 2015; Stein et al. 2012). This study was by two orders of magnitude the largest ever of asymmetry with respect to subcortical structures of the human brain, and factors affecting its variability. This allowed us to establish subtle but definitive findings of sex and age-related effects on some of the structures, where previously the literature has been inconsistent and contradictory (see Discussion). We also measured the heritabilities of subcortical and hippocampal asymmetries in a large family dataset, as previous studies have suggested these to be partially heritable (Eyler et al. 2014; Hulshoff Pol et al. 2006; Renteria 2013). This heritability screen is a valuable precursor to future genome-wide association studies of laterality in brain traits, as well as identifying genetic overlap between asymmetries and cognitive or psychiatric disorders.
Methods
Datasets
The participating sites were members of the Lateralization working-group within the ENIGMA Consortium (Thompson et al. 2014), who contributed data from 52 independent samples to this study comprising a total of 15,847 healthy participants (7524 males and 8323 females). Samples were drawn from the general population or comprised healthy controls from clinical studies. Table 1 and Supplemental Information S1 summarize the datasets’ sample sizes and age distributions. Each dataset and its image acquisition protocols are described in Supplemental Information S2.
Handedness of participants was known for a subset of the overall sample. The method of assessment varied per dataset (see Supplemental Information S3). An ambidextrous category was not included and only datasets with enough left-handers to perform statistical comparisons were considered. In total, 959 and 11,236 subjects were left- and right-handed, respectively.
The final numbers of subjects and datasets that were used for meta-analyses differed per test and structure according to the availability of covariate and structure-specific volumetric information, and the minimum sample-size criteria. Details are given below per analysis.
Image acquisition and subcortical segmentation
Image acquisition and subcortical volume measurement has been described in previous reports from the ENIGMA Consortium (e.g. (Hibar et al. 2015; Stein et al. 2012) , and is consistent enough to detect SNP effects at a genome-wide significant level, which individually account for less than 1 % of the variance in structure volumes. To summarize, T1-weighted brain structural MRI scans were acquired at multiple different sites using scanners of mostly 1.5 or 3 Tesla field strengths. One dataset (QTIM) was acquired with a 4 Tesla field strength scanner. See Supplemental Information S2 for detailed information on manufacturers and image acquisition parameters per dataset. All sites followed the same protocol for segmentation of subcortical structures, volume computation, and quality control. The protocol is downloadable from the ENIGMA website (http://enigma.ini.usc.edu/protocols/imaging-protocols/). Specifically, image pre-processing and subcortical segmentation were done with FreeSurfer versions 4.3 through to 5.3 (Fischl et al. 2002), using the “-recon-all” pipeline and default settings. This pipeline performs automated bias field correction, spatial normalization, skull stripping, and segments brain tissue into cortical gray/white matter, as well as into several non-cortical tissues. This resulted in volume estimates for the following seven bilaterally paired structures: nucleus accumbens, amygdala, caudate nucleus, globus pallidus, hippocampus, putamen, and thalamus, and estimates of total intracranial volume (ICV).
Quality control was performed separately by each of the contributing sites, and followed the harmonized protocol developed by the ENIGMA consortium (http://enigma.ini.usc.edu/protocols/imaging-protocols/). The protocol consisted of visually checking individual images, plotted from a set of axial slices. Volume estimates derived from poorly segmented structures (i.e. where tissue labels were assigned incorrectly) were excluded from each site’s datasets and subsequent analyses. In addition, a number of checks were performed to assess potential errors in the left-right orientation of the data (see Supplemental Information S4 for details).
Within-dataset analyses
For each dataset and each of the seven bilaterally paired structures, the volumetric asymmetries, descriptive and statistical analyses were computed at each participating site using a single script in R (R Development Core Team; 2012), on table-formatted data. Asymmetry Indices (AI) were defined as the relative volume difference between the left and right structure in relation to its total bilateral volume: (Left - Right)/(Left + Right). To exclude possible outliers in volumes or AIs we used an adaptive SD threshold (SDThresh) depending on each dataset’s sample size (N < 150 ⇒ SDThresh = 2.5; 150 ≥ N ≥ 1000 ⇒ SDThresh = 3; N > 1000 ⇒ SDThresh = 3.5). Statistical tests were run on the seven subcortical AIs separately. Differences between sexes or handedness groups were assessed by Welch’s two-sample t-test, to avoid assuming balanced group sizes and equal variances (Ruxton 2006). Tests were performed on residualised AIs, after removing effects of age and ICV (and sex for the handedness tests) by linear regression. Possible non-linear effects of ICV were investigated using the BIG sample but found to be negligible (Supplemental Information S5), hence all analyses were performed using only linear correction for this covariate. The effects of age on AIs were estimated by ANCOVAs, modelled together with sex and ICV as covariate factors.
This approach supported the subsequent application of meta-analysis methodology, through using within-site summary statistics, and without sites needing to share primary data.
AI heterogeneity between datasets
For each of the seven AIs we assessed heterogeneity due to dataset differences through analyses of variance, with ‘dataset’ and ‘FreeSurfer version’ as the main factors. For this analysis we re-computed the total AI variance for a given structure and partitioned it into between-and within-'dataset’ contributions, and between- and within-'FreeSurfer version’ contributions. This allowed us to calculate estimates of eta-squared (η2), i.e., the percentage of the total variance explained by each factor. Given that individual sites ran their analyses on one version of FreeSurfer only, we computed main effects but not their potential interactions.
Meta-analyses
We combined the test statistics obtained across datasets by means of random-effect meta-analyses (Borenstein et al. 2010). This method calculates and tests the significance of a pooled effect while weighting each dataset’s contribution to the overall effect by the inverse of its error variance. In contrast to a fixed-effect meta-analysis test, this method also takes into account the amount of variability present between effects from different studies in its calculation, and hence does not make strong assumptions regarding equal effects underlying all datasets (Borenstein et al. 2010).
For meta-analyses of sex and handedness effects, we used the mean group differences in residualised AIs and recomputed the standard errors from 95 % confidence intervals and degrees of freedom generated by the Welch’s two sample t-tests. For meta-analyses of age-effects, we used the coefficients from the ANCOVA regressions of AIs on age, and their corresponding standard errors.
Including results based on too few observations is likely to reduce reliability, therefore we chose to test with a cut-off of 15 observations per group and include assessments of fail-safe N’s for any significant finding (at a corrected alpha of P = 0.007). The method used was Rosenberg’s (Rosenberg 2005), which takes into account the weighted nature of the meta-analysis test, and its outcome can be interpreted as the number of studies averaging null-results which would be needed to render the observed p-value non-significant (P >= 0.007). Furthermore, effect heterogeneity was assessed by Cochran’s Q and the complementary Higgins’ I2, which both assess the contribution of dataset differences to the observed pooled effect. All tests were performed in R using the ‘metafor’ library (Viechtbauer 2010).
For the sex group comparisons, a 15-observation minimum threshold resulted in totals between 6867 and 6962 males versus 7708 to 7897 females, depending on the specific structure. For handedness, the totals were from 644 to 668 left handers versus 7298 to 7667 right handers. For meta-analyses of age-effects we applied the threshold of at least 15 observations per sex group and included an extra criterion based on the age-range of each dataset. Only results from datasets with a minimum 5-year range between their 1st and 3rd quartile (50 % of the dataset) were included.
To assess the pattern of statistically significant age effects across the lifespan, we performed a post-hoc weighted meta-regression of the age coefficients from each dataset on the corresponding median ages. Datasets were weighted by the square root of their corresponding sample size. The same criterion for dataset inclusion was used as described above.
Population-level lateralization
T-scores and corresponding P-values were calculated for the difference between the mean AI and zero (i.e. the point of volumetric symmetry) for each structure and dataset, separately by sex. These were combined to assess population-level lateralizations for each structure, separately for each sex.
Heritability of AIs
We estimated the heritability of volumetric asymmetries using the Genetics of Brain Structure (GOBS) dataset (McKay et al. 2014; Mitchell et al. 1996). This analysis included data from 1170 subjects of Mexican-American ancestry, belonging to 71 extended pedigrees. Heritability estimates were derived from variance-component analysis (Almasy and Blangero 1998). The method partitions the observed phenotypic variance into sub-components based on the relationship structures within the families, in order to estimate the proportion of overall phenotypic variance due to additive genetic effects. To calculate this family-based heritability estimate, the method requires large pedigrees and accurate kinship estimates between family members. For a more detailed description of the approach, applied to brain imaging measures, see (Chouinard-Decorte et al. 2014; Koran et al. 2014). These analyses were performed using SOLAR (Almasy and Blangero 1998) including age, sex, and ICV as covariates. For each of the seven structures we estimated the heritability of the AI and the heritability of the phenotypic correlation (i.e. genetic correlation) between left and right volumes. Lastly, we also assessed the phenotypic and genetic correlations across all seven AIs.
Results
AI heterogeneity between datasets
We observed notable heterogeneity in the AI distributions across datasets (Table 2 and Supplemental Information S6). Except for the hippocampus and putamen, dataset heterogeneity explained over 10 % (η2 > 0.1) of the total observed variance per structure. Likewise, heterogeneity attributable to different versions of FreeSurfer was also substantial, with η2 > 0.1 for AI’s of the nucleus accumbens, globus pallidus and thalamus.
Independent of dataset mean differences, the nucleus accumbens showed the most variable AI estimates, and the caudate nucleus was the least variable (see Table 2). The average variability around AI means, as a proportion of bilateral volume (σ2 within*100), was 7.8 % for the nucleus accumbens and 2.5 % for the caudate nucleus. All structures showed highly significant mean lateralization, as well as consistency in mean direction of lateralization between the sexes (see Table 2 and Fig. 1, as well as Supplemental Information S7).
Meta-analysis of group differences by sex and handedness
After adjusting the significance threshold to P = 0.007 for multiple testing of seven structures, meta-analyses showed significant differences in AIs between males and females for the globus pallidus and putamen (Table 3 and Fig. 2), corrected for covariate effects of age and intracranial volume (ICV) within datasets. The direction of the sex difference for the putamen was negative (see Table 3), indicating a lower AI in males versus females, i.e. a rightwards shift in asymmetry in males. The opposite was found for the globus pallidus, where a leftward shift in AI was observed in males relative to females. Table 3 also reports the results of the study-heterogeneity and fail-safe N estimations. We observed no significant heterogeneity in sex effects across datasets for putamen and globus pallidus. A nominally significant sex effect (uncorrected P = 0.02) was also found for the hippocampus. Meta-analyses of handedness effects on AIs showed no significant group differences (uncorrected P-values > 0.1, results not shown).
Forest plots of the mean sex differences in AIs per dataset, for the structures that showed significant sex effects in meta-analysis. For each structure, the datasets are ordered top-to-bottom by their estimated sex difference. The identities of the datasets are given by the numbers in the left-hand columns, with reference to Table 1. The size of a square is proportional to the weights assigned in meta-analysis. The confidence intervals are shown, as well as dashed vertical lines to indicate the point of no mean sex difference
Meta-analysis of age effects on AIs
After adjusting the significance threshold to P = 0.007 for multiple testing over seven structures, meta-analysis revealed a significant effect of age on the AI of the putamen (see Table 4 and Fig. 3a), corrected for covariate effects of sex and ICV within datasets. A positive pooled effect for the putamen indicated increasingly leftward shifts in asymmetry with increasing age. Table 4 also reports the results of the study-heterogeneity tests. While the heterogeneity tests for age effects on putamen AI were statistically significant, the estimate of a fail-safe N (n = 85) suggested that a biasing influence of study heterogeneity on the results was unlikely.
Results from meta-analysis of age effects. a Forest plot of the age coefficients for each dataset on putamen AI. The datasets are ordered top-to-bottom by their estimated age coefficient. The identities of the datasets are given by the numbers in the left-hand columns, with reference to Table 1. The size of a square is proportional to the weights assigned in meta-analysis. The confidence intervals are also depicted, as well as dashed vertical lines to indicate the point of an age coefficient with value zero. b Plot of the weighted regression of the age coefficients on each sample’s median age. The dotted line represents the best linear fit (P = 0.03). The size of a point is proportional to the square-root of a dataset’s sample size
In our post-hoc analysis of age effects, by means of weighted meta-regressions, the putamen showed effects that changed across the median ages of our samples. We found larger effects of age in the younger datasets, compared to the older datasets (see Fig. 3).
Heritability of AIs
AIs of the globus pallidus, hippocampus, putamen, and thalamus showed modest but statistically significant heritabilities, ranging from h2 = 0.15 to 0.27 (using a corrected alpha of P = 0.007; Table 5). For each subcortical region, we also estimated the genetic correlation (the proportion of variance that two traits share due to the additive effects of genes) between the absolute volumes of the left and right structures. While these correlations were all high (indicating partial pleiotropy), most were significantly different from 1 (i.e., complete pleiotropy; see Table 5). In other words, most genetic effects on volume variation are shared between the left and right hemispheres and therefore affect bilateral volumes of these structures, but some independent or quantitatively different genetic effects may operate uniquely on each hemisphere, thus constituting heritable effects on asymmetry. The nucleus accumbens also showed a suggestively significant heritability of its AI using an uncorrected alpha of 0.05 (see Table 5).
Pairwise phenotypic and genetic correlations, assessed in the GOBS sample, are shown in Supplemental Information S8. Many of the phenotypic correlations were significant, but only the AIs of the putamen and thalamus showed a nominally significant genetic correlation (r = −0.48, uncorrected P = 0.037) in the presence of a significant phenotypic correlation (r = −0.26, P = 8.26*10−23). In other words, there may be genetic variability which affects these two AIs in opposite directions.
Discussion
Establishing effects of age, sex, and genetics
There is an inconsistent literature regarding basic biological factors that may affect subcortical and hippocampal asymmetries, including age, handedness, and sex. Subcortical asymmetries are subtle compared to some cerebral cortical asymmetries, and have so far only been assessed in small sample sizes, often with different analysis methods across studies (see Introduction). Compared to prior reports on subcortical asymmetries, our study analyzed a large number of datasets worldwide using a harmonized protocol and meta-analysis methods. To our knowledge, this was by far the largest ever study of healthy variation in any aspect of human brain asymmetry. The 52 datasets had technical and demographic differences, which appeared to influence the levels of asymmetry measured, but in this respect were representative of the heterogeneity that exists across cohorts worldwide. Given the scale of our study, and in contrast to literature-based meta-analyses, ours was not affected by publication bias nor by spurious results from underpowered studies. For future genome-wide screens, we also revealed significant heritabilities of asymmetries in a family sample.
We found reliable sex differences in asymmetries of the globus pallidus and putamen which, together with the hippocampus and thalamus, were also the most strongly heritable asymmetries among the seven structures analyzed. With increasing age, there were changes in the mean asymmetry of the putamen. Handedness was not detectably related to any subcortical asymmetry. The ENIGMA Consortium (Thompson et al. 2014) plans future genome-wide association studies in sample sizes comparable to, or greater than, that used here. Our data show which subcortical asymmetries are heritable and suitable for detecting subtle modulatory effects and group differences. Taken together, our heritability- and meta-analyses indicate that asymmetries of the putamen, globus pallidus, hippocampus and thalamus are the most likely structures through which genetic variation may impact lateralization for human cognition, its variability, and susceptibility to brain disorders.
From a developmental perspective, some human CNS lateralizations change throughout life (Kovalev et al. 2003). Asymmetries are detectable during fetal gestation behaviorally (Hepper 2013) and anatomically (Corballis 2013), so differential development between the two human brain hemispheres must, at least in part, be genetically coded in utero (Francks 2015). Three prior reports have suggested genetic contributions to variability in subcortical asymmetries based on twin-based heritability estimates. One found evidence for amygdala volumes being under strong genetic control, with higher heritability for the left than the right hemisphere (h2 = 0.80 and 0.55, respectively; (Hulshoff Pol et al. 2006)). Another found that genetic contributions to left and right volume variability were partly distinct for the nucleus accumbens and globus pallidus in particular (Eyler et al. 2014). A third found significant heritabilities of asymmetry indexes for the caudate nucleus and putamen, h2 = 0.17 and 0.32, respectively (Renteria 2013).
In terms of developmental biology and molecular genetics, the best studied model organism for CNS lateralization is the zebrafish. During the zebrafish’s development, there is a left-biased migration of a midline structure (the parapineal organ) that results in differential innervation of the bilateral epithalamus into the surrounding tissue, which later affects other brain regions (Concha et al. 2009). Specific molecular contributions to this process have been identified (Colombo et al. 2013). The relevance of this mechanism to humans is not clear, but a subcortical origin of lateralized development in the zebrafish brain suggests that similar or related mechanisms may be important in our species. Cerebral cortical lateralization may even be a downstream consequence of early subcortical lateralization.
For the putamen AI, asymmetry increased with age. In particular, the observed effect of age was more pronounced in samples with younger median ages, suggesting a non-linear relationship. Environmental or age-dependent genetic factors may contribute to this increased lateralization over time. To our knowledge, these associations have not been reported before, except for an opposite age effect for the putamen in 120 healthy, young adults (Abedelahi et al. 2013). We tested only linear effects of age at the dataset level, and these coefficients were meta-analyzed. Non-linear changes in AI with age might have gone undetected in our analysis, and may affect the measured linear effects. However, these meta-analyses were restricted to age effects observed in datasets with at least a 5-year age-range between the first and third quartile of participants. Most of our datasets had median ages between 20 and 60 years, so a linear regressor would have captured main effects of age on AIs, in these datasets, even if there were subtle non-linear effects. However, these factors should be considered when interpreting the pooled effect sizes reported.
Perhaps surprisingly, handedness had no detectable effect on subcortical asymmetries. However, as there are fewer left-handers than right-handers, the effective sample size was roughly one sixth for this analysis than for our analysis of sex differences. It remains possible, through even larger-scale meta-analysis, that handedness will relate to subcortical asymmetries. However, based on our present data, such effects must be very small.
Dataset heterogeneity
Studies of subcortical structure have been greatly advanced by in vivo imaging. Even so, findings of population-level mean lateralizations of subcortical structures have been inconsistently reported. For example, there have been reports of the putamen being leftward lateralized on average (i.e. the left volume larger than the right (Giedd et al. 1996; Kang et al. 2015), as well as rightward lateralized (Abedelahi et al. 2013). Likewise the globus pallidus has been reported as leftward lateralized (Kang et al. 2015), as well as rightward lateralized (Wyciszkiewicz and Pawlak 2014). Similar discrepancies have also been found for the hippocampus (Kang et al. 2015; Niemann et al. 2000; Shi et al. 2009), amygdala (Makris et al. 2004; Niu et al. 2004; Szeszko et al. 1999) and the caudate nucleus (Abedelahi et al. 2013; Glenthoj et al. 2007; Raz et al. 1995; Vernaleken et al. 2007).
Here we used uniform image processing protocols, but our analysis showed substantial differences in mean AIs across datasets, which were partly attributable to different versions of FreeSurfer (see Table 2 and Supplemental Information S3). However, the majority of datasets (39 out of 52) were processed using version 5.3, so that our ability to assess the contributions of other FreeSurfer versions to AI variability was limited. Variability in image acquisition is likely a substantial source of dataset AI heterogeneity. The ability to distinguish different structures using MRI depends on the contrast achieved between different tissues. Subcortical structures and the surrounding tissue are often imperfectly contrasted, so that automated methods of image analysis must rely to some extent on atlas-derived information. These are often based on manual segmentations of existing datasets, which will reflect any mean asymmetries present in those datasets (Han and Fischl 2007; Patenaude et al. 2011). In addition, any subtle but uncorrected scanner magnetic field inhomogeneities may lead to geometric distortions in segmentation of brain structures (Han and Fischl 2007; Jovicich et al. 2009). These factors might bias segmentation, subtly affecting AI means. Manual segmentation does not avoid this problem, and can introduce asymmetric biases (Maltbie et al. 2012). In particular for assessing population variability (as opposed to as a diagnostic tool), automated methods clearly outperform manual segmentation in their reproducibility and feasibility for larger-scale studies (Guadalupe et al. 2014b; Morey et al. 2010).
In our study, all structures showed highly significant deviations from mean AI = 0, i.e. all showed population-level lateralization. Except for the hippocampus, the directions of significant mean AIs were in line with those reported in a study of 138 young adults, based also on subcortical volumes generated by FreeSurfer (Kang et al. 2015). However, given the caveats outlined above, we are cautious about interpreting the mean population AIs at face value. Different AI means across datasets may indicate which structures are more or less susceptible to methodological biases. The mean AIs for the hippocampus, amygdala, and putamen differed the least between datasets. The mean AI of the thalamus, on the other hand, showed the highest heterogeneity attributable to dataset heterogeneity (including FreeSurfer versions), and at the same time showed one of the strongest population-level AI lateralizations. This pattern is in line with our previous report that the hippocampus AI showed the highest scan-rescan correlation of all structures quantified with FreeSurfer (among the seven structures studied here), while the thalamus showed the second lowest scan-rescan correlation, in subjects scanned twice using varying protocols, and sometimes using different scanners with different field strengths (Guadalupe et al. 2014b).
In contrast to the substantial heterogeneity across datasets in mean AIs for some structures, there was less evidence for dataset heterogeneity in the effects of sex on mean AIs. We detected stable sex differences in AIs regardless of differences in age or ICV between and within datasets, and the sex differences were highly significant in our meta-analyses. The structures for which we detected sex differences in AIs showed L > R population-level asymmetry. For the globus pallidus this was more pronounced in males, while the opposite was observed for the putamen.
Implications for future studies
Our study underlines the utility, and indeed the necessity, of analyzing subtle subcortical asymmetries in vast samples. Regarding clinical studies, some brain disorders may be associated with larger alterations in subcortical asymmetries than variables such as sex, handedness, and age. Nonetheless future studies linking subcortical asymmetries to disorders should be better powered if they analyze larger samples than used previously. Such studies will be possible within the ENIGMA Consortium.
It is reassuring that consistent sex differences could be measured in our study, even when AI means varied across cohorts. Some AIs were also heritable, based on studying relative-pair similarities. It is therefore clear that automated segmentation methods can measure meaningful individual differences in subcortical and hippocampal volumetric asymmetries (Guadalupe et al. 2014b; Hibar et al. 2015). It follows that genome-wide association studies of subcortical and hippocampal AIs are supported by this methodology, which will require very large samples for their success (Hibar et al. 2015; Stein et al. 2012).
References
Abedelahi, A., Hasanzadeh, H., Hadizadeh, H., & Joghataie, M. T. (2013). Morphometric and volumetric study of caudate and putamen nuclei in normal individuals by MRI: effect of normal aging, gender and hemispheric differences. Polish Journal of Radiology, 78(3), 7–14.
Almasy, L., & Blangero, J. (1998). Multipoint quantitative-trait linkage analysis in general pedigrees. American Journal of Human Genetics, 62(5), 1198–1211.
Altarelli, I., Leroy, F., Monzalvo, K., Fluss, J., Billard, C., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Galaburda, A. M., & Ramus, F. (2014). Planum temporale asymmetry in developmental dyslexia: revisiting an old question. Human Brain Mapping, 35(12), 5717–5735.
Amunts, K., Schlaug, G., Schleicher, A., Steinmetz, H., Dabringhaus, A., Roland, P. E., & Zilles, K. (1996). Asymmetry in the Human Motor Cortex and Handedness. NeuroImage, 4(3), 216–222.
Balconi, M., & Finocchiaro, R. (2015). Decisional impairments in cocaine addiction, reward bias, and cortical oscillation “unbalance”. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 11, 777–786.
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., & Rothstein, H. R. (2010). A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res Synth Methods, 1(2), 97–111.
Bryden, M. P. (1982). Laterality: Functional asymmetry in the intact brain. New York: Academic Press.
Castellanos, F. X., Giedd, J. N., Berquin, P. C., Walter, J. M., Sharp, W., Tran, T., Vaituzis, A. C., Blumenthal, J. D., Nelson, J., Bastain, T. M., et al. (2001). Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(3), 289–295.
Caviness Jr., V. S., Kennedy, D. N., Richelme, C., Rademacher, J., & Filipek, P. A. (1996). The human brain age 7-11 years: a volumetric analysis based on magnetic resonance images. Cerebral Cortex, 6(5), 726–736.
Cendes, F., Leproux, F., Melanson, D., Ethier, R., Evans, A., Peters, T., & Andermann, F. (1993). MRI of amygdala and hippocampus in temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography, 17(2), 206–210.
Chouinard-Decorte, F., McKay, D. R., Reid, A., Khundrakpam, B., Zhao, L., Karama, S., Rioux, P., Sprooten, E., Knowles, E., Kent, J. W., et al. (2014). Heritable changes in regional cortical thickness with age. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 8(2), 208–216.
Colombo, A., Palma, K., Armijo, L., Mione, M., Signore, I. A., Morales, C., Guerrero, N., Meynard, M. M., Perez, R., Suazo, J., et al. (2013). Daam1a mediates asymmetric habenular morphogenesis by regulating dendritic and axonal outgrowth. Development, 140(19), 3997–4007.
Concha, M. L., Signore, I. A., & Colombo, A. (2009). Mechanisms of directional asymmetry in the zebrafish epithalamus. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology, 20(4), 498–509.
Corballis, M. C. (2013). Early signs of brain asymmetry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(11), 554–555.
DeLisi, L. E., Svetina, C., Razi, K., Shields, G., Wellman, N., & Crow, T. J. (2002). Hand preference and hand skill in families with schizophrenia. Laterality, 7(4), 321–332.
Eyler, L. T., Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2012). A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism. Brain, 135(3), 949–960.
Eyler, L. T., Vuoksimaa, E., Panizzon, M. S., Fennema-Notestine, C., Neale, M. C., Chen, C. H., Jak, A., Franz, C. E., Lyons, M. J., Thompson, W. K., et al. (2014). Conceptual and data-based investigation of genetic influences and brain asymmetry: a twin study of multiple structural phenotypes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(5), 1100–1117.
Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., van der Kouwe, A., Killiany, R., Kennedy, D., Klaveness, S., et al. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33(3), 341–355.
Foundas, A. L., Hong, K., Leonard, C. M., & Heilman, K. M. (1998). Hand preference and magnetic resonance imaging asymmetries of the central sulcus. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 11(2), 65–71.
Foundas AL, Mock JR, Cindass R, Jr., Corey DM. (2013): Atypical caudate anatomy in children who stutter. Perceptual and Motor Skills 116(2):528–543.
Francks, C. (2015). Exploring human brain lateralization with molecular genetics and genomics. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. doi:10.1111/nyas.12770.
Giedd, J. N., Snell, J. W., Lange, N., Rajapakse, J. C., Casey, B. J., Kozuch, P. L., Vaituzis, A. C., Vauss, Y. C., Hamburger, S. D., Kaysen, D., et al. (1996). Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: ages 4–18. Cerebral Cortex, 6(4), 551–560.
Glenthoj, A., Glenthoj, B. Y., Mackeprang, T., Pagsberg, A. K., Hemmingsen, R. P., Jernigan, T. L., & Baare, W. F. (2007). Basal ganglia volumes in drug-naive first-episode schizophrenia patients before and after short-term treatment with either a typical or an atypical antipsychotic drug. Psychiatry Research, 154(3), 199–208.
Guadalupe, T., Willems, R. M., Zwiers, M. P., Arias Vasquez, A., Hoogman, M., Hagoort, P., Fernandez, G., Buitelaar, J., Franke, B., Fisher, S. E., et al. (2014a). Differences in cerebral cortical anatomy of left- and right-handers. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(261). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00261.
Guadalupe, T., Zwiers, M. P., Teumer, A., Wittfeld, K., Vasquez, A. A., Hoogman, M., Hagoort, P., Fernandez, G., Buitelaar, J., Hegenscheid, K., et al. (2014b). Measurement and genetics of human subcortical and hippocampal asymmetries in large datasets. Human Brain Mapping, 35(7), 3277–3289.
Guadalupe, T., Zwiers, M. P., Wittfeld, K., Teumer, A., Vasquez, A. A., Hoogman, M., Hagoort, P., Fernandez, G., Buitelaar, J., van Bokhoven, H., et al. (2015). Asymmetry within and around the human planum temporale is sexually dimorphic and influenced by genes involved in steroid hormone receptor activity. Cortex, 62, 41–55.
Haaland, K. Y., & Harrington, D. L. (1996). Hemispheric asymmetry of movement. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6(6), 796–800.
Han, X., & Fischl, B. (2007). Atlas renormalization for improved brain MR image segmentation across scanner platforms. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 26(4), 479–486.
Hepper, P. G. (2013). The developmental origins of laterality: Fetal handedness. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(6), 588–595.
Herbert, M. R., Ziegler, D. A., Deutsch, C. K., O’Brien, L. M., Kennedy, D. N., Filipek, P. A., Bakardjiev, A. I., Hodgson, J., Takeoka, M., Makris, N., et al. (2005). Brain asymmetries in autism and developmental language disorder: a nested whole-brain analysis. Brain, 128(1), 213–226.
Hibar, D. P., Stein, J. L., Renteria, M. E., Arias-Vasquez, A., Desrivieres, S., Jahanshad, N., Toro, R., Wittfeld, K., Abramovic, L., Andersson, M., et al. (2015). Common genetic variants influence human subcortical brain structures. Nature, 520(7546), 224–229.
Hopkins, W. D., Phillips, K. A., Bania, A., Calcutt, S. E., Gardner, M., Russell, J., Schaeffer, J., Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R., & Schapiro, S. J. (2011). Hand preferences for coordinated bimanual actions in 777 great apes: implications for the evolution of handedness in hominins. Journal of Human Evolution, 60(5), 605–611.
Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Schnack, H. G., Posthuma, D., Mandl, R. C. W., Baare, W. F., van Oel, C., van Haren, N. E., Collins, D. L., Evans, A. C., Amunts, K., et al. (2006). Genetic Contributions to Human Brain Morphology and Intelligence. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(40), 10235–10242.
Hynd, G. W., Hern, K. L., Novey, E. S., Eliopulos, D., Marshall, R., Gonzalez, J. J., & Voeller, K. K. (1993). Attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder and asymmetry of the caudate nucleus. Journal of Child Neurology, 8(4), 339–347.
Jovicich, J., Czanner, S., Han, X., Salat, D., van der Kouwe, A., Quinn, B., Pacheco, J., Albert, M., Killiany, R., Blacker, D., et al. (2009). MRI-derived measurements of human subcortical, ventricular and intracranial brain volumes: Reliability effects of scan sessions, acquisition sequences, data analyses, scanner upgrade, scanner vendors and field strengths. NeuroImage, 46(1), 177–192.
Kang, X., Herron, T. J., Ettlinger, M., & Woods, D. L. (2015). Hemispheric asymmetries in cortical and subcortical anatomy. Laterality, 20(6), 6581–6584.
Karlebach, G., & Francks, C. (2015). Lateralization of gene expression in human language cortex. Cortex, 67, 30–36.
Kloppel, S., van Eimeren, T., Glauche, V., Vongerichten, A., Munchau, A., Frackowiak, R. S., Buchel, C., Weiller, C., & Siebner, H. R. (2007). The effect of handedness on cortical motor activation during simple bilateral movements. NeuroImage, 34(1), 274–280.
Koran, M. E., Thornton-Wells, T. A., Jahanshad, N., Glahn, D. C., Thompson, P. M., Blangero, J., Nichols, T. E., Kochunov, P., & Landman, B. A. (2014). Impact of family structure and common environment on heritability estimation for neuroimaging genetics studies using sequential Oligogenic linkage analysis routines. J Med Imaging (Bellingham), 1(1), 014005.
Kovalev, V. A., Kruggel, F., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2003). Gender and age effects in structural brain asymmetry as measured by MRI texture analysis. NeuroImage, 19(3), 895–905.
Lee, J. S., Yoo, S. S., Cho, S. Y., Ock, S. M., Lim, M. K., & Panych, L. P. (2006). Abnormal thalamic volume in treatment-naive boys with Tourette syndrome. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 113(1), 64–67.
Makris, N., Gasic, G. P., Seidman, L. J., Goldstein, J. M., Gastfriend, D. R., Elman, I., Albaugh, M. D., Hodge, S. M., Ziegler, D. A., Sheahan, F. S., et al. (2004). Decreased absolute amygdala volume in cocaine addicts. Neuron, 44(4), 729–740.
Maltbie, E., Bhatt, K., Paniagua, B., Smith, R. G., Graves, M. M., Mosconi, M. W., Peterson, S., White, S., Blocher, J., El-Sayed, M., et al. (2012). Asymmetric bias in user guided segmentations of brain structures. NeuroImage, 59(2), 1315–1323.
Mazoyer, B., Zago, L., Jobard, G., Crivello, F., Joliot, M., Perchey, G., Mellet, E., Petit, L., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2014). Gaussian mixture modeling of hemispheric lateralization for language in a large sample of healthy individuals balanced for handedness. PloS One, 9(6), e101165.
McKay, D. R., Knowles, E. E., Winkler, A. A., Sprooten, E., Kochunov, P., Olvera, R. L., Curran, J. E., Kent Jr., J. W., Carless, M. A., Goring, H. H., et al. (2014). Influence of age, sex and genetic factors on the human brain. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 8(2), 143–152.
Mellet, E., Jobard, G., Zago, L., Crivello, F., Petit, L., Joliot, M., Mazoyer, B., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2014). Relationships between hand laterality and verbal and spatial skills in 436 healthy adults balanced for handedness. Laterality, 7, 7.
Mitchell, B. D., Kammerer, C. M., Blangero, J., Mahaney, M. C., Rainwater, D. L., Dyke, B., Hixson, J. E., Henkel, R. D., Sharp, R. M., Comuzzie, A. G., et al. (1996). Genetic and environmental contributions to cardiovascular risk factors in Mexican Americans. The San Antonio Family Heart Study. Circulation, 94(9), 2159–2170.
Morey, R. A., Selgrade, E. S., Wagner, H. R., Huettel, S. A., Wang, L. H., & McCarthy, G. (2010). Scan-rescan reliability of subcortical brain volumes derived from automated segmentation. Human Brain Mapping, 31(11), 1751–1762.
Niemann, K., Hammers, A., Coenen, V. A., Thron, A., & Klosterkotter, J. (2000). Evidence of a smaller left hippocampus and left temporal horn in both patients with first episode schizophrenia and normal control subjects. Psychiatry Research, 99(2), 93–110.
Niu, L., Matsui, M., Zhou, S. Y., Hagino, H., Takahashi, T., Yoneyama, E., Kawasaki, Y., Suzuki, M., Seto, H., Ono, T., et al. (2004). Volume reduction of the amygdala in patients with schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Research, 132(1), 41–51.
Ocklenburg, S., & Gunturkun, O. (2012). Hemispheric asymmetries: the comparative view. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 5.
Oertel-Knochel, V., Knochel, C., Stablein, M., & Linden, D. E. (2012). Abnormal functional and structural asymmetry as biomarker for schizophrenia. Current Topics in Medicinal Chemistry, 12(21), 2434–2451.
Patenaude, B., Smith, S. M., Kennedy, D. N., & Jenkinson, M. (2011). A Bayesian model of shape and appearance for subcortical brain segmentation. NeuroImage, 56(3), 907–922.
Raz, N., Torres, I. J., & Acker, J. D. (1995). Age, gender, and hemispheric differences in human striatum: a quantitative review and new data from in vivo MRI morphometry. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 63(2), 133–142.
Renteria, ME. (2013): Mapping the genetic architecture of subcortical brain anatomy. PhD Thesis (http://espace.library.uq.edu.au/view/UQ:320790).
Roos, A., Jones, G., Howells, F. M., Stein, D. J., & Donald, K. A. (2014). Structural brain changes in prenatal methamphetamine-exposed children. Metabolic Brain Disease, 29(2), 341–349.
Rosenberg, M. S. (2005). The file-drawer problem revisited: a general weighted method for calculating fail-safe numbers in meta-analysis. Evolution, 59(2), 464–468.
Ruxton, G. D. (2006). The unequal variance t-test is an underused alternative to Student’s t-test and the Mann–Whitney U test. Behavioral Ecology, 17(4), 688–690.
Shi, F., Liu, B., Zhou, Y., Yu, C., & Jiang, T. (2009). Hippocampal volume and asymmetry in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: Meta-analyses of MRI studies. Hippocampus, 19(11), 1055–1064.
Singer, H. S., Reiss, A. L., Brown, J. E., Aylward, E. H., Shih, B., Chee, E., Harris, E. L., Reader, M. J., Chase, G. A., Bryan, R. N., et al. (1993). Volumetric MRI changes in basal ganglia of children with Tourette’s syndrome. Neurology, 43(5), 950–956.
Soininen, H. S., Partanen, K., Pitkanen, A., Vainio, P., Hanninen, T., Hallikainen, M., Koivisto, K., & Riekkinen Sr., P. J. (1994). Volumetric MRI analysis of the amygdala and the hippocampus in subjects with age-associated memory impairment: correlation to visual and verbal memory. Neurology, 44(9), 1660–1668.
Stein, J. L., Medland, S. E., Vasquez, A. A., Hibar, D. P., Senstad, R. E., Winkler, A. M., Toro, R., Appel, K., Bartecek, R., Bergmann, O., et al. (2012). Identification of common variants associated with human hippocampal and intracranial volumes. Nature Genetics, 44(5), 552–561.
Szabo, C. A., Xiong, J., Lancaster, J. L., Rainey, L., & Fox, P. (2001). Amygdalar and hippocampal volumetry in control participants: differences regarding handedness. AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 22(7), 1342–1345.
Szeszko, P. R., Robinson, D., Alvir, J. M., Bilder, R. M., Lencz, T., Ashtari, M., Wu, H., & Bogerts, B. (1999). Orbital frontal and amygdala volume reductions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(10), 913–919.
Thompson, P., Stein, J., Medland, S., Hibar, D., Vasquez, A., Renteria, M., et al. (2014). The ENIGMA Consortium: large-scale collaborative analyses of neuroimaging and genetic data. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 8(2), 153–182.
Titova, O. E., Hjorth, O. C., Schioth, H. B., & Brooks, S. J. (2013). Anorexia nervosa is linked to reduced brain structure in reward and somatosensory regions: a meta-analysis of VBM studies. BMC Psychiatry, 13, 110.
Vernaleken, I., Weibrich, C., Siessmeier, T., Buchholz, H. G., Rosch, F., Heinz, A., Cumming, P., Stoeter, P., Bartenstein, P., & Grunder, G. (2007). Asymmetry in dopamine D(2/3) receptors of caudate nucleus is lost with age. NeuroImage, 34(3), 870–878.
Viechtbauer W. (2010): Conducting Meta-Analyses in R with the metafor Package. 2010 36(3):48.
Willems, R. M., Van der Haegen, L., Fisher, S. E., & Francks, C. (2014). On the other hand: including left-handers in cognitive neuroscience and neurogenetics. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 15(3), 193–201.
Willford, J., Day, R., Aizenstein, H., & Day, N. (2010). Caudate asymmetry: a neurobiological marker of moderate prenatal alcohol exposure in young adults. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 32(6), 589–594.
Wyciszkiewicz, A., & Pawlak, M. A. (2014). Basal ganglia volumes: MR-derived reference ranges and lateralization indices for children and young adults. The Neuroradiology Journal, 27(5), 595–612.
Xia, J., Chen, J., Zhou, Y., Zhang, J., Yang, B., Xia, L., & Wang, C. (2004). Volumetric MRI analysis of the amygdala and hippocampus in subjects with major depression. Journal of Huazhong University of Science and Technology. Medical Sciences, 24(5), 500–2–50506.
Yamashita, K., Yoshiura, T., Hiwatashi, A., Noguchi, T., Togao, O., Takayama, Y., Nagao, E., Kamano, H., Hatakenaka, M., & Honda, H. (2011). Volumetric asymmetry and differential aging effect of the human caudate nucleus in normal individuals: a prospective MR imaging study. Journal of Neuroimaging, 21(1), 34–37.
Zhou, S. Y., Suzuki, M., Hagino, H., Takahashi, T., Kawasaki, Y., Nohara, S., Yamashita, I., Seto, H., & Kurachi, M. (2003). Decreased volume and increased asymmetry of the anterior limb of the internal capsule in patients with schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 54(4), 427–436.
Zimmerman, A. M., Abrams, M. T., Giuliano, J. D., Denckla, M. B., & Singer, H. S. (2000). Subcortical volumes in girls with tourette syndrome: support for a gender effect. Neurology, 54(12), 2224–2229.
Acknowledgments
Open access funding provided by Max Planck Society. The QTIM study thanks research assistants Marlene Grace, Ann Eldridge, Richard Parker, Lenore Sullivan, Lorelle Nunn, Kerrie Mcaloney, Kori Johnson, Aaron Quiggle, and Natalie Garden, radiographers Matthew Meredith, Peter Hobden, Kate Borg, Aiman Al Najjar, and Anita Burns for acquisition of the scans, and David Smyth, Anthony Conciotrorre, Daniel Park, and David Butler for IT support. The IDIVAL-PAFIP study thanks the IDIVAL Neuroimaging Unit for its help in the technical execution of this work. Both Sydney MAS and OATS thank their respective research teams. The LBC1936 study would like to thank Catherine Murray for recruitment of the participants, the radiographers and other staff at the Brain Research Imaging Centre. The Zurich OCD study thanks Reto Iannaccone for supporting the data collection. BIG would also like to thank Hans van Bokhoven for his contributions to the Cognomics initiative.
Finally, all sites within the ENIGMA-Lateralization working group are very grateful to the involved participants for their generosity of time and willingness to participate in each of the collaborating studies. All authors have no conflict of interests to declare for this study.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
ENIGMA protocol development is supported by the NIH BD2K “Big Data to Knowledge” initiative (U54 020403; PI: Paul Thompson) which is funded by a cross-NIH partnership.
The AMC OCD dataset was supported by grants from ZonMW (grant numbers: 3160007, 91676084, 31160003, 31180002, 31000056, 2812412, 100001002, 100002034), NWO (grant numbers: 90461193, 40007080, 48004004, 40003330), and grants from the Amsterdam Brain Imaging Platform, Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam and the Dutch Brain foundation. The processing with FreeSurfer was performed on the Dutch e-Science Grid through BiG Grid project and COMMIT project “e-Biobanking with imaging for healthcare”, which are funded by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO).
The Barcelona OCD study was supported by project grants no. PI09/01331, PI10/01753, PI10/01003, CP10/00604, PI13/01958 and CIBER-CB06/03/0034 from the Carlos III Health Institute, grant no. 2014SGR1672 from the Agency for Administration of University and Research (AGAUR), and a ‘Miguel Servet’ contract (CP10/00604) from the Carlos III Health Institute to Dr. Soriano-Mas.
The Brain Imaging Genetics (BIG) database was established in Nijmegen in 2007. This resource is now part of Cognomics, a joint initiative by researchers of the Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, the Human Genetics and Cognitive Neuroscience departments of the Radboud University Medical Center, and the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. The Cognomics Initiative is supported by the participating departments and centres and by external grants, i.e. the Biobanking and Biomolecular Resources Research Infrastructure (Netherlands) (BBMRI-NL), the Hersenstichting Nederland, and the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO). The research on BIG also receives funding from the European Community‘s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) under grant agreements #602450 (IMAGEMEND) and #602805 (Aggressotype) and from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Consortium grant U54 EB020403, supported by a cross-NIH alliance that funds Big Data to Knowledge Centers of Excellence. We would also like to thank Hans van Bokhoven for his contributions to the Cognomics initiative and to all persons who kindly participated in this research. In addition, AF Marquand gratefully acknowledges support from the Language in Interaction project, funded by the NWO under the Gravitation Programme (grant 024.001.006).
The CIAM and OCD-SU datasets were supported by the Medical Research Council of South Africa .
CLiNG and HMS studies were partially supported by a research grant from the Competence Network Schizophrenia to Oliver Gruber.
The FBIRN study was supported by the National Center for Research Resources at the National Institutes of Health (grant numbers: NIH 1 U24 RR021992 (Function Biomedical Informatics Research Network) and NIH 1 U24 RR025736–01 (Biomedical Informatics Research Network Coordinating Center; http://www.birncommunity.org). FBIRN data was processed by the UCI High Performance Computing cluster supported by Joseph Farran, Harry Mangalam, and Adam Brenner and the National Center for Research Resources and the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, National Institutes of Health, through Grant UL1 TR000153. FBIRN thanks Mrs. Liv McMillan for overall study coordination.
The HUBIN study was supported by the Swedish Research Council (grant numbers K2009-62X-15077-06-3 and K2012-61X-15077-09-3), the Karolinska Institutet and the Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation.
The IDIVAL-PAFIP study was supported by Instituto de Salud Carlos III, FIS 00/3095, 01/3129, PI020499, PI060507, PI10/00183, PI14/00639, the SENY Fundació Research Grant CI 2005–0308007, and the Fundación Marqués de Valdecilla API07/011. We thank IDIVAL Neuroimaging Unit for its help in the technical execution of this work.
The IMpACT study was supported by a grant from the Brain & Cognition Excellence Program and a Vici grant (to Barbara Franke) of the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO, grant numbers 433–09-229 and 016–130-669) and in part by the Netherlands Brain Foundation (grant number, 15F07[2]27)and the and BBMRI-NL (grant CP2010–33). The research leading to these results also received funding from the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) under grant agreement no. 602805 (Aggressotype), no. 278948 (TACTICS), and no. 602450 (IMAGEMEND). In addition, the project received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement no 643051 (MiND), the NIH BD2K U54 020403 grant for the ENIGMA Consortium, and from the ECNP Network ADHD across the lifespan.
IMAGEN received support from the following sources: the European Union-funded FP6 Integrated Project IMAGEN (Reinforcement-related behaviour in normal brain function and psychopathology) (LSHM-CT- 2007-037286), the FP7 projects IMAGEMEND(602450; IMAging GEnetics for MENtal Disorders), AGGRESSOTYPE (602805) and MATRICS (603016), the Innovative Medicine Initiative Project EU-AIMS (115300–2), a Medical Research Council Programme Grant “Developmental pathways into adolescent substance abuse” (93558), the Swedish funding agency FORMAS, the Medical Research Council and the Wellcome Trust (Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge), the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London, the Bundesministeriumfür Bildung und Forschung (BMBF grants 01GS08152; 01EV0711; eMED SysAlc01ZX1311A; Forschungsnetz AERIAL), the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG grants SM 80/7–1, SM 80/7–2, SFB 940/1), the National Institutes of Health, U.S.A. (Axon, Testosterone and Mental Health during Adolescence; RO1 MH085772-01 A1), and by NIH Consortium grant U54 EB020403, supported by a cross-NIH alliance that funds Big Data to Knowledge Centres of Excellence.
LBC1936: Data collection was supported by the Disconnected Mind project, funded by Age UK. J.M.W. is partly funded by the Scottish Funding Council as part of the SINAPSE Collaboration. The work was undertaken by The University of Edinburgh Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, part of the cross-council Lifelong Health and Wellbeing Initiative (MR/K026992/1). Funding from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC) and MRC is gratefully acknowledged. We thank the study participants. We also thank Catherine Murray for recruitment of the participants and the radiographers and other staff at the Brain Research Imaging Centre.
Sydney MAS and OATS were supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC)/Australian Research Council Strategic Award (Grant 401162); NHMRC Program Grants (350833, 568969) and a Project Grant (1045325). OATS was facilitated through access to the Australian Twin Registry, which is funded by the NHMRC Enabling Grant 310667. Karen Mather is supported by the NHMRC Capacity Building Grant 568940 and an Alzheimer’s Australia Dementia Research Foundation Postdoctoral Fellowship. We would like to thank the Sydney MAS and OATS participants and their respective research teams.
The MCIC study was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH/NCRR P41RR14075 and R01EB005846 to Vince D. Calhoun), the Department of Energy (DE-FG02-99ER62764), the Mind Research Network, the Morphometry BIRN (1 U24, RR021382A), the Function BIRN (U24RR021992–01, NIH.NCRR MO1 RR025758–01,1RC1MH089257 and 5P20RR021938/P20GM103472 to Vince D. Calhoun), the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (research fellowship to Stefan Ehrlich and Esther Walton), and a NARSAD Young Investigator Award (to Stefan Ehrlich).
The Münster Neuroimaging Cohort (MüNC) was supported by grants from the German Research Foundation (DFG; grant FOR 2107; DA1151/5–1 to UD) and Innovative Medizinische Forschung (IMF) of the Medical Faculty of Münster (DA120903 to UD, DA111107 to UD, and DA211012 to UD).
The NCNG study was supported by the Research Council of Norway (#154313, #177458, and #231286).
The infrastructure for the NESDA study (www.nesda.nl) is funded through the Geestkracht program of the Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development (Zon-Mw, grant number 10–000-1002) and is supported by participating universities (VU University Medical Center, GGZ inGeest, Arkin, Leiden University Medical Center, GGZ Rivierduinen, University Medical Center Groningen) and mental health care organizations, see www.nesda.nl. Lianne Schmaal is supported by The Netherlands Brain Foundation Grant number F2014(1)-24.
The NeuroIMAGE study was supported by NIH Grant R01MH62873, NWO Large Investment Grant 1750102007010, and grants from Radboud university medical center, University Medical Center Groningen and Accare, and VU University Amsterdam. This work was also supported by a grant from NWO Brain & Cognition (433–09-242). Further support was received from the European Union FP7 programmes TACTICS (278948) and IMAGEMEND (602450).
The OCD-London dataset was supported by project grant no. 064846 from the Wellcome Trust and a pilot R&D grant from the South London & Maudsley Trust, UK.
The Osaka study was partially supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 25293250 and 23659565, MEXT Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas (Comprehensive Brain Science Network) Grant Number 221S0003, and Brain/MINDS, AMED. Part of computations were performed using Research Center for Computational Science, Okazaki, Japan.
QTIM: Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (Project Grants No. 496682 and 1009064 to MJ Wright and Fellowship No. 464914 to IB Hickie), US National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (RO1HD050735 to MJ Wright), and US National Institute on Drug Abuse (R00DA023549 to NA Gillespie). Baptiste Couvy-Duchesne is supported by a University of Queensland International PhD scholarship. We are grateful to the twins for their generosity of time and willingness to participate in our studies. We thank research assistants Marlene Grace, Ann Eldridge, Richard Parker, Lenore Sullivan, Lorelle Nunn, Kerrie Mcaloney, Kori Johnson, Aaron Quiggle, and Natalie Garden, radiographers Matthew Meredith, Peter Hobden, Kate Borg, Aiman Al Najjar, and Anita Burns for acquisition of the scans, and David Smyth, Anthony Conciotrorre, Daniel Park, and David Butler for IT support.
The Study of Health in Pomerania (SHIP) is supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (grants 01ZZ9603, 01ZZ0103 and 01ZZ0403) the Ministry of Cultural Affairs as well as the Social Ministry of the Federal State of Mecklenburg-West Pomerania. MRI scans were supported by Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany. SHIP-LEGEND was supported by the German Research Foundation (GR1912/5–1).
The TCD|NUIG sample was supported by grant funding from the Health Research Board (grant number HRA_POR/2011/100; HRA_POR/2012/54), Science Foundation ireland (12/IP/1359; 08/IN.1/B1916), the Wellcome Trust (grant number 072894/2/03/Z) and the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (grant number 17026), USA.
The TOP study was supported by the Research Council of Norway (#213837, #217776, #223273), the South-East Norway Health Authority (2013–123), and the KG Jebsen Foundation.
The Zürich OCD study was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (No. 320030_130237) and the Hartmann Müller Foundation (No. 1460). We thank Reto Iannaccone for supporting the data collection. In the last 10 years, Prof. Dr. Susanne Walitza has received lecture honoraria from Janssen Cilag, AstraZeneca, Opopharma, and Eli Lilly.
All participating studies were approved by their local ethical committee. Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved. All authors have no conflict of interests to declare for this study.
Electronic supplementary material
ESM 1
(PDF 262 kb)
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
About this article
Cite this article
Guadalupe, T., Mathias, S.R., vanErp, T.G.M. et al. Human subcortical brain asymmetries in 15,847 people worldwide reveal effects of age and sex. Brain Imaging and Behavior 11, 1497–1514 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-016-9629-z
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-016-9629-z