Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Interpersonal synchrony increases social cohesion, reduces work-related stress and prevents sickdays: a longitudinal field experiment

Interpersonelle Synchronität fördert den Zusammenhalt und vermindert Stress und Krankentage: ein längsschnittliches Feldexperiment

  • Hauptbeiträge - Offener Teil
  • Published:
Gruppe. Interaktion. Organisation. Zeitschrift für Angewandte Organisationspsychologie (GIO) Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Background

This longitudinal field experiment examines synchrony to facilitate collaboration in organizations.

Objectives

Specifically, we tested if (1) the cohesion-enhancing effect of synchronous movement replicates in an organizational setting and (2) synchronous movement reduces stress on the level of the individual.

Materials and Methods

Employees of a publishing company who underwent a physical exercise intervention that involved synchronous movement for 9 weeks were compared with a treated (asynchronous movement) and a nontreated control group (no movement). Data were obtained at three measurement points.

Results

Synchronous movement enhanced social closeness, reduced work-related stress and diminished sickdays immediately following the intervention. The longer-term effects were either smaller than the short-term effects or not discernible at all.

Discussion

As the synchrony intervention was short, simple, and well accepted among employees, it is a potentially useful component of workplace health or team development initiatives.

Zusammenfassung

Hintergrund

Dieses längsschnittliche Feldexperiment untersuchte interpersonelle Synchronität in Gruppen.

Ziele

Dabei untersuchten wir, ob (1) die im Labor gezeigte zusammenhaltstiftende Wirkung synchroner Bewegung in einem organisationalen Kontext replizierbar ist und (2) synchrone Bewegung das Stresserleben von Arbeitnehmern vermindert.

Methode

Angestellte eines Verlages durchliefen ein neunwöchiges Bewegungsprogramm, welches synchrone Bewegungen beinhaltete. Diese behandelte Experimentalgruppe wurde verglichen mit einer behandelten Kontrollgruppe, die sich asynchron bewegte sowie mit einer unbehandelten Kontrollgruppe, die sich gar nicht bewegte. Es gab drei Messzeitpunkte.

Ergebnisse

Synchrone Bewegung verstärkte den Zusammenhalt zwischen den Teilnehmenden, verminderte das individuelle Erleben von Stress und reduzierte ihre Krankentage unmittelbar nach Teilnahme am Programm. Längerfristige Wirkungen der synchronen Bewegung waren entweder geringer ausgeprägt oder gar nicht feststellbar.

Diskussion

Aufgrund der Kürze des Bewegungsprogramms, seiner Einfachheit und seiner Begrüßung durch die Belegschaft stellt es potentiell eine nützliche Komponente in Maßnahmen des betrieblichen Gesundheitsmanagements oder der Teamentwicklung dar.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen, D. G., Renn, R. W., & Griffeth, R. W. (2003). The impact of telecommuting design on social systems, self-regulation, and role boundaries. Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management, 22, 125–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychological Association (2017). Stress in America: Coping with change. https://www.apa.org/news/press/releases/stress/2017/state-nation.pdf. Accessed 06.2018.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anshel, A., & Kipper, D. A. (1988). The influence of group singing on trust and cooperation. Journal of Music Therapy, 25(3), 145–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beal, D. J., Cohen, R. R., Burke, M. J., & McLendon, C. L. (2003). Cohesion and performance in groups: A meta-analytic clarification of construct relations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(6), 989–1004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bittman, B. B., Berk, L. S., Felten, D. L., Westengard, J., Simonton, O. C., Pappas, J., & Ninehouser, M. (2001). Composite effects of group drumming music therapy on modulation of neuroendocrine-immune parameters in normal subjects. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, 7(1), 38–47.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bittman, B. B., Berk, L. S., Shannon, M., Sharaf, M., Westengard, J., Guegler, K. J., & Ruff, D. W. (2005). Recreational music-making modulates the human stress response: A preliminary individualized gene expression strategy. Medical Science Monitor, 11(2), 31–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bittman, B. B., Croft JR, D. T., Brinker, J., van Laar, R., Vernalis, M. N., & Ellsworth, D. L. (2013). Recreational music-making alters gene expression pathways in patients with coronary heart disease. Medical Science Monitor, 19, 139–147.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bittman, B. B., Snyder, C., Bruhn, K. T., Liebfreid, F., Stevens, C. K., Westengard, J., & Umbach, P. O. (2004). Recreational music-making: An integrative group intervention for reducing burnout and improving mood states in first year associate degree nursing students: insights and economic impact. International Journal of Nursing Education Scholarship, 1(1), 12. https://doi.org/10.2202/1548-923x.1044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boecker, H., Sprenger, T., Spilker, M. E., Henriksen, G., Koppenhoefer, M., Wagner, K. J., et al. (2008). The runner’s high: Opioidergic mechanisms in the human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 18(11), 2523–2531.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • CDC Foundation (2015). Worker illness and injury costs U.S. employers $225.8 billion annually. http://www.cdcfoundation.org/pr/2015/worker-illness-and-injury-costs-us-employers-225-billion-annually. Accessed 06.2018.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., & Trainor, L. J. (2014). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Developmental Science, 17(6), 1003–1011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, E. E. A., Ejsmond-Frey, R., Knight, N., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2010). Rowers’ high: Behavioural synchrony is correlated with elevated pain thresholds. Biology Letters, 6(1), 106–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crutzen, R., & Göritz, A. S. (2010). Social desirability and self-reported health risk behaviors in web-based research: Three longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health, 10(1), 720.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, A., Taylor, J., & Cohen, E. (2015). Social bonds and exercise: Evidence for a reciprocal relationship. PloS ONE, 10(8), e136705.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • De Dreu, C. K., & Weingart, L. R. (2003). Task versus relationship conflict, team performance, and team member satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(4), 741.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drolet, G., Dumont, É. C., Gosselin, I., Kinkead, R., Laforest, S., & Trottier, J.-F. (2001). Role of endogenous opioid system in the regulation of the stress response. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 25(4), 729–741.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M., Baron, R., Frangou, A., Pearce, E., van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Stow, J., et al. (2012a). Social laughter is correlated with an elevated pain threshold. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1731), 1161–1167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M., Kaskatis, K., MacDonald, I., & Barra, V. (2012b). Performance of music elevates pain threshold and positive affect: Implications for the evolutionary function of music. Evolutionary Psychology, 10(4), 688–702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M., Teasdale, B., Thompson, J., Budelmann, F., Duncan, S., van Emde Boas, E., & Maguire, L. (2016). Emotional arousal when watching drama increases pain threshold and social bonding. Royal Society Open Science, 3(9), 160288.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, A. P. (2006). System breakdown: The role of mental models and transactive memory in the relationship between acute stress and team performance. Academy of Management Journal, 49(3), 576–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folkestad, J., & Gonzalez, R. (2010). Teamwork for innovation: A content analysis of the highly read and highly cited literature on innovation. Advances in Developing Human Resources, 12(1), 115–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Good, A., Choma, B., & Russo, F. A. (2017). Movement synchrony influences intergroup relations in a minimal groups paradigm. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 39(4), 231–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guler, I., & Nerkar, A. (2012). The impact of global and local cohesion on innovation in the pharmaceutical industry. Strategic Management Journal, 33(5), 535–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halbesleben, J. R. (2006). Sources of social support and burnout: A meta-analytic test of the conservation of resources model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(5), 1134–1145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C., & Ehlert, U. (2003). Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biological Psychiatry, 54(12), 1389–1398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27, 949–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4‑year-old children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(5), 354–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kokal, I., Engel, A., Kirschner, S., & Keysers, C. (2011). Synchronized drumming enhances activity in the caudate and facilitates prosocial commitment—if the rhythm comes easily. PLoS ONE, 6(11), e27272.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Koyama, M., Wachi, M., Utsuyama, M., Bittman, B., Hirokawa, K., & Kitagawa, M. (2009). Recreational music-making modulates immunological responses and mood states in older adults. Journal of Medical and Dental Sciences, 56, 79–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kristensen, T. S. (1991). Sickness absence and work strain among Danish slaughterhouse workers: An analysis of absence from work regarded as coping behaviour. Social Science & Medicine, 32(1), 15–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakens, D. (2010). Movement synchrony and perceived entitativity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(5), 701–708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lantz, B. (2013). The impact of sample non-normality on ANOVA and alternative methods. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 66(2), 224–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Launay, J., Tarr, B., Dunbar, R. I. M., & Bshary, R. (2016). Synchrony as an adaptive mechanism for large-scale human social bonding. Ethology, 122, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Machin, A. J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2011). The brain opioid theory of social attachment: A review of the evidence. Behaviour, 148(9), 985–1025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathieu, J. E., Tannenbaum, S. I., & Salas, E. (1992). Influences of individual and situational characteristics on measures of training effectiveness. Academy of Management Journal, 35(4), 828–847.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazzurega, M., Pavani, F., Paladino, M. P., & Schubert, T. W. (2011). Self-other bodily merging in the context of synchronous but arbitrary-related multisensory inputs. Experimental Brain Research, 213(2–3), 213–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michie, S. (2003). Reducing work related psychological ill health and sickness absence: A systematic literature review. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 60(1), 3–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K., & Macrae, C. N. (2009). The rhythm of rapport: Interpersonal synchrony and social perception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), 585–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mohr, G., Rigotti, T., & Müller, A. (2005). Irritation – ein Instrument zur Erfassung psychischer Beanspruchung im Arbeitskontext. Skalen- und Itemparameter aus 15 Studien. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 49(1), 44–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, R. B., & Casper, W. J. (2000). Examining the factor structure of participant reactions to training: A multidimensional approach. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 11(3), 301–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mothes, H., Klaperski, S., Seelig, H., Schmidt, S., & Fuchs, R. (2014). Regular aerobic exercise increases dispositional mindfulness in men: A randomized controlled trial. Mental Health and Physical Activity, 7(2), 111–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mucci, N., Giorgi, G., Roncaioli, M., Fiz Perez, J., & Arcangeli, G. (2016). The correlation between stress and economic crisis: A systematic review. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 12, 983–993.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith of happy people. American Psychologist, 55(1), 56–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nummenmaa, L., Manninen, S., Tuominen, L., Hirvonen, J., Kalliokoski, K. K., Nuutila, P., et al. (2015). Adult attachment style is associated with cerebral mu-opioid receptor availability in humans. Human Brain Mapping, 36(9), 3621–3628.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paladino, M.-P., Mazzurega, M., Pavani, F., & Schubert, T. W. (2010). Synchronous multisensory stimulation blurs self-other boundaries. Psychological Science, 21(9), 1202–1207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Páez, D., Rimé, B., Basabe, N., Wlodarczyk, A., & Zumeta, L. (2015). Psychosocial effects of perceived emotional synchrony in collective gatherings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(5), 711–729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reagans, R., & McEvily, B. (2003). Network structure and knowledge transfer: The effects of cohesion and range. Administrative Science Quarterly, 48(2), 240–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reddish, P., Bulbulia, J., & Fischer, R. (2013a). Does synchrony promote generalized prosociality? Religion, Brain & Behavior, 4(1), 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reddish, P., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. (2013b). Let’s dance together: Synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation. PLoS ONE, 8(8), e71182.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rennung, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2015). Facing sorrow as a group unites. Facing sorrow in a group divides. PLoS ONE, 10(9), e136750.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rennung, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2016). Prosocial consequences of interpersonal synchrony: A meta-analysis. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 224(3), 168–189.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rennung, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2017). Die Wirkung organisationaler Rituale – Eine qualitative Interviewstudie. Gruppe. Interaktion. Organisation. Zeitschrift für Angewandte Organisationspsychologie, 48(4), 327–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roumois, U. H. (2007). Studienbuch Wissensmanagement (3rd edn.). Zürich: Orell Füssli.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaufeli, W. B., Bakker, A. B., & van Rhenen, W. (2009). How changes in job demands and resources predict burnout, work engagement, and sickness absenteeism. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30(7), 893–917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steel, R. P. (2003). Methodological and operational issues in the construction of absence variables. Human Resource Management Review, 13, 243–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tarr, B., Launay, J., Cohen, E., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2015). Synchrony and exertion during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bonding. Biology Letters, 11(10), 20150767.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tarr, B., Launay, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Silent disco: Dancing in synchrony leads to elevated pain thresholds and social closeness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(5), 343–349.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tunçgenç, B., & Cohen, E. (2016). Movement synchrony forges social bonds across group divides. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 782.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ulich, E., & Wülser, M. (2012). Gesundheitsmanagement in Unternehmen. Wiesbaden: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vacharkulksemsuk, T., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2012). Strangers in sync: Achieving embodied rapport through shared movements. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 399–402.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion, 11(2), 262–266.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., & DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(4), 693–695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wachi, M., Koyama, M., Utsuyama, M., Bittman, B. B., Kitagawa, M., & Hirokawa, K. (2007). Recreational music-making modulates natural killer cell activity, cytokines, and mood states in corporate employees. Medical Science Monitor, 13(2), 57–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, E. T., Ying, T.-C., Jiang, J. J., & Klein, G. (2006). Group cohesion in organizational innovation: An empirical examination of ERP implementation. Information and Software Technology, 48(4), 235–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson-Jones, R. E., & Legare, C. H. (2016). The social functions of group rituals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(1), 42–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, D., Launay, J., Pearce, E., Dunbar, R. I. M., & Stewart, L. (2016). Singing and social bonding: Changes in connectivity and pain threshold as a function of group size. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(2), 152–158.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wiltermuth, S. S. (2012a). Synchrony and destructive obedience. Social Influence, 7(2), 78–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiltermuth, S. S. (2012b). Synchronous activity boosts compliance with requests to aggress. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 453–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20(1), 1–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeager, K. L., & Nafukho, F. M. (2012). Developing diverse teams to improve performance in the organizational setting. European Journal of Training and Development, 36(4), 388–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the publishing company Zypresse, Freiburg, Germany, (www.zypresse.com) for providing the field environment in which this study was run.

Funding

This work was supported by research grant GO 1107/13-1 from the German Research Foundation (DFG) to Göritz.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anja S. Göritz.

Additional information

A.S. Göritz and M. Rennung contributed equally to this work. The order of authorship is alphabetical.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Göritz, A.S., Rennung, M. Interpersonal synchrony increases social cohesion, reduces work-related stress and prevents sickdays: a longitudinal field experiment. Gr Interakt Org 50, 83–94 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11612-019-00450-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11612-019-00450-8

Keywords

Schlüsselwörter

Navigation