Skip to main content
Log in

Ein Experiment zum Einfluss der Situation auf physische und psychische Nähe

  • Betriebliche Gesundheitsförderung
  • Published:
Gruppe. Interaktion. Organisation. Zeitschrift für Angewandte Organisationspsychologie (GIO) Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Zusammenfassung

In einem Experiment (N = 90) wurde der Einfluss situativer Bedingungen auf den Sitzabstand und auf die per Fragebogen erfasste subjektiv erlebte Nähe zu einer anderen Person überprüft. Es wurden eine Kooperationsbedingung, eine neutrale Bedingung und eine Wettbewerbsbedingung realisiert. Es zeigte sich, dass die neutrale Bedingung die geringste räumliche Nähe hervorrief, während die Kooperationsbedingung die höchste emotionale Nähe auslöste. Zur Interpretation wird die Annahme vertreten, dass die neutrale Bedingung sich von der Wettbewerbsbedingung darin unterscheidet, dass sie von den Probanden als irrelevant empfunden wird und dadurch keine Aufmerksamkeitszuwendung auf die andere Person auslöst. Weiterhin wird angenommen, dass emotionale Nähe das Ergebnis von positiver Atmosphäre und Relevanz der Interaktion ist. Eine weitere Fragestellung bezog sich auf den Zusammenhang zwischen (geringem) Sitzabstand und emotionaler Nähe, der in Übereinstimmung mit Studien zur Einstellungs-Verhaltens-Konsistenz relativ gering ausfiel.

Abstract

The influence of the social situation on the physical distance and, as measured by a questionnaire, emotional intimacy between participants was experimentally studied (N = 90) in a cooperative, a competitive, and a neutral social situation. Results indicate that the physical distance was greatest under neutral conditions. The cooperative condition produced the strongest feeling of intimacy. It is argued that the neutral condition is irrelevant to the participants’ behaviour and level of attention to other persons. Further, intimacy can only arise if an interaction is both positive and relevant. The correlation between physical and emotional distance turned out to be relatively small, in accordance with the range usually given for attitudebehaviour correlations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Literaturverzeichnis

  • Argyle, M. (1975). Soziale Interaktion. Köln: Kiepenheuer & Witsch.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartholomew, K. & Horowitz, L.M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R.F. & Bratslavsky, E. (1999). Passion, intimacy, and time: Passionate love as a function of change in intimacy. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 49–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berry, J.T., Hurley, J.H. & Worthington, E.L. (1990). Empirical validity of the Kvebaek Family Sculpture Technique. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 18, 19–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bierhoff-Alfermann, D. & Bierhoff, H.W. (1976). Sozialpsychologische Aspekte der Umweltpsychologie. In Kaminski, G. (Hrsg.). Umweltpsychologie (pp. 40–58). Stuttgart: Klett.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bischof, N. (1997). Das Rätsel Ödipus. Die biologischen Wurzeln des Urkonflikts von Intimität und Autonomie. 4. Aufl. München: Piper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Canary, D.J. & Emmers-Sommer, T.M. (1997). Sex and gender differences in personal relationships. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chelune, G.J., Robison, J.T. & Kommor, M.J. (1984). A cognitive interactional model of intimate relationships. In V.J. Derlega (Ed.), Communication, intimacy, and close relationships (pp.11–40). Orlando: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G.L. & Gormly, J.B. (1974). Knowing, feeling, and liking. A psychophysiological study of attraction. Journal of Research in Personality, 8, 218–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffitt, W. (1970). Environmental effects on interpersonal affective behavior: Ambient effective temperature and attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 240–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hatfield, E. (1984). The dangers of intimacy. In V.J. Derlega (Ed.), Communication, intimacy, and close relationships (pp. 207–220). Orlando: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick D.T. & Johnson, G.A. (1979). Interpersonal attraction in aversive environments: A problem for the classical conditioning paradigm? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 572–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levinger, G. (1977). The embrace of lives: Changing and unchanging. In G. Levingerjuvy and H.L. Raush (Eds.), Close relationships (pp. 1–16). Amherst: University of Massachusetts Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lott, A.J. & Lott, B.E. (1961). Group cohesiveness, communication level, and conformity. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62, 408–412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lott. A.J. & Lott, B.E. (1974). The role of reward in the formation of positive interpersonal attitudes. In T.L. Huston (Ed.), Foundations of interpersonal attraction (pp. 171–189). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M.L. (1988). Functions of nonverbal behavior in close relationships. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships (pp. 41–56). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peplau, L.A. & Gordon, S.L. (1985). Women and men in love: Gender differences in close heterosexual relationships. In V.E. O’Leary, R.K. Ungerjuvy and B.S. Wallston (Eds.), Women, gender, and social psychology. (pp. 257–291) Hillsdale, Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1937). The child’s construction of reality. London: Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prager, K.J. (2000). Intimacy in personal relationships. In C. Hendrickjuvy and S. Hendrick (Eds.), Close relationships — A sourcebook (pp. 229–242). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reis, H. T. & Shaver, P. R. (1988). Intimacy as an interpersonal process. In S.W. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships (pp. 367–389). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotter, J. (1954). Social learning and clinical psychology. Englewood Cliffs, Prentice-Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Salewski, C. (1993). Raumlicher Abstand in dyadischen Interaktionen. Gruppendynamik, 24, 5–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, S. & Singer, J.E. (1962). Cognitve, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sewell, A.F. & Heisler, J.T. (1973). Personality correlates of proximity preferences. Journal of Psychology, 85, 151–155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wicker (1969). Attitude versus action: The relationship of verbal and overt behavioral responses to attitude objects. Journal of Social Issues, 25, 41–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ina Grau.

Additional information

PD Dr. Ina Grau, Dipl. Psych., Universität Bielefeld, Fakultät für Soziologie, Lehrstuhl Sozialpsychologie, Prof. Dr. Hans-Werner Bierhoff, Dipl. Psych., Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Fakultät für Psychologie, Lehrstuhl Sozialpsychologie

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Grau, I., Bierhoff, HW. Ein Experiment zum Einfluss der Situation auf physische und psychische Nähe. Gruppendynamik 33, 437–450 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11612-002-0037-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11612-002-0037-z

Navigation