1 Introduction

Ethnic–racial discrimination, the differential treatment of individuals based on ethnic or racial group membership (Carter and Forsyth 2010), is a chronic, potent stressor with adverse effects on mental health outcomes such as anxiety (Berger and Sarnyai 2015; Priest et al. 2013). However, the neurobiological mechanisms underlying these observed associations are inconclusive, particularly during the transition from middle childhood to early adolescence when emotion regulatory networks are developing and social networks are expanding (Scherf et al. 2013). The current study is aimed at addressing this gap by testing whether volumetric alterations of the amygdala, a highly stress-sensitive neural region, account for associations between ethnic–racial discrimination exposure and anxiety symptoms. This study was conducted in preadolescent Latina girls, a group exhibiting heightened levels of untreated anxiety (Ginsburg and Silverman 1996) that is also disproportionately subjected to ethnic–racial discrimination (Vargas et al. 2021). If replicated, identifying how this form of sociocultural stress influences threat-relevant brain networks and escalates the risk for anxiety represents a promising potential step toward developing culturally informed treatment programs to reduce the burden of ethnic- and racial-based mental health disparities in minoritized youth.

1.1 Discrimination and Anxiety in Latinx Youth

Antagonistic political views and growing animosity toward immigrants and people of color have recently brought ethnic–racial discrimination into the limelight of the United States’ political, economic, and social landscape. Ethnic–racial discrimination is more prevalent among people from disadvantaged backgrounds (i.e., individuals with limited access to social and financial resources), including immigrant populations (Carter and Forsyth 2010), than those from privileged backgrounds. For instance, approximately half of Latinx people have experienced ethnic–racial discrimination, with immigration status as a primary reason for unfair treatment (Lopez et al. 2020). Latinx youth, in particular, report experiencing both overt and subtle forms of ethnic–racial discrimination from multiple sources (e.g., peers, teachers, strangers) and across multiple contexts (e.g., at school, in stores, on public transportation) (Pasco et al. 2022).

Latinx people are the largest and second fastest-growing ethnically minoritized group in the USA (United States Census Bureau 2020). Despite this trend, there is substantially less research on the mental health costs of ethnic–racial discrimination in Latinx populations than in African American and Asian populations (Araújo and Borrell 2006). Even though discriminatory treatment is an experience shared across ethnic and racial groups, different groups are embedded within unique sociohistorical contexts, and their experiences of mistreatment may thus possess unique sociocultural underpinnings. Examining these processes in Latinx individuals could illuminate the generalizability of currently understood sociocultural risk and protective processes. Doing so is consequential because exposure to discrimination from birth through adolescence predicts various physical and mental health outcomes, including anxiety (Priest et al. 2013). Latina girls, vulnerable to multiple or intersectional forms of discrimination based on race, ethnicity, and gender, may be at heightened risk. For instance, a study of internalizing symptomatology and suicidality in Latinx youth observed a pronounced effect of discrimination on suicide risk for Latina girls relative to Latino boys (Vargas et al. 2021).

An estimated one-third of adolescent girls are affected by internalizing symptoms, including anxiety, and rates among Latina adolescents, in particular, have significantly increased in recent years (Anderson and Mayes 2010; Ginsburg and Silverman 1996; Kessler et al. 1994; Varela et al. 2004). In both community-based and clinically referred samples, Latinx youth demonstrate higher rates of anxiety than their European American peers (Glover et al. 1999; Pina and Silverman 2004; Roberts et al. 2006). Further, Latina girls report more anxiety symptoms than European American, African American, and Latino youth (McLaughlin et al. 2007). Because sex differences in the incidence of anxiety begin in childhood and increase in magnitude during adolescence (Lewinsohn et al. 1998; Roza et al. 2003), characterizing mechanisms by which sociocultural stressors, like ethnic–racial discrimination, may escalate anxiety risk at the transition to adolescence is crucial. This work has significant implications for culturally informed intervention and prevention efforts to reduce the disproportionately high rates of anxiety observed in Latina youth.

1.2 Minority Stress Processes

One potential neurobiological pathway between ethnic–racial discrimination experiences and anxiety is that ongoing exposure to racially charged stressors presents imminent threats that may perturb stress-sensitive neural networks (Bale and Jovanovic 2021). Exposure to stress triggers an adaptive response that helps the body to prepare for danger, injury, or infection (Boyce and Ellis 2005). However, when this stress becomes chronic, our capacity to efficiently process it declines, and takes its toll on our health. Importantly, these processes are shaped by physical risk factors, individual behaviors, and social conditions. Social determinants of health—defined as the circumstances in which people are born, grow up, live, work, and age—are as influential on health as access to health care (Lucyk and McLaren 2017). Theories of social stress suggest that adverse conditions in the social environment can overwhelm our stress response systems and induce physical or mental illness (Dohrenwend 2000). Minority stress theory, in particular, posits that social stress can significantly and adversely impact the lives of people belonging to stigmatized social groups, including categories related to ethnicity or race (Meyer 1995; Meyer and Dean 1998). One such chronic, potent social stressor experienced by minoritized individuals is ethnic–racial discrimination. Repeated exposure to cumulative experiences associated with discrimination and victimization overtaxes individuals’ stress response systems, contributing to health deterioration (Geronimus et al. 2006) and escalating their vulnerability to stress-related health problems over the life course. Empirically, experiencing ethnic–racial discrimination has been observed to predict greater cardiovascular reactivity, including higher systolic and diastolic blood pressure throughout the day (Steffen et al. 2003). However, the link between experienced discrimination and anxiety-relevant neurobiological stress-response systems is relatively unexplored, particularly among youth.

1.2.1 Neurobiological Pathways

Ethnic–racial discrimination predicts wide-ranging physical and mental health outcomes (Gaylord-Harden and Cunningham 2009; Pachter et al. 2018; Steffen et al. 2003; Stein et al. 2019), including elevating stress and anxiety among children and adolescents (Priest et al. 2013). The mechanisms underlying these observed associations are complex and not yet fully understood. However, neuroscience, particularly when situated in a sociological framework, provides a valuable tool for measuring and characterizing how experiences of ethnic–racial discrimination contribute to anxiety symptoms (Firat 2019, 2021). Several brain regions are responsible for processing different aspects of fear, threat, and pain. Specifically, the amygdala is essential to monitoring the social environment for basic threats, including threats of discriminatory treatment (Krill and Platek 2009), and coordinating with other brain regions to mediate stress response systems (Calder et al. 2001; Fanselow and LeDoux 1999). When chronic stressors like ethnic–racial discrimination perturb communication within and among these systems, this can engender anxious hypervigilance or an excessive anticipatory response to potential social threats (Goosby et al. 2018; van Marle et al. 2009), a core feature of pathological anxiety. Among the most consistent findings in individuals with an anxiety disorder is exaggerated amygdala activation (see Etkin and Wager 2007, for a review). Individuals with subthreshold anxiety exhibit elevated amygdala responses to social anxiety-inducing cues (Beaton et al. 2008); Díaz et al. 2024), suggesting that anxiety might fall on a continuum (Jayakar et al. 2020).

Because the amygdala aids centrally in threat vigilance, it is conceivable that it would be similarly active in response to discriminatory treatment. Indeed, emerging functional and structural neuroimaging evidence in adults documents the detrimental and compounding effects of ethnic–racial discrimination on the neural architecture of the amygdala (Hobson et al. 2022). For example, the amygdala is highly responsive to social exclusion (Eisenberger 2013) and these processes relate to increased sympathetic nervous system activity (Critchley et al. 2003; McEwen and Gianaros 2010), a known biomarker of heightened anxiety (Wenner 2018). Individuals who report higher levels of ethnic–racial discrimination also exhibit heightened spontaneous amygdala activity (Krill and Platek 2009) and greater functional connectivity with regions involved in threat processing and emotion regulation during resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging (Clark et al. 2018; Webb et al. 2022). A related study finds that greater amygdala functional connectivity to threat-relevant brain regions among Hispanic and Black participants relative to white participants is, in part, driven by race-related differences in structural inequities (Harnett et al. 2023).

Fewer studies have examined the effect of ethnic–racial discrimination on brain anatomy, with one recent study documenting larger amygdala volumes in adults exposed to higher levels of ethnic–racial discrimination (Rosario et al. 2020). Importantly, no studies of ethnic–racial discrimination, to date, have investigated functional or structural alterations of the amygdala in youth. This is consequential, as anxiety disorders that affect adults typically begin in late childhood (Glenn et al. 2012). Further, the transition from childhood to adolescence is marked by a host of connected neurophysiological (Glenn et al. 2022) and social changes, including a heightened sensitivity to social exclusion (Masten et al. 2009) and an increasing awareness of social status and positions in social hierarchies (Goosby et al. 2013). Therefore, examining these processes in preadolescence could be informative for developing culturally informed intervention and prevention efforts to reduce the burden of ethnicity- and race-based health disparities in minoritized youth (Michalska and Davis 2019).

1.3 Study Overview

The current study utilized structural brain imaging to evaluate whether altered amygdala volume is one mechanism underlying vulnerability to anxiety in preadolescent Latina girls experiencing ethnic–racial discrimination. We expected that youth with greater ethnic–racial discrimination exposure would report higher anxiety levels. We also tested a mediation model that evaluated an indirect effect of ethnic–racial discrimination exposure on anxiety symptoms through amygdala volume. We complement and extend prior work in several ways. First, emerging empirical work (see Fani et al. 2021, 2022) examining neural mechanisms underlying the association between ethnic–racial discrimination and mental health is still in its infancy. Here, we add to a growing body of literature probing these mechanisms. Second, to our knowledge, this is the first neurobiological study of ethnic–racial discrimination and anxiety in preadolescent youth, enabling examination of these processes during an earlier developmental period marked by significant neurophysiological and social transitions. Third, we study a sample consisting exclusively of Latina girls, a group exhibiting heightened levels of untreated anxiety (Ginsburg and Silverman 1996; McLaughlin et al. 2007; Varela et al. 2004) that is disproportionately subjected to experiences of discrimination (Vargas et al. 2021) and has been underrepresented in research focused on the mental health costs of ethnic–racial discrimination (Araújo and Borrell 2006; La Scala et al. 2023).

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

Participants included 30 Latina girls (MAge = 9.76, SD = 1.11, range = 8–12 years, 66.67% Mexican, 10.00% Cuban, and 23.33% of mixed or unspecified Latina heritage) residing in the Inland Empire Region of Southern California. Participants were recruited via fliers in outpatient mental health clinics and local hospitals, and the University of California, Riverside Psychology Department shared database of child participants. Participant eligibility was determined by phone screening with a parent. Children were eligible for participation if they were fluent in English, aged 8–13 years, self-identified as Latina, were pre-menstrual, had no contraindications for neuroimaging, and did not meet any exclusionary criteria. We note that although menstruation was initially included as an exclusion criterion, it was later dropped to increase sample size and two post-menstrual participants were recruited. Exclusion criteria for children were a current psychiatric diagnosis of Tourette’s syndrome or obsessive-compulsive disorder, suicidal ideation, active medical problems, or any lifetime history of mania, psychosis, or pervasive developmental disorder. On average, families had household incomes ($ 25,000–$ 39,999) below the median annual household income in the USA ($ 70,784; United States Census Bureau 2022). The income distribution of the current sample appears in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Pie chart depicting the distribution of annual household incomes

2.2 Procedures

Data for the present analyses were collected at participants’ first of several laboratory assessments as part of an ongoing longitudinal study of socioemotional development (Díaz et al. 2024; Glenn et al. 2022; Mullins et al. 2021). Upon participant arrival, written parent consent and child assent were obtained. During the laboratory session, children completed a structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan and a battery of self-report questionnaires assessing demographics, discrimination exposure, and anxiety symptoms. At the end of the laboratory session, participants were compensated with a gift card and a small prize. The university Institutional Review Board approved all study procedures.

2.3 Measures

2.3.1 Brain Imaging, Data Processing, and Analysis

Magnetic resonance imaging scans were acquired on a 3-Tesla Siemens Magnetom Prisma scanner (Siemens, Malvern, PA, USA) with a 32-channel receive-only head coil at the University of California, Riverside Center for Advanced Neuroimaging. Participants completed a T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid conditioning gradient-echo scan with the following parameters: 208 sagittally acquired 0.80-mm slices, 320 × 300 matrix, 0.8 mm3 isotropic voxels, flip angle = 8°, field of view = 256 × 240 mm, repetition time = 2400 ms, echo time = 2.72 ms, inversion time = 1060 ms. Using Freesurfer Version 6.0.0 (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu), T1 images were processed, cortical surface reconstruction was performed, and the left and right amygdala volume measurements were extracted using standard processing pipelines (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Using Freesurfer Version 6.0.0 (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/), volume measurements for the left and right amygdala were extracted. For visualization in FMRIB Software Library (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/), the sample subject Freesurfer amygdala labels were converted to volumetric masks and overlaid on a coronal view of the Freesurfer-provided brain mask as the underlay

2.3.2 Experiences of Discrimination

To assess experiences of ethnic–racial discrimination, participants completed the Perceptions of Racism in Children and Youth (PRaCY; Pachter et al. 2010). Prior to questionnaire administration, children were told, “When people are racially discriminated against, they are treated badly, not given respect, or are considered inferior because of the color of their skin, because they speak a different language or have an accent, or because they come from a different country or culture. For each of the following situations, think whether you have ever in your life felt discriminated against because of your color, language or accent, or because of your culture or country of origin.” Children were then provided a list of 23 discriminatory experiences (e.g., “someone called you an insulting name,” “teachers assume you’re not smart or intelligent,” etc.) and were asked to indicate which of them they had experienced. For each endorsed item, children used a five-point Likert scale to indicate how often this had happened to them (1 = Once to 5 = Weekly) and how much this experience upset them (0 = Not at all to 4 = Extremely). Responses to these scales were summed to index the prevalence and severity of discriminatory experiences respectively—children who did not endorse any experiences of ethnic–racial discrimination received prevalence and severity scores of 0. The PRaCY has been utilized as a valid measure of perceived discriminatory experiences in studies of both children (Marcelo and Yates 2019) and adolescents (Park et al. 2017).

2.3.3 Anxiety Symptoms

Participants completed the 41-item Screen for Child Anxiety-Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED; Birmaher et al. 1999) to provide continuous measures of anxiety symptoms across five domains: panic/somatic, generalized anxiety, separation anxiety, social phobia, and school phobia. For each item, children indicated on a three-point Likert scale (0 = Not true or hardly ever true to 2 = Very true or often true) the extent to which each was true for them. Responses were summed across all items, as well as within each subdomain. The SCARED demonstrated strong internal consistency in the current sample with a Cronbach’s α of 0.93. Of note, 80.0% of the sample met clinical thresholds for anxiety disorder (≥ 25).

2.4 Data Analysis

Linear regression analysis implemented in SPSS Version 28.0.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY: https://www.ibm.com/spss), together with the PROCESS macro (Hayes 2013), was used to evaluate the associations of ethnic–racial discrimination and anxiety symptoms and mediating effects of amygdala volume on any observed associations. To do this, we first evaluated whether ethnic–racial discrimination exposure was associated with amygdala volume (a path). Next, we examined whether amygdala volume was associated with anxiety symptoms, controlling for ethnic–racial discrimination exposure (b path). Finally, we assessed whether amygdala volume indirectly mediated the association between ethnic–racial discrimination and anxiety symptoms. Separate regression models were conducted for each predictor (i.e., ethnic–racial discrimination prevalence, ethnic–racial discrimination severity) and mediator (i.e., left amygdala volume, right amygdala volume) of interest. As such, four regression models were conducted, each controlling for total intracranial volume and annual household income, which has been linked with alterations in brain structure (Merz et al. 2018).

All variables were continuous and mean centered prior to analysis, and the estimated effects are reported as standardized regression coefficients. To address any non-normality in the distribution of the outcomes, the models were estimated using bootstrapped samples (bootstrap N = 5000) to produce 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals (BC 95% CI) around the parameter estimates. Effects were determined to be significant at p < 0.05 if the upper and lower limits of the CIs did not contain zero. To account for multiple tests, Bonferroni corrections were applied.

3 Results

3.1 Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive statistics appear in Table 1, and bivariate correlations appear in Table 2. Of note, a significant positive association between ethnic–racial discrimination severity and anxiety symptoms emerged (r = 0.38, p = 0.040). Scatter plots depicting the association of amygdala volume with ethnic–racial discrimination prevalence, ethnic–racial discrimination severity, and anxiety symptoms appear in Fig. 3.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for key study variables
Table 2 Bivariate correlations (and corresponding p values) for variables of interest
Fig. 3
figure 3

Scatter plots depicting the association of amygdala volume with a ethnic–racial discrimination prevalence, b ethnic–racial discrimination severity, and c anxiety symptoms. (ERD Ethnic–Racial Discrimination)

3.2 Ethnic–Racial Discrimination and Anxiety Symptoms

Controlling for total intracranial volume and annual household income, regression analysis revealed that the effect of children’s concurrent ethnic–racial discrimination experiences on anxiety symptoms was positive but not statistically significant for both ethnic–racial discrimination prevalence (β = 0.20, SE = 0.25, BC 95% CI [−0.259, 0.766], p = 0.318) and ethnic–racial discrimination severity (β = 0.31, SE = 0.24, BC 95% CI [−0.101, 0.885], p = 0.114). Because it is appropriate to examine a mediation analysis even when the total effect does not reach conventional thresholds of statistical significance, as such a requirement reduces the statistical power to detect mediation (Hayes 2009; MacKinnon et al. 2007; Muele 2019; Zhao et al. 2010), we conducted analyses to test whether ethnic–racial discrimination indirectly contributed to greater anxiety symptoms through alterations in amygdala volume. As a reminder, we evaluated: first, whether ethnic–racial discrimination exposure was associated with amygdala volume (a path); second, whether amygdala volume was associated with anxiety symptoms, controlling for ethnic–racial discrimination exposure (b path); and third, whether amygdala volume indirectly mediated the association between ethnic–racial discrimination and anxiety symptoms.

3.3 Ethnic–Racial Discrimination and Brain Structure (a Path)

Participants who reported more severe ethnic–racial discrimination exposure had a significantly smaller left amygdala volume than participants who reported less severe ethnic–racial discrimination exposure (β = −0.37, SE = 2.13, BC 95% CI [−9.680, −0.902], p = 0.020), controlling for total intracranial volume and annual household income. This association was not significant for the right amygdala or the prevalence of ethnic–racial discrimination exposure, all ps > 0.066.

3.4 Brain Structure and Anxiety (b Path)

Participants with a larger left amygdala volume reported more anxiety symptoms than participants with a smaller left amygdala volume (β = 0.76, SE = 0.02, BC 95% CI [0.030, 0.106], p = 0.001), controlling for total intracranial volume, annual household income, and the severity of ethnic–racial discrimination exposure. This association was not significant for the right amygdala, p = 0.774. Participants with a larger left amygdala volume reported more anxiety symptoms than participants with a smaller left amygdala volume (β = 0.57, SE = 0.02, BC 95% CI [0.011, 0.092], p = 0.015), controlling for total intracranial volume, annual household income, and the prevalence of ethnic–racial discrimination exposure. This association was not significant for the right amygdala, p = 0.990.

3.5 Mediation Analyses

Mediation analyses revealed indirect effects of ethnic–racial discrimination on anxiety symptoms via amygdala volume (Table 3). The first analysis tested whether left amygdala volume mediated the association between ethnic–racial discrimination prevalence and anxiety symptoms. Controlling for total intracranial volume and annual household income, no significant mediating effect emerged, BC 95% CI [−0.483, 0.065], p = 0.093. The second linear regression analysis tested whether left amygdala volume mediated the association between ethnic–racial discrimination severity and anxiety symptoms, controlling for total intracranial volume and annual household income. The BC 95% CI of the standardized index of mediation for the model with left amygdala volume (β = −0.28, SE = 0.17, BC 95% CI [−0.690, −0.017], p = 0.014) did not include zero, suggesting a significant indirect effect for this model (Fig. 4). The third linear regression analysis tested whether right amygdala volume mediated the association between ethnic–racial discrimination prevalence and anxiety symptoms. Controlling for total intracranial volume and annual household income, no significant mediating effect emerged, BC 95% CI [−0.241, 0.185], p = 0.982. The fourth linear regression analysis tested whether right amygdala volume mediated the association between ethnic–racial discrimination severity and anxiety symptoms. Controlling for total intracranial volume and annual household income, no significant mediating effect emerged, BC 95% CI [−0.299, 0.155], p = 0.712. We note that because of the direction of our paths (see Fig. 4), our results do not explicitly support a mediation effect but rather two indirect paths linking (a) ethnic–racial discrimination severity with left amygdala volume and (b) left amygdala volume with child anxiety symptoms.

Table 3 Linear regression path estimates for the tested mediation models, each controlling for annual household income and total intracranial volume
Fig. 4
figure 4

Controlling for age and total intracranial volume, two indirect paths were observed: a negative association between ethnic–racial discrimination severity and left amygdala volume and a positive association between left amygdala volume and anxiety symptoms. Although the mediation model was significant, (β = −0.28, SE = 0.17, BC 95% CI [−0.690, −0.017], p = 0.014), we interpret these data as two indirect paths rather than partial or full mediation as c’ was not smaller than c. (Effects are reported as standardized regression coefficients. ERD Ethnic–Racial Discrimination. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001)

3.5.1 Bonferroni Corrections

The current analyses separately tested the indirect effects of left and right amygdala volume on the association between ethnic–racial discrimination exposure and anxiety symptoms. As such, Bonferroni corrections were applied to correct for multiple tests of laterality. With an adjusted p value of 0.05/2 = 0.025, the observed indirect effect of left amygdala volume on the association between ethnic–racial discrimination severity and anxiety symptoms remained significant, p = 0.014.

4 Discussion

We provide preliminary evidence that racialized threats in childhood may contribute to smaller left amygdala volume among Latina girls and that alterations in amygdala volume may indirectly contribute to increased vulnerability to anxiety symptoms. Our data elucidate a potential mechanism by which experiences of ethnic–racial discrimination can adversely impact mental health, particularly in the transition from middle childhood to early adolescence, a period marked by a host of interlinked neurophysiological and social changes. Prior work has consistently shown that youth who have experienced adversity, including abuse, neglect, and violence exposure, are at an elevated risk for mental health problems (King 2021). However, scholars have only recently begun investigating the neurobiological mechanisms underlying these patterns, including their extension to stressors unique to underrepresented and marginalized groups. Such efforts are consequential because chronic sociocultural stress may compound the effects of more widely experienced social stressors, suggesting that the health of individuals of color might be at a heightened risk (Meyer 1995; Meyer and Dean 1998).

In partial support of our first hypothesis, ethnic–racial discrimination severity correlated positively with anxiety symptoms. This result parallels previous findings that discrimination is related to detriments in mental health, including elevated anxiety (Priest et al. 2013). Even though we did not specify distinct effects of ethnic–racial discrimination severity and prevalence on anxiety symptoms, it is worth noting that only the severity of discriminatory experiences, not their prevalence, was associated with higher anxiety levels in our sample. This observed association is consistent with work on more conventionally studied forms of adversity (e.g., abuse, neglect), which suggests that the subjective experience of adversity might be a more potent predictor of child psychopathology than the objective occurrence (Baldwin and Degli Esposti 2021; Danese and Widom 2020). Under this notion, the degree to which children are upset by ethnic–racial discrimination experiences (i.e., severity) may be a unique indicator of anxiety risk relative to the frequency of ethnic–racial discrimination experiences accumulated (i.e., prevalence). As such, continued research on ethnic–racial discrimination must carefully examine these distinctions to characterize the potential differential effects of distinct facets of ethnic–racial discrimination on mental health.

In partial support of our second hypothesis, we observed evidence of racialized stress sensitization operating indirectly on anxiety symptoms via reduced left amygdala volume. Ethnic–racial discrimination severity was associated with reduced volume in the left amygdala. This finding is consistent with prior studies examining the association between trauma exposure and amygdala volume in children and adolescents, finding smaller amygdala volumes among children exposed to maltreatment (McLaughlin et al. 2016, 2019; Saxbe et al. 2018; Weissman et al. 2020). Importantly, our work suggests that sociocultural adversity might similarly implicate the structure of this stress-sensitive neural region. Left amygdala volume, in turn, was associated with elevated anxiety symptoms. This result parallels previous structural and functional work identifying larger amygdala volumes in anxious relative to non-anxious youth (De Bellis et al. 2000) and a compounding effect of adverse life experiences on linkages between amygdala reactivity and anxiety (Silvers et al. 2017). We note that subcortical structure does not consistently map onto function and subsequent psychopathology. Whereas increased amygdala volume may index heightened sensitivity to threat and create a vulnerability to anxiety, it is simultaneously plausible that increased amygdala volume might be the result of increased anxiety during development. Thus, we contend that in addition to identifying structural alterations in threat-relevant brain regions, we must also identify functional perturbations within and among these neural networks. Doing so would more completely clarify the role of amygdala volume and reactivity in anxiety risk.

Whereas ethnic–racial discrimination severity did not significantly predict anxiety symptoms, a significant indirect effect of ethnic–racial discrimination on anxiety symptoms via left amygdala volume emerged, identifying a potential neurobiological pathway by which ethnic–racial discrimination may contribute to elevated anxiety. Finally, these associations were not observed for the prevalence of ethnic–racial discrimination or right amygdala volume. The absence of an effect of the prevalence of ethnic–racial discrimination on left amygdala volume was surprising, given its high correlation with the severity of ethnic–racial discrimination. We note that the associative patterns of these discrimination indices with amygdala volume are qualitatively similar (see Fig. 3) and attribute the lack of similar quantitative effects to our modest sample size. However, future work in larger samples would be informative. Relatedly, we expect the observed lateral differences in amygdala volume to be a function of sample size too and argue that continued research is needed to determine the role the amygdala plays in mediating sociocultural stress and mental health changes depending on laterality.

The current findings should be considered in the context of several limitations. First, our sample size was modest, potentially reducing statistical power. Although analyses utilized bootstrapping techniques to assist with this, future work in larger samples would be informative. Of note, our mediation analysis included negative (i.e., effect of ethnic–racial discrimination severity on left amygdala volume) and positive (i.e., effect of left amygdala volume on anxiety symptoms) effects, suggesting that an untested variable might be moderating one or both paths linking racialized stress, amygdala volume, and anxiety symptoms. Larger samples may provide opportunities to parse out interactive effects of more proximal environmental factors through tests of moderated paths. For instance, structural inequities such as income detrimentally affect brain structure and function (Harnett et al. 2023; Merz et al. 2018), although household income was not significantly related to any variables of interest in the current study. Future work should continue probing the effects of income and other forms of structural inequity on the links among discrimination, neurobiology, and mental health. Second, this was a cross-sectional design. Longitudinal studies testing whether the effects of ethnic–racial discrimination on anxiety via alterations in amygdala volume prospectively sensitize Latina girls’ threat systems and exacerbate anxiety symptoms are necessary to more completely understand the toll of discrimination on stress-sensitive neurocircuitry and mental health over time. Third, this was a structural MRI investigation, limiting our ability to make inferences about the role of amygdala reactivity in the link between ethnic–racial discrimination and anxiety. Functional neuroimaging studies would elucidate whether ethnic–racial discrimination disrupts activity within and surrounding this stress-sensitive region, consequently elevating anxiety. Fourth, considering that the sample was primarily community-based (La Scala et al. 2023), participants exhibited unexpectedly high levels of anxiety, which may limit generalizability. Although these trends are consistent with a growing body of research showing heightened levels of untreated anxiety in Latina youth (Anderson and Mayes 2010; McLaughlin et al. 2007; Pina and Silverman 2004; Polo et al. 2024) and may further support our hypothesis that discriminatory experiences elevate anxiety, future work in samples with more varied levels of anxiety is warranted.

Several strengths mitigate these limitations and offer data about the amygdala’s role in linking ethnic–racial discrimination exposure and anxiety symptoms in Latina youth. First, emerging empirical work (see Fani et al. 2021, 2022) examining neurobiological mechanisms by which ethnic–racial discrimination shapes mental health is still in its infancy. Here, we add to a growing body of literature probing these pathways and to broader investigations of the influence of sociocultural processes on the psychobiology of emotional development (Michalska and Davis 2019). Second, to our knowledge, this is the first neurobiological study of ethnic–racial discrimination and anxiety in preadolescents, enabling examination of these processes during an earlier developmental period marked by significant neurophysiological and social transitions. Third, we study a sample consisting exclusively of Latina girls, a group exhibiting heightened levels of untreated anxiety (Ginsburg and Silverman 1996; McLaughlin et al. 2007; Varela et al. 2004) that is disproportionately subjected to experiences of discrimination (Vargas et al. 2021), and has been underrepresented in research focused on the mental health costs of ethnic–racial discrimination (Araújo and Borrell 2006).

In sum, the current study elucidates a potential neurobiological pathway by which negative social experiences detrimentally impact mental health. Specifically, we add to existing literature identifying ethnic–racial discrimination as a threat-relevant social stressor related to altered threat neurocircuitry and elevated anxiety. This work highlights the utility of neuroscience in measuring and understanding how adverse social conditions shape health trajectories and likely perpetuate ethnicity- and race-based health disparities. Together with other efforts, our investigation lays the initial groundwork for developing culturally-informed intervention and prevention efforts targeting long-term mental health outcomes in minoritized youth.