Skip to main content
Log in

Structuring process and closure principle in spatial and temporal reproduction tasks

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Neurodynamics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The goal of the experiment reported was to replicate the previous Sarrazin’s (2000) study in order to verify, with an adequate methodological procedure, whether or not the closure principle applied in spatial and temporal reproduction tasks. The hypothesis defended was that the closure of the pattern is an intrinsic property of the structuring process in spatial memory. The stimuli consisted of eight visually presented dots that appeared sequentially with inter-dot distances corresponding to inter-dot durations. After a learning phase, participants reproduced the spatial (space condition) or temporal (time condition) characteristics of the target 60 times in succession. We analyzed the variance level for both element location and Inter-Element-Interval (IEI) on spatial and temporal responses. Two main results emerge from this experiment: (1) the critical dependency of the closure principle to the nature (spatial or temporal) of the response, (2) the importance to consider both locations and intervals as complementary information. These results are discussed in the light of physical system, in particular in term of compensation phenomenon and we proposed a mathematical model that replicates the qualitative feature of variance for both space and time conditions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson NH (1974) Algebric models in perception. In: Carterette EC, Friedman MP (Eds) Handbook of perception, vol 2. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett FC (1932) Remembering: a study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Berberian B (2003) Dynamique visuo-spatiale du souvenir: étude de la structuration de l’image en mémoire. Master in Human Movement Sciences. University of the Mediterranee

  • Braun J (1999) On the detection of salient contours. Spat Vis 12(2):211–225

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Burnham WH (1903) Retroactive amnesia: illustrative cases and a tentative explanation. Am J Psychol 14:382–396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter GA, Grossberg S (2003) Adaptive resonance theory. In: Arbib MA (Ed) The handbook of brain theory and neural networks, 2 edn. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 87–90

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins JJ, De Luca CJ (1993) Open-loop and closed-loop control of posture: a random-walk analysis of center-of-pressure trajectories. Exp Brain Res 95(2):308–318

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Collyer CE (1977) Discrimination of spatial and temporal intervals defined by three light flashes: effects of spacing on temporal judgements. Percept Psychophys 21:357–364

    Google Scholar 

  • DeCamp JE (1915) A study of retroactive inhibition. Psychol Monogr 19(4), whole no 84

    Google Scholar 

  • Elder J, Zucker S (1993) The effect of contour closure on the rapid discrimination of two-dimensional shapes. Vision Res 33(7):981–991

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Finke RA, Shepard RN (1986) Visual functions of mental imagery. John Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1929) The reproduction of visually perceived forms. J Exp Psychol 12(1):1–39

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1966) The problem of temporal order in stimulation and perception. J Exp Psychol 62:141–149

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Giraudo MD, Pailhous J (1999) Dynamic instability of visuospatial images. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 25(6):1495–1516

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg S (1987) Competitive learning: from interactive activation to adaptive resonance. Cognit Sci 11:23–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg S, Mingolla E (1985) Neural dynamics of perceptual grouping: textures, boundaries, and emergent segmentations. Percept Psychophys 38:141–171

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hebb DO (1949) Organization of behavior: a neuropsychological theory. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones B, Huang YL (1982) Space–time dependencies in psychological judgment of extent and duration: algebric model of the Tau and Kappa effects. Psychol Bull 91:128–142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koffka K (1935) Principles of Gestalt psychology. Harcourt, Brace and World, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler W (1947) Gestalt psychology. Liveright, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Koshman S (2006) Visualization-based information retrieval on the web. Libr Inf Sci Res 28(2):192–207

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs I, Julesz B (1993) A closed curve is much more than an incomplete one: effect of closure in figure-ground segmentation. Proc Nat Acad Sci 90(16):7495–7497

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley KS (1950) In search of the engram. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Loeb J (1901/1973) Comparative physiology of the brain and comparative psychology. Arno Press, New York

  • Luchins AS, Luchins EH (1959) Rigidity of behavior - a variational approach to the effect of Einstellung. University of Oregon Books, Eugene, Oregon

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM, Corcos DM (1993) Issues in variability and motor control. In: KM Newell, DM Corcos (Eds) Variability and motor control. Human Kinetics Publishers. Champaign IL, pp 1–12

    Google Scholar 

  • Pettet MW, McKee SP, Grzywacz NM (1998) Constraints on long range interactions mediating contour detection. Vision Res 38(6):865–879

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sarrazin JC (2000) Exactitude et variabilité de patrons spatio-temporels. Master in Human Movement Sciences. University of the Mediterranee

  • Sarrazin JC, Giraudo MD, Pailhous J, Bootsma RJ (2004) Dynamics of balancing space and time in memory: tau and kappa effects revisited. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 30(3):411–430

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schöner G, Kelso JA (1988) Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. Science 239(4847):1513–1520

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tiernan JJ (1938) The Principle of Closure in Terms of Recall and Recognition. Am J Psychol 51(1):97–108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky T, Geisler WS, Perry JS (2004) Contour grouping: closure effects are explained by good continuation and proximity. Vision Res 44(24):2769–2777

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Geert P (1998) A dynamic systems model of basic developmental mechanisms: Piaget, Vygotsky, and beyond. Psychol Rev 105(4):634–677

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Wieringen PCW, Beek PJ (1997) Distance versus position information in the control of aiming movements. Behav Brain Sci 20:323–324

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webber CL Jr, Zbilut JP (1994) Dynamical assessment of physiological systems and states using recurrence plot strategies. J Appl Physiol 76(2):965–973

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer M (1923) Untersuchungen zur Lehre der Gestalt, II. Psychologische Forschung

Download references

Acknowledgement

This research was supported by a grant from ACI CNRS “Systèmes Complexes en SHS”.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marie-Dominique Giraudo.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sarrazin, JC., Tonnelier, A., Berberian, B. et al. Structuring process and closure principle in spatial and temporal reproduction tasks. Cogn Neurodyn 2, 273–282 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11571-008-9039-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11571-008-9039-z

Keywords

Navigation