Skip to main content
Log in

Accuracy in judgments of study time predicts academic success in an engineering course

  • Published:
Metacognition and Learning Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present work examines the accuracy of self-reports of study time for college students. In a 10-week Mechanical Engineering course, 99 college students accessed their textbook, homework solutions, graded work, and lecture slides via custom software that recorded objective measures of reading time. In addition, the students provided subjective judgments of the time they spent reading these materials. Comparisons between the objective and subjective measures reveal that students significantly overestimated time with the textbook, homework solutions, graded work, and lecture slides, with higher performing students overestimating to a lesser degree. The difference between objective and subjective judgments of study time correlated significantly and negatively with final course grade for the textbook (r = −.31), homework solutions (r = −.39), and lecture slides (r = −.24), but not for graded work (r = −.05). This study calls into question the utility of self-report data in studies of student study habits, and showcases the value of objective technology-based measures of such habits.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Austin, E. J., Deary, I. J., Gibson, G. J., McGregor, M. J., & Dent, J. B. (1998). Individual response spread in self-report scales: Personality correlations and consequences. Personality and Individual Differences, 24(3), 421–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Azevedo, R., & Aleven, V. (Eds.). (2013). International handbook of metacognition and learning technologies. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bash, K. L., & Kreiner, D. S. (2014). Student perceptions of study time. Psi Chi Journal of Psychological Research, 19(1), 3–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berry, T., Cook, L., Hill, N., & Stevens, K. (2010). An exploratory analysis of textbook usage and study habits: Misperceptions and barriers to success. College Teaching, 59(1), 31–39. https://doi.org/10.1080/87567555.2010.509376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (1963). A model of school learning. Teachers College Record, 64, 723–733.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, J. S., & Gonyea, R. M. (2010). Accuracy of self-reported SAT and ACT test scores: Implications for research. Research in Higher Education, 51(4), 305–319. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11162-009-9160-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, K., French, T., & Cooney, P. J. (2002). Student textbook use in introductory physics. In Paper presented at Physics Education Research Conference 2002. Boise: Idaho.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniel, D. B., & Woody, W. D. (2013). E-textbooks at what cost? Performance and use of electronic v. print texts. Computers & Education, 62, 18–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2012.10.016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Nelson, T. O. (1992). Importance of the kind of cue for judgments of learning (JOL) and the delayed-JOL effect. Memory & Cognition, 20(4), 374–380. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210921.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiorella, L., & Mayer, R. E. (2015). Learning as a generative activity: Eight learning strategies that promote understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Gog, T. (2013). Time on task. In J. Hattie & E. M. Anderman (Eds.), International guide to student achievement (pp. 432–433). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonyea, R. M. (2005) Self‐reported data in institutional research: Review and recommendations. New Directions for Institutional Research, 2005, 73–89. https://doi.org/10.1002/ir.156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Junco, R., & Clem, C. (2015). Predicting course outcomes with digital textbook usage data. The Internet and Higher Education, 27, 54–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.iheduc.2015.06.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuncel, N. R., Credé, M., & Thomas, L. L. (2005). The validity of self-reported grade point averages, class ranks, and test scores: A meta-analysis and review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 75(1), 63–82. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543075001063.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landrum, R. E., Gurung, R. A. R., & Spann, N. (2012). Assessments of textbook usage and the relationship to student course performance. College Teaching, 60(1), 17–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/87567555.2011.609573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masui, C., Broeckmans, J., Doumen, S., Groenen, A., & Molenberghs, G. (2014). Do diligent students perform better? Complex relations between student and course characteristics, study time, and academic performance in higher education. Studies in Higher Education, 39(4), 621–643. https://doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2012.721350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2016). Role of metacognition in STEM games and simulations. In H. F. O'Neil, E. L. Baker, & R. S. Perez (Eds.), Using games and simulations for reaching and assessment (pp. 183–205). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., Stull, A. T., Campbell, J., Almeroth, K., Bimber, B., Chun, D., & Knight, A. (2007). Overestimation Bias in self-reported SAT scores. Educational Psychology Review, 19(4), 443–454. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-006-9034-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognitive judgments and control of study. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(3), 159–163. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01628.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Finn, B. (2008). Evidence that judgments of learning are causally related to study choice. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(1), 174–179. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.1.174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Survey of Student Engagement. (2016). Engagement insights: Survey findings on the quality of undergraduate education—Annual results 2016. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Center for Postsecondary Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, B. J., & Phillips, F. (2007). Sink or skim: Textbook Reading behaviors of introductory accounting students. Issues in Accounting Education, 22(1), 21–44. https://doi.org/10.2308/iace.2007.22.1.21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pilegard, C., & Mayer, R. E. (2015a). Within-subject and between-subject conceptions of metacomprehension accuracy. Learning and Individual Differences, 41, 54–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pilegard, C., & Mayer, R. E. (2015b). Adding judgments of understanding to the metacognitive toolbox. Learning and Individual Differences, 41, 62–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A., Garcia, T., & Mckeachie, W. J. (1993). Reliability and predictive validity of the motivated strategies for learning questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53, 801–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Podolefsky, N., & Finkelstein, N. (2006). The perceived value of college physics textbooks: Students and instructors may not see eye to eye. The Physics Teacher, 44(6), 338–342. https://doi.org/10.1119/1.2336132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, S. R. (2011). Do College student surveys have any validity? The Review of Higher Education, 35(1), 45–76. https://doi.org/10.1353/rhe.2011.0034.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K., Stahovich, T. F., & Mayer, R. E. (2017). Homework and achievement: Using smartpen technology to find the connection. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109, 208–219. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. G., & Tauber, S. K. (2011). The influence of delaying judgments of learning on metacognitive accuracy: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 137(1), 131–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmerón, L., Naumann, J., García, V., & Fajardo, I. (2017). Scanning and deep processing of information in hypertext: An eye tracking and cued retrospective think-aloud study. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 33(3), 222–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sato, H., & Kawahara, J. I. (2011). Selective bias in retrospective self-reports of negative mood states. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 24(4), 359–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G. (2009). Measuring metacognitive judgements. In J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Handbook of metacognition in education (pp. 415–429). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G. (2010). Measuring self-regulation in computer-based learning environments. Educational Psychologist, 45, 258–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., & Dennison, R. S. (1994). Assessing metacognitive awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19(4), 460–475. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1994.1033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., Crippen, K. J., & Hartley, K. (2006). Promoting self-regulation in science education: Metacognition as part of a broader perspective on learning. Research in Science Education, 36(1–2), 111–139. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-005-3917-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schuman, H., Walsh, E., Olson, C., & Etheridge, B. (1985). Effort and reward: The assumption that college grades are affected by quantity of study. Social Forces, 63(4), 945–966. https://doi.org/10.2307/2578600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sikorski, J. F., Rich, K., Saville, B. K., Buskist, W., Drogan, O., Davis, S. F., Griggs, R. A., Jackson, S. L., Marek, P., Boyce, T. E., Geller, E. S., Harvey, J. H., & Hofmann, W. J. (2002). Faculty forum. Teaching of Psychology, 29(4), 312–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, B. D., & Jacobs, D. C. (2003). TextRev: A window into how general and organic chemistry students use textbook resources. Journal of Chemical Education, 80(1), 99. https://doi.org/10.1021/ed080p99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stinebrickner, R., & Stinebrickner, T. R. (2004). Time-use and college outcomes. Journal of Econometrics, 121(1–2), 243–269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jeconom.2003.10.013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M. V., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1(1), 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-006-6893-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walentynowicz, M., Schneider, S., & Stone, A. A. (2018). The effects of time frames on self-report. PLoS One, 13(8), e0201655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M. C., Haertel, G. D., & Walberg, H. J. (1990). What influences learning? A content analysis of review literature. The Journal of Educational Research, 84(1), 30–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1990.10885988.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, A., & Fry, J. D. (2008). Metacognitive awareness and academic achievement in college students. Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 8(2), 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This project was supported by the National Science Foundation under Award Numbers 0935239, 1432820, and 1612511.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas F. Stahovich.

Ethics declarations

The authors listed on this manuscript declare that they have no conflicts of interest to report.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gyllen, J.G., Stahovich, T.F., Mayer, R.E. et al. Accuracy in judgments of study time predicts academic success in an engineering course. Metacognition Learning 14, 215–228 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-019-09207-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-019-09207-6

Keywords

Navigation