Abstract
Multidomain interventions have shown tremendous potential for improving cognition in older adults. It is unclear if multidomain interventions can be delivered remotely and whether remote intervention is beneficial for older adults who are vulnerable or at risk of cognitive decline. In a 26-week multi-site, home-based, double-blind, randomized controlled trial, 120 cognitively healthy older adults (75 robust, 45 pre-frail; age range = 60–94) recruited from Switzerland, Canada, and Belgium were randomized to receive either the StayFitLonger (SFL) computerized multidomain training program or an active control intervention. Delivered on tablets, the SFL intervention combined adapted physical exercises (strength, balance, and mobility), cognitive training (divided attention, problem solving, and memory), opportunities for social and contributive interactions, and psychoeducation. The active control intervention provided basic mobilization exercises and access to video games. Cognitive outcomes were global cognition (Z-scores of attention, verbal fluency, and episodic memory for nondemented older adults; ZAVEN), memory, executive function, and processing speed. Linear mixed model analyses indicated improved performance on the ZAVEN global cognition score in the SFL group but not in the active control group. Stratified analyses by frailty status revealed improved ZAVEN global cognition and processing speed scores following SFL in the pre-frail group but not in the robust group. Overall, the study indicates that a computerized program providing a multidomain intervention at home can improve cognition in older adults. Importantly, pre-frail individuals, who are at higher risk of cognitive decline, seem to benefit more from the intervention. Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov, NCT037519 Registered on January 22, 2020—Retrospectively registered, https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT04237519.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
Age-related cognitive decline is associated with modifiable risk factors that can be addressed with non-pharmacological approaches [1]. For this reason, multidomain prevention programs that target a subset of modifiable factors have been developed to promote cognitive health in older adults [2]. The positive impact of multidomain interventions has been observed in a few studies that evaluated their effect in older adults at risk of cognitive decline. For example, the FINGER study, which combined face-to-face physical activity with computerized cognitive training [3], reported a positive effect on overall cognition and a reduced risk of cognitive decline. Thus, prevention programs have enormous potential to protect older adults from the deleterious effects of brain aging on cognition, which can ultimately preserve their independence [2, 4].
While prior studies reported encouraging effects, some issues remain to be addressed. The first relates to the accessibility and flexibility of face-to-face multidomain interventions. Older adults may have mobility challenges or live in remote areas without access to community resources providing face-to-face interventions. With the increase in technological literacy among older adults, there has been considerable recent interest in developing computerized programs to deliver home-based interventions. These interventions can increase flexibility of use, reduce costs, and thus facilitate the scaling up of interventions. Computerized programs allow for real-time feedback on performance, control of item timing, and gamification, among other advantages. Surprisingly, only a few studies have evaluated at-home physical activity training or multidomain programs [5,6,7,8,9].
A second important issue to address is interindividual variability in response to computerized multidomain interventions. From a personalized medicine perspective, it is important to know the responders and their characteristics. In the present study, we examined efficacy as a function of frailty status–defined as a state of heightened vulnerability due to impairment of multiple systems [10, 11]. Frailty is an important predictor of loss of independence and cognitive decline and thus, a highly relevant marker of vulnerability in old age [12]. Predictions is based on two frameworks: The compensation/reserve model posits that vulnerable older adults will benefit the most from these interventions, while the magnification model posits that cognitive improvement following an intervention involves brain plasticity and that the fittest individuals will benefit most because their brain is more plastic [13, 14].
Here, we report on a 26-week double-blind parallel-group randomized controlled trial (RCT), which examined the cognitive effects of the home-based computerized multidomain intervention StayFitLonger (SFL), combining physical exercise and cognitive training, compared to an active control condition. Results are reported for the full sample and then separately for pre-frail and robust older adults. We hypothesized that the SFL group would have a larger pre-post intervention effect than the control group. As the compensation model has been most often supported, we predicted a larger SFL advantage in pre-frail participants compared to robust ones.
Methods
The study was pre-registered (ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT04237519) and follows the recommendations of the updated Consolidated Standards of Reporting Statement [15, 16]. All procedures were reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Board (REB) in each country: Switzerland: REB Canton de Vaud (application #2018–01898, last approval December 4 2018); Canada: REB vieillissement-neuroimagerie of the CIUSSS-CSMTL (application #18–19–29, last approval December 14 2018); Belgium: REB Cliniques Universitaires Saint-Luc, UCLouvain, Bruxelles (application #B403201941535, last approval October 15 2019). The nature, benefits, and risks of the study were explained to all subjects, and their written informed consent was obtained prior to participation. The cognitive outcomes reported here were identified as secondary outcomes. The primary outcome and secondary psychosocial outcomes are reported separately. As the protocol of the SFL study was published previously [17], only the main aspects of the methods are described.
Design
The efficacy trial was a 26-week double-blind parallel group multi-centric RCT. Participants were randomized to either the SFL home-based computerized multidomain intervention or a home-based active control intervention. Outcome measures were collected at pre-training (PRE; within 6 weeks prior to the start of the intervention) and post-training (POST, within 4 weeks following the end of the intervention). Randomization was done independently from the research team with a 1:1 ratio stratified based on the frailty status using REDCap. Participants were blinded to the nature of their intervention (experimental vs comparator), and assessors were blinded to the hypotheses and the participants’ assignment. Statistical analyses were blinded to the intervention condition.
Study population and entry criteria
Participants were recruited from three sites: Centre Leenaards de la mémoire – Centre hospitalier universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Switzerland; Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal of the Centre intégré universitaire de santé et de services sociaux Centre-Sud-de-l’Île-de-Montréal (CIUSSS-CSMTL), Canada; and Brusano and Centre Public d’Action Sociale (CPAS) of Woluwe-Saint-Lambert, Belgium. The participant flow is shown in Fig. 1. Of the 161 participants tested for eligibility, 120 were randomized (64 in Switzerland, 32 in Canada, and 24 in Belgium). Fifty-nine were allocated to the SFL intervention and 61 to the active control. As participants from the Belgian site were included during the COVID-19 pandemic, the introductory courses, which were provided in group sessions in the other sites [17], were provided to participants through videos followed by a home visit from the instructor.
Included participants were fluent French-speaking community-dwelling adults aged 60 years and over with normal scores on the 4-Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (4-IADL) scale [18], a score ≥ 26 on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [19], a score < 3 on the Fried’s frailty index [11], no motor or vision problems, no current neurological or psychiatric diagnoses (e.g., Parkinson’s disease), and access to a wireless Internet connection at home. Participants were identified as either robust (score of 0) or pre-frail (score of 1 or 2) based on Fried’s index.
Interventions
Interventions were provided on a tablet (Samsung Galaxy Tab S2) and took place at home. Participants received occasional home visits and monthly phone calls to monitor their use and address any problems with the program. The mean overall time (in hours) that each group spent using the program was recorded and will be only briefly summarized here as it will be the topic of a separate publication on adherence (see design paper [17]).
SFL intervention
The SFL intervention included physical and cognitive training activities. The physical exercises (Exercise) focused on strength, balance, and mobility with various difficulty levels [20]. Cognitive training included activities for divided attention [21], problem solving [22, 23], and memory [24]. To increase adherence and social interactions, participants had access to a moderated Chat Room, the possibility to create material for the activities, psycho-educational content, and gamification elements (e.g., rewards, leaderboards). A customizable virtual guide provided participants with instructions, reminders, and feedback. Participants were asked to engage in physical exercise at least 3 days per week for 30–45 min and cognitive exercise for at least three 15-min sessions per week.
Active control intervention
The active control intervention had similar structure, timing, and organization as the SFL program. Physical exercises included advice and tips to stay physically active and exercises to train strength, mobility, and balance of the upper and lower extremities. Unlike the SFL, the active control only had a limited number of physical exercises and did not include interactive videos, personalization, chat rooms, psycho-educational content, or virtual guide. The cognitive activities were commercially available games that did not target specific cognitive processes or strategies [25,26,27,28,29] (e.g., crossword puzzles, Sudoku, maze arcade).
Outcome variables
Global cognition was measured with an adapted version of the ZAVEN composite score [30], which is the averaged z-scores from the delayed free recall of the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), delayed recall of the Wechsler Memory Scale-IV logical memory subtest [31], number of correct symbols reported in the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS)-IV, digit symbol substitution test (DSST) [32], and letter fluency (the letter P at the pre-training and R at the post-training) [33]. An executive function composite score was computed by combining z-scores from the letter fluency test, Trail Making Test (TMT) part B-A (time) [34], interference index of the Victoria Stroop Test [35], and number of omissions on the divided attention subtest of the Test of Attention Performance [36]. A memory composite score was obtained from the delayed free recall score of the CVLT [37, 38] and logical memory task. A processing speed composite score was obtained from the TMT part A (time), number of correct answers on the DSST, and the naming condition of the Victoria Stroop Test (time) [39]. Scores were inverted, when necessary, so that larger scores always reflected better performance. The composite scores were computed by standardizing performance on individual tests using the baseline mean and standard deviation (SD) of the entire group. A preliminary internal consistency analysis was conducted to contextualise the measures. This analysis was particularly relevant for the executive function, memory, and processing speed composite scores because they were meant to reflect a single cognitive construct. In contrast, the ZAVEN composite score was developed to diagnose preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and intended to cover multiple cognitive domains to provide greater sensitivity to cognitive decline. Because differences in expectations might explain some of the intervention effects, participant’s expectations were measured at PRE and POST on a 15-item ad hoc questionnaire on a 7-point Likert scale.
Statistical analyses
The sample size was determined with a Marker Stratified DesignFootnote 1 considering a dropout rate of about 25% based on prior studies. All statistical tests were two-tailed with a p value < 0.05. Groups were compared for demographics and baseline characteristics with t-tests or chi-square analyses. A linear mixed model was used to analyze the intervention effect controlled for age, sex, education, score on MoCA at baseline, and site. The fixed effects were intervention (SFL vs. active control), time (PRE, POST), and their interaction. In the presence of a significant interaction, post hoc comparisons were computed between PRE and POST in each group and mean and confidence intervals were assessed on pre-post change scores. Separate analyses were computed for each outcome. Significant interactions and group differences in favor of the SFL at POST and on change scores were expected if the SFL intervention was more beneficial than the active control. All analyses were first performed with the total sample, followed by separate analyses for pre-frail and robust individuals. A comparison of the clinical and socio-demographic characteristics of participants, who completed vs withdrew from the study, are presented in Supplementary Table 1. To comply with an intention-to-treat (ITT) approach, all randomized participants were included in the model, and the characteristics of participants who withdrew were compared to those remaining in the study (Supplementary Table 1). The effect of sex and other controlled variables on the cognitive outcomes are shown in Supplementary Table 3.
Results
The mean age of the total sample was 71.33 years (range = 60–94; SD = 5.87). The average score on the MoCA was 28.97 (range = 26–30; SD = 1.17). 79/120 of participants were women. Table 1 reports the baseline characteristics of the sample as a function of the intervention condition and frailty status. There were no differences at baseline for sociodemographic or clinical variables between participants in the SFL vs. active control intervention (Supplementary Table 1). Mean time weekly spent using the program was 2.6 (SD = 0.3) and 3.8 (SD = 0.4) hours for the total SFL group and the active control condition, respectively; 2.4 (SD = 0.4) and 3.4 (SD = 0.7) hours for the pre-frail SFL group and the active control condition, respectively; and 2.7 (SD = 0.4) and 4.0 (SD = 0.4) hours for the robust SFL group and the active control condition, respectively. Cronbach alpha values for the composite scores were 0.54, 0.73, 0.54, and 0.66 for the ZAVEN, memory, executive, and speed composite scores, respectively.
Total sample
Figure 2 shows results for global cognition (Fig. 2A), executive function (Fig. 2B), processing speed (Fig. 2C), and memory (Fig. 2D) composite scores. For global cognition, the mixed model indicated no effect of intervention, F(1, 110.9) = 0.33, p = 0.57, or time, F(1, 109.7) = 2.20, p = 0.14, but there was an intervention × time interaction, F(1, 109.6) = 6.44, p = 0.01, effect size = 0.297. The estimated global cognition mean POST–PRE change score for the SFL intervention group was 0.14, 95% CI [0.04, 0.25]. The estimated global cognition mean POST–PRE change score for the active control group was − 0.04, 95% CI [− 0.14, 0.60].
The mixed model for executive function indicated no intervention, F(1, 109.8) = 1.47, p = 0.23, time, F(1, 111.1) = 0.37, p = 0.55, or intervention × time interaction, F(1, 110.9) = 0.12, p = 0.73. For processing speed, there was a time effect, F(1, 109.6) = 10.08, p = 0.002, but no intervention, F(1, 110.6) = 2.43, p = 0.12, or intervention × time interaction, F(1, 109.5) = 2.52, p = 0.12. The mixed model for the memory composite score indicated a time effect, F(1, 109.4) = 19.10, p < 0.001, but no intervention effect, F(1, 110.1) = 0.21, p = 0.65, or intervention × time interaction, F(1,109.2) = 1.98, p = 0.16.
Pre-frail group
Figure 3 shows the global cognition (Fig. 3A), executive (Fig. 3B), processing speed (Fig. 3C), and memory (Fig. 3D) composite scores for the pre-frail group. An intervention × time interaction was expected to support a larger effect due to the SFL intervention.
For global cognition, the mixed model indicated no effect of intervention, F(1, 35.6) = 0.01, p = 0.92, or time, F(1, 37.6) = 0.18, p = 0.68, but there was an intervention × time interaction, F(1, 37.5) = 7.18, p = 0.01, effect size = 0.451. The estimated global cognition mean POST–PRE change score for the SFL intervention group was 0.20, 95% CI [0.01, 0.38]. Estimated global cognition mean POST–PRE change scores for the active control group was − 0.14, 95% CI [− 0.32, − 0.04]. For the executive function composite score, there was no intervention effect, F(1, 35.9) = 0.06, p = 0.81, time effect, F(1, 38.3) = 0.99, p = 0.32, or intervention × time interaction, F(1, 38.2) = 0.48, p = 0.49. The mixed model for the processing speed composite score indicated no Intervention effect, F(1, 35.9) = 0.001, p = 0.97, but there was a time effect, F(1, 37.6) = 4.08, p = 0.05, and an intervention × time interaction, F(1, 37.5) = 7.41, p = 0.01, effect size = 0.379. The estimated processing speed mean POST–PRE change score for the SFL intervention group was 0.38, 95% CI [0.15, 0.62]. The estimated processing speed mean POST–PRE change scores for the active control group was − 0.06, 95% CI [-0.29, 0.17]. The mixed model for the memory composite score indicated no effect of intervention, F(1, 36.2) = 0.73, time, F(1, 38.0) = 1.82, or intervention × time interaction, F(1, 37.9) = 1.40.
Robust group
Figure 4 shows results for the global cognition (Fig. 4A), executive function (Fig. 4B), processing speed (Fig. 4C), and memory (Fig. 4D) composite scores. For global cognition, the mixed model indicated no effect of intervention, F(1, 66.2) = 0.52, p = 0.47, time, F(1, 70.4) = 2.18, p = 0.14, or intervention × time interaction, F(1, 70.4) = 1.18, p = 0.28. The estimated global cognition mean POST–PRE change score for the SFL intervention group was 0.11, 95% CI [− 0.02, 0.24]. The estimated global cognition mean POST–PRE change scores for the active control group was − 0.02, 95% CI [− 0.10, 0.13]. The mixed model for the executive function composite score indicated an intervention effect, F(1, 65.5) = 4.04, p = 0.048, but no time, F(1, 70.6) = 2.52, p = 0.12, or intervention × time interaction, F(1, 70.6) = 0.04, p = 0.85. The mixed model for the processing speed composite score indicated an intervention effect, F(1, 66.4) = 5.73, p = 0.019, and a time effect, F(1, 70.4) = 5.64, p = 0.02, but no intervention × time interaction, F(1, 70.4) = 0.83, p = 0.77. The mixed model for the memory composite score indicated a time effect, F(1, 69.8) = 18.84, p < 0.001, but no intervention, F(1, 65.8) = 1.37, p = 0.25, or intervention × time interaction, F(1, 69.8) = 0.74, p = 0.39.
The analyses on expectations showed no intervention × time interaction in the total sample, robust, or pre-frail groups, indicating that changes in expectations cannot account for the intervention effect (Supplementary Table 2). Regarding the controlled variables, we observed a site effect as participants from the Swiss site performed better than those in the Canadian and Belgian sites (Supplementary Table 3). We also found a sex effect in favor of women for the Zaven global cognition and memory scores.
Discussion
This RCT assessed the efficacy of a computerized multidomain home-based intervention combining physical and cognitive exercises on the cognition of older adults. The ZAVEN global cognition score indicated a significant intervention × time interaction as the cognition of participants improved in the SFL intervention after training, unlike those in the active control condition. This finding is consistent with the FINGER study, which reported positive effects for a 2-year multidomain intervention on global cognition [3]. Unlike the FINGER study, which used usual care as a control, we used an active control condition where participants received physical activity guidelines and access to low-stimulation cognitive games. Furthermore, our study demonstrates a positive effect even though the duration is shorter than that of the FINGER study (6 months versus 2 years) and even though the intervention was provided remotely.
The positive effect found here deviates from the results summarized by Whitfield et al.’s [5] meta-analysis of four RCTs consisting of remotely delivered multidomain interventions, which reported no cognitive improvement. However, Whitfield et al.’s results should be interpreted with caution given the small number of studies. Furthermore, there are important differences between this study and those reviewed by Whitfield et al. One relates to the intervention content as the SFL intervention includes an individualized, progressive physical activity program with numerous illustrative videos, and empirically supported gamified cognitive exercises.
Another important aspect here was to examine effects on pre-frail individuals, who demonstrated a better response to training, which could have increased our ability to detect an intervention effect. Indeed, we found that pre-frail individuals randomized to the SFL intervention improved their global cognition and processing speed scores after the intervention, unlike participants randomized to the active control condition and unlike robust participants enrolled in either intervention. Thus, the effect observed when examining the entire group seems to be largely driven by the pre-frail participants, who showed a stronger response to this multidomain intervention. The effect found on the speed composite score might suggest that the improvement in processing speed drove the improvement in the ZAVEN global cognition score. Note, however, that there were benefits from the intervention when looking at the data from the other cognitive domains, even though these were non-significant. Hence, future research should focus on determining the cognitive domains that benefit most from similar interventions. The observed difference between pre-frail and robust individuals is consistent with the reserve/compensation hypothesis, which posits that vulnerable individuals are more likely to benefit from interventions designed to compensate for their difficulties, weaknesses or disabilities [13, 14]. There is some indication from prior studies that interventions may be beneficial to those who need them most, particularly if they are tailored to the characteristics of the target population (e.g., [40]). The physical exercises used here focused on strength and balance with a gradual, self-managed approach tailored to the sedentary older person. Similarly, our cognitive exercises were playful, which may be especially supportive for more vulnerable older adults. This underscores the importance of taking individual differences into account when designing and prescribing multidomain intervention programs.
The study has limitations that should be acknowledged: First, participants for the Belgian site were recruited and tested during the COVID-19 pandemic. Although we observed a site effect, this was due to performance of the Belgian and Canadian sites, being lower than the Swiss site. Thus, there is no indication for an effect specific to the Belgian site and no evidence that it modified the intervention effect. Second, frail individuals were excluded from our sample because we focused on prevention, but it could be interesting to examine whether the program has a positive effect on cognition in frail older adults. The sample size was estimated based on the physical outcome. Two of the composite scores used, executive and global cognition, showed low internal consistency. This indicates that they may reflect more than one cognitive construct. This was expected for the global cognition but not for the executive composite score. While it is an important issue, we did not include data on transfer to real-world daily functioning as our focus here was on cognition. Finally, the use of a purely computer-based intervention requires older adults to be technologically literate, which means that our group was biased toward those with technological skills.
In conclusion, we report positive effects of a multidomain remote intervention on cognition in older adults and propose that pre-frail older adults may benefit most from the program. One other important feature of the study was the use of a computerized program that allowed the intervention to be conducted entirely in the participantʼs home, which has rarely been done in past studies. Using a computerized remote approach has many benefits: It reaches a larger audience than face-to-face interventions, it is cost-effective in the long-term, it increases accessibility and flexibility, and it allows for personalization of the activities. The finding that more vulnerable older adults benefit most from an intervention to reduce cognitive decline provides support for public health interventions that encourage prevention strategies in older adults by specifically targeting a vulnerable population.
Change history
17 July 2023
A Correction to this paper has been published: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11357-023-00868-5
Notes
Marker-by-treatment interaction http://www.bigted.org/NonAdaptiveDesigns/MarkerStratifiedDesigns.html
References
Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396(10248):413–46.
Norton S, Matthews FE, Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Brayne C. Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based data. Lancet Neurol. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70136-X.
Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, Levalahti E, Ahtiluoto S, Antikainen R, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(15)60461-5.
Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, Ames D, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6.
Whitfield T, McConnell B, Renouf P, Mansour H, Zabihi S, Aguirre E, et al. The effect of remotely delivered lifestyle interventions on cognition in older adults without dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2021.101505.
Lee KS, Lee Y, Back JH, Son SJ, Choi SH, Chung Y-K, et al. Effects of a multidomain lifestyle modification on cognitive function in older adults: an eighteen-month community-based cluster randomized controlled trial. Psychother Psychosom. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1159/000360820.
Richard E, van Charante EPM, Hoevenaar-Blom MP, Coley N, Barbera M, van der Groep A, et al. Healthy ageing through internet counselling in the elderly (HATICE): a multinational, randomised controlled trial. Lancet Digital Health. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2589-7500(19)30153-0.
Roh HW, Hong CH, Lim HK, Chang KJ, Kim H, Kim N-R, et al. A 12-week multidomain intervention for late-life depression: a community-based randomized controlled trial. J Affect Disord. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.12.013.
Vanoh D, Shahar S, Razali R, Ali NM, Manaf ZA, Mohd Noah SA, et al. The effectiveness of a web-based health education tool, WESIHAT 2.0, among older adults: a randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;70(s1):S255–S70.
Fried LP, Ferrucci L, Darer J, Williamson JD, Anderson G. Untangling the concepts of disability, frailty, and comorbidity: implications for improved targeting and care. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/59.3.M255.
Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, Newman AB, Hirsch C, Gottdiener J, et al. Frailty in older adults: evidence for a phenotype. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2001. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/56.3.M146.
Borges MK, Canevelli M, Cesari M, Aprahamian I. Frailty as a predictor of cognitive disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Med. 2019. https://doi.org/10.3389/fmed.2019.00026.
Willis SL, Belleville S. Cognitive training in later adulthood. Handbook of the psychology of aging. 8th ed. Academic Press; 2016. p. 219–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-411469-2.00012-1.
Lövdén M, Brehmer Y, Li S-C, Lindenberger U. Training-induced compensation versus magnification of individual differences in memory performance. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00141.
Boutron I, Altman DG, Moher D, Schulz KF, Ravaud P. CONSORT statement for randomized trials of nonpharmacologic treatments: a 2017 update and a CONSORT extension for nonpharmacologic trial abstracts. Ann Intern Med. 2017. https://doi.org/10.7326/M17-0046.
Witham MD, Stott DJ. Conducting and reporting trials for older people. Age Ageing. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afx153.
Belleville S, Cuesta M, Bieler-Aeschlimann M, Giacomino K, Widmer A, Hager AM, et al. Rationale and protocol of the StayFitLonger study: a multicentre trial to measure efficacy and adherence of a home-based computerised multidomain intervention in healthy older adults. BMC Geriatr. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-020-01709-2.
Barberger-Gateau P, Fabrigoule C, Rouch I, Letenneur L, Dartigues JF. Neuropsychological correlates of self-reported performance in instrumental activities of daily living and prediction of dementia. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1999. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/54B.5.P293.
Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x.
Mittaz Hager AG, Mathieu N, Lenoble-Hoskovec C, Swanenburg J, de Bie R, Hilfiker R. Effects of three home-based exercise programmes regarding falls, quality of life and exercise-adherence in older adults at risk of falling: protocol for a randomized controlled trial. BMC Geriatr. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-018-1021-y.
Belleville S, Mellah S, de Boysson C, Demonet JF, Bier B. The pattern and loci of training-induced brain changes in healthy older adults are predicted by the nature of the intervention. PLoS ONE. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0102710.
Warmington M, Hitch GJ. Enhancing the learning of new words using an errorless learning procedure: Evidence from typical adults. Memory. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.807841.
Dresler M, Shirer WR, Konrad BN, Muller NCJ, Wagner IC, Fernandez G, et al. Mnemonic Training Reshapes Brain Networks to Support Superior Memory. Neuron. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.003.
Hering A, Rendell PG, Rose NS, Schnitzspahn KM, Kliegel M. Prospective memory training in older adults and its relevance for successful aging. Psychol Res. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0566-4.
Chesham A, Wyss P, Müri RM, Mosimann UP, Nef T. What older people like to play: genre preferences and acceptance of casual games. JMIR Serious Games. 2017;5(2):e8.
Brooker H, Wesnes KA, Ballard C, Hampshire A, Aarsland D, Khan Z, et al. The relationship between the frequency of number-puzzle use and baseline cognitive function in a large online sample of adults aged 50 and over. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.5085.
Jin G, Li K, Qin Y, Zhong N, Zhou H, Wang Z, et al. fMRI study in posterior cingulate and adjacent precuneus cortex in healthy elderly adults using problem solving task. J Neurol Sci. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2012.02.032.
Ferreira N, Owen A, Mohan A, Corbett A, Ballard C. Associations between cognitively stimulating leisure activities, cognitive function and age-related cognitive decline. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4155.
Murphy M, O’Sullivan K, Kelleher KG. Daily crosswords improve verbal fluency: a brief intervention study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4079.
Lim YY, Snyder PJ, Pietrzak RH, Ukiqi A, Villemagne VL, Ames D, et al. Sensitivity of composite scores to amyloid burden in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Introducing the Z-scores of Attention, Verbal fluency, and Episodic memory for Nondemented older adults composite score. Alzheimers Dement. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2015.11.003.
Wechsler D. MEM-IV Echelle clinique de memoire de Wechsler. 4th ed. MN: Pearson; 2012. Available from: https://www.ecpa.fr/psychologie-clinique/test.asp?id=1987.
Wechsler D. WAIS-IV Nouvelle version de lʼéchelle dʼintelligence de Wechsler pour adultes. 4th ed. MN: Pearson; 2011. Available from: https://www.ecpa.fr/psychologie-clinique/test.asp?id=1968.
Cardebat D, Doyon B, Puel M, Goulet P, Joanette Y. Formal and semantic lexical evocation in normal subjects. Performance and dynamics of production as a function of sex, age and educational level. Acta Neurol Belg. 1990;90(4):207–17.
Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8.
Bayard S, Erkes J, Moroni C. Victoria Stroop Test: normative data in a sample group of older people and the study of their clinical applications in the assessment of inhibition in Alzheimerʼs disease. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2011. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acr053.
Leclercq M, Zimmermann P, H. van Zomeren A. Applied neuropsychology of attention: theory, diagnosis and rehabilitation. Taylor & Francis ed. Edition s, editor. London: Psychology Press; 2002.
Delis DC, Fine EM, Stricker JL, Houston WS, Wetter SR, Cobell K, et al. Comparison of the traditional recall-based versus a new list-based method for computing semantic clustering on the California Verbal Learning Test: evidence from Alzheimerʼs disease. Clin Neuropsychol. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854040903002232.
Poitrenaud J, Deweer B, Kalafat M, Van Der Linden M. CVLT Test dʼapprentissage et de mémoire verbale. MN: Pearson; 2017; Available from: https://www.ecpa.fr/psychologie-clinique/test.asp?id=1803.
Moroni C, Bayard S. Inhibitory process: what evolution after the age of 50? Psychol Neuropsychiatr Vieil. 2009;7(2):121–9.
Belleville S, Cloutier S, Mellah S, Willis S, Vellas B, Andrieu S, et al. Is more always better? Dose effect in a multidomain intervention in older adults at risk of dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 2022. https://doi.org/10.1002/alz.12544.
Acknowledgements
We thank the members of the national teams who were involved in contacting, assessing, training, and supervising participants or provided logistical support: Tania Javaux, Thomas Genoud-Prachex, Alexandre Pinault, and Benedetta Leidi-Maimone in Switzerland; Mehdi Essounni, Vincent Marcangeli, Maxime Bergevin, Carole-Anne Lachance, Marie-Claude Veilleux, Ève Picard, and Jordan Granet in Canada; Elise Grimm, Gaëtan Devos, Christophe Dohn, Pia Vandebergh, Virginie Leblanc, and Martine Callaert in Belgium. Thanks to Annie Webb for English editing, Ali Filali for statistical support, and Stéphanie Grégoire and Charlotte Bédard-Delisle for their help in preparing the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by a grant from the Active and Assisted Living Programme (AAL-call-068–2017) in partnership with national sponsors. In Switzerland, the project is funded by the AAL association, and the Swiss Confederation represented by the State Secretariat for Education, Research and Innovation. In Belgium, the project is funded by the AAL association and the “Gouvernement de la region de Bruxelles Capitale.” In Canada, the work was supported by a grant from the following sponsors: Networks of Centres of Excellence AGE-WELL and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (AW-AAL 2017; 36 months starting on October 1, 2017). MC’s salary was supported by the AGE-WELL grant. The sponsors were not involved in the design, methods, recruitment, data collection, analysis, or preparation of the paper. SB holds a Canada Research Chair Tier 1 in Cognitive Neuroscience of Aging and Brain Plasticity. LB is supported by the Mirella and Lino Saputo Chair from Université de Montreal at the Montreal Heart Institute. NB is supported by a research scholar award from the Fonds de la recherche du Québec-Santé.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
SB has been a consultant for research development on the prevention of Alzheimer’s disease for the Fondation IUGM (2016), Sojecci/Lucilab (2017 to current), and the development of a cognitive stimulation programme for the Centre de promotion de la Santé Avant Âge (2015). She has intellectual property rights on the “Programme de Stimulation pour une santé cognitive, Memoria, Batterie d’évaluation de la mémoire Côte-des-Neiges’ and ‘MEMO, Méthode d’Entrainement pour une Mémoire Optimale.” DPM and SC are employees of MindMaze SA. The remaining authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
This article is published under an open access license. Please check the 'Copyright Information' section either on this page or in the PDF for details of this license and what re-use is permitted. If your intended use exceeds what is permitted by the license or if you are unable to locate the licence and re-use information, please contact the Rights and Permissions team.
About this article
Cite this article
Belleville, S., Cuesta, M., Bieler-Aeschlimann, M. et al. Pre-frail older adults show improved cognition with StayFitLonger computerized home–based training: a randomized controlled trial. GeroScience 45, 811–822 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11357-022-00674-5
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11357-022-00674-5