Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Framing innocence: an experimental test of the effects of wrongful convictions on public opinion

  • Published:
Journal of Experimental Criminology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives

Discourse about criminal justice in the USA increasingly revolves around wrongful convictions. Research has documented the emergence of the “innocence frame,” but relatively little is known about its effects on public opinion. We utilize framing theory to examine how various presentations of wrongful conviction information affect attitudes toward the justice system and highlight the consequences of the innocence movement for public opinion.

Methods

We implement two survey experiments to test the effects of innocence information for criminal justice attitudes. In the first experiment, we test the impact of wrongful conviction numbers relative to a control group for death penalty support. In the second experiment, we analyze the effects—both separately and jointly—of exoneration numbers and a wrongful conviction narrative relative to a control group for attitudes toward the death penalty and police reform, trust in the justice system, and personal concern.

Results

We demonstrate that the presentation of factual numbers of exonerations reduces support for capital punishment and erodes trust in the justice system, but fails to garner support for police reforms or increase personal concern about wrongful convictions. However, a narrative about an individual wrongful conviction predictably has more pronounced effects on death penalty attitudes and increases personal concern and support for police reform, but has little effect on trust in the justice system more broadly.

Conclusions

Wrongful convictions are consequential for public opinion, but the effects are contingent on how the information is framed and the attitudinal outcome of interest. Our findings have implications for criminal justice attitudes and policy, the innocence movement, and framing theory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See Butler et al. (2018) for a replication effort focused on the racial-argument treatment in Peffley and Hurwitz (2007).

  2. Simmons (2017) examined news exposure and support for punitive policies, finding that exposure may increase punitiveness. Simmons suggests that this may be due to the inclusion of more episodic frames in local television news coverage than in print media, but does not assess or manipulate the frames, nor does the study analyze non-punitive attitudes.

  3. We opted for a no information control rather than an unrelated information message (i.e., numbers about something else). We think that no information versus treatment information provides a cleaner test of hypotheses, and it eliminates the risk that what is assumed to be “benign” information in a control group activates unforeseen considerations.

  4. Matching incorporates age, race, education, gender, party identification, ideology, employment, political interest, and born-again status.

  5. Using the dichotomous dependent variable, the unweighted percentages of support for the death penalty by experimental condition are 69.17% for the control group and 62.13% for the treatment group.

  6. The sample was matched based on age, gender, ethnicity, region, and partisanship.

  7. This figure was based on common estimates of a wrongful conviction rate, which generally fall between 1 and 5% (e.g., Gross et al. 2014). There are more than 2.1 million people incarcerated in the USA (Kaeble and Glaze 2016); thus, an error rate of 5% would equate to more than 100,000 innocents incarcerated.

  8. We thought this rhetoric might resonate with conservatives and death penalty supporters; one innocence advocate described the “true-perpetrator angle” as the “Republican pitch” for wrongful conviction reform (Norris 2017, 158). For example, New Jersey State Senator Joseph Pennacchio (R), a “law and order conservative,” recently stated, “If we convict somebody wrongfully, that means the person who committed the crime is still out there.” (Sullivan 2017).

  9. We pre-tested our treatment on a sample of university students. Regarding the certainty of innocence based on DNA evidence, 95% of respondents in our pre-test sample reported that they believed the individual was innocent after reading the narrative. The remaining individuals selected “do not know”; no respondent believed he was “guilty.” Data from the Social Security Administration reveals that Michael was in the top 15 most common names from 1950 to 2017. According to 2010 Census data, Williams is the third most common surname and is almost equally divided between white (45.8%) and black (47.7%) individuals.

  10. These are two well-known examples of miscarriages of justice in popular culture. The Serial podcast was downloaded more than 100 million times within its first 2 years and it is estimated that Making a Murderer was watched nearly 20 million times within a month of its release (Nededog 2016; Nyman 2016).

  11. If the 7-point variable is simplified to oppose/support, the raw percentages of support for the death penalty by condition are as follows: 66.09% for the control group, 59.22% for the numbers frame, 55.24% for the narrative frame, and 53.63% for the combined frame.

  12. Because the response options have a meaningful ordering, we employ ordered-logistic regression. Analyses using multinomial logit do not alter inferences about treatment effects.

  13. The raw percentage supporting the death penalty using the 3-item measure are as follows: 53.75% for the control group, 46.3% for the numbers frame, 42.14% for the narrative frame, and 40% for the combined frame.

  14. In the model without controls, the narrative slightly reduces trust in the system, but this is only significant if a one-tailed test of significance is used (see Supplementary Materials).

References

  • Aaroe, L. (2011). Investigating frame strength: the case of episodic and thematic frames. Political Communication, 28, 207–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Acker, J. R. (2013). The flipside injustice of wrongful convictions: when the guilty go free. Albany Law Review, 76, 1629–1712.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Community Survey. (2012). Retrieved from https://www.census.gov/programs-surveys/acs/guidance/comparing-acs-data/2012.html. Accessed 16 May 2019

  • Alexander, M. (2012). The new Jim Crow: mass incarceration in the age of colorblindness. New York: New Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Applegate, B. K., & Sanborn, J. B. (2011). Public opinion on the harshness of local courts: an experimental test of question wording effects. Criminal Justice Review, 36, 487–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumer, E. P., Messner, S. F., & Rosenfeld, R. (2003). Explaining spatial variation in support for capital punishment: a multilevel analysis. American Journal of Sociology, 108, 844–875.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumgartner, F. R., DeBoef, S. L., & Boydstun, A. E. (2008). The decline of the death penalty and the discovery of innocence. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumgartner, F. R., Grigg, A., Ramirez, R., Rose, K. J., & Lucy, J. S. (2018). The mayhem of wrongful liberty: documenting the crimes of true perpetrators in cases of wrongful incarceration. Albany Law Review, 81, 1261–1286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, J. G., Clow, K. A., & Ricciardelli, R. (2008). Causes of wrongful conviction: looking at student knowledge. Journal of Criminal Justice Education, 19, 75–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobo, L. D., & Johnson, D. (2004). A taste for punishment: black and white Americans’ views on the death penalty and the war on drugs. Du Bois Review, 1, 151–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borg, M. J. (1997). The southern subculture of punitiveness? Regional variation in support for capital punishment. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 34, 25–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brace, P., & Boyea, B. D. (2008). State public opinion, the death penalty, and the practice of electing judges. American Journal of Political Science, 52, 360–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brace, P. R., & Hall, M. G. (1997). The interplay of preferences, case facts, context, and rules in the politics of judicial choice. Journal of Politics, 59, 1206–1231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burstein, P. (2003). The impact of public opinion on public policy: a review and an agenda. Political Research Quarterly, 56, 29–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Busby, E. C., Flynn, D. J., & Druckman, J. N. (2018). Studying framing effects on political preferences: existing research and lingering questions. In P. D’Angelo (Ed.), Doing news framing analysis II. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, R., Nyhan, B., & Montgomery, J. M. (2018). Revisiting white backlash: does race affect death penalty opinion. Research and Politics, 5, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007a). Framing theory. Annual Review of Political Science, 10, 103–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007b). Framing public opinion in competitive democracies. American Political Science Review, 101, 637–655.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2010). Dynamic public opinion: communication effects over time. American Political Science Review, 104, 663–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Tal-Or, N., & Mazor-Tregerman, M. (2015). The tempering effect of transportation. Journal of Communication, 65, 237–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dardis, F. E., Baumgartner, F. R., Boydstun, A. E., De Boef, S., & Shen, F. (2008). Media framing of capital punishment and its impact on individuals’ cognitive responses. Mass Communication and Society, 11, 115–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Graaf, A., Hoeken, H., & Sanders, J. (2011). Identification as a mechanism of narrative persuasion. Communication Research, 20, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donovan, K. M., & Klahm, C. F. (2018). How priming innocence influences public opinion on police misconduct and false convictions: a research note. Criminal Justice Review, 43, 174–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2001). On the limits of framing effects: who can frame? Journal of Politics, 63, 1041–1066.

    Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & Leeper, T. J. (2012). Learning more from political communication experiments: pretreatment and its effects. American Journal of Political Science, 56, 875–896.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fan, D. P., Keltner, K. A., & Wyatt, R. O. (2002). A matter of guilt or innocence: how news reports affect support for the death penalty in the United States. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 14, 439–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flatow, N. (2013). Ten ways criminal justice is one of the great civil rights crises of our time. Think Progress August 28. https://thinkprogress.org.

  • Gallup News. n.d. Death penalty. http://news.gallup.com/poll/1606/death-penalty.aspx (Feb. 16, 2018).

  • Gamson, W. A., & Modigiliani, A. (1989). Media discourse and public opinion on nuclear power: a constructionist approach. American Journal of Sociology, 95, 1–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, B. L. (2011). Convicting the innocent: where criminal prosecutions go wrong. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 701–721.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, K. (2008). Framing persuasive appeals: episodic and thematic framing, emotional response, and policy opinion. Political Psychology, 29, 169–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, S. R. (2017). What we think, what we know and what we think we know about false convictions. Ohio State Journal of Criminal Law, 14, 753–786.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, S. R., O’Brien, B., Hu, C., & Kennedy, E. H. (2014). Rate of false conviction of criminal defendants who are sentenced to death. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 7230–7235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haider-Markel, D. P., & Joslyn, M. R. (2001). Gun policy, opinion, tragedy, and blame attribution: the conditional influence of issue frames. Journal of Politics, 63, 520–543.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huff, C. R., Rattner, A., & Sagarin, E. (1986). Guilty until proved innocent: wrongful conviction and public policy. Crime & Delinquency, 32, 518–544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible? How television frames political issues. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S. (1996). Framing responsibility for political issues. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 546, 59–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Junkin, T. (2004). Bloodsworth. Chapel Hill: Algonquin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaeble, D., & Glaze, L. (2016). Correctional populations in the United States, 2015. Washington, DC: U. S. Department of Justice.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M., Drizin, S. A., Grisso, T., Gudjonsson, G. H., Leo, R. A., & Redlich, A. D. (2010). Police-induced confessions: risk factors and recommendations. Law and Human Behavior, 34, 3–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kent, S., & Carmichael, J. T. (2015). Legislative responses to wrongful conviction: do partisan principals and advocacy efforts influence state-level criminal justice policy? Social Science Research, 52, 147–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhne, R., Weber, P., & Sommer, K. (2015). Beyond cognitive framing processes. Journal of Communication, 65, 259–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, E., & Clarke, A. (2001). The impact of information on an individual’s support of the death penalty: a partial test of the Marshall hypothesis among college students. Criminal Justice Policy Review, 12, 215–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, E. G., Camp, S. D., Clarke, A., & Jiang, S. (2011). The impact of information on death penalty support, revisited. Crime & Delinquency, 57, 572–599.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leeper, T., & Mullinix, K. J. (2018). Motivated reasoning. Oxford bibliographies. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Retrieved from https://www.oxfordbibliographies.com/view/document/obo-9780199756223/obo-9780199756223-0237.xml. Accessed 16 May 2019

  • Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: the effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2098–2109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, L. C. (2004). The innocence revolution and the death penalty. Ohio State Journal of Criminal Law, 1, 1573–1584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medwed, D. S. (2008). Innocentrism. University of Illinois Law Review, 2008, 1549–1572.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mooney, C. Z., & Lee, M. (2000). The influence of values on consensus and contentious morality policy: U.S. death penalty reform, 1956-1982. Journal of Politics, 62, 223–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Registry of Exonerations. (n.d.). Retrieved from https://www.law.umich.edu/special/exoneration/Pages/about.aspx. Accessed 16 May 2019

  • National Research Council. (2009). Strengthening forensic science in the United States: a path forward. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nededog, J. (2016). Here’s how popular Netflix’s ‘making a murderer’ really was according to a research company. Business Insider February 12. http://www.businessinsider.com.

  • Nelson, T. E., Clawson, R. A., & Oxley, Z. M. (1997). Media framing of civil liberties conflict and its effect on tolerance. American Political Science Review, 91, 567–583.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: strategies and shortcoming in social judgment. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norrander, B. (2000). The multi-layered impact of public opinion on capital punishment implementation in the American states. Political Research Quarterly, 53, 771–793.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris, R. J. (2017). Exonerated: a history of the innocence movement. New York: NYU Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris, R. J., Bonventre, C. L., Redlich, A. D., Acker, J. R., & Lowe, C. (2017). Preventing wrongful convictions: an analysis of state investigation reforms. Criminal Justice Policy Review. Online first, https://doi.org/10.1177/0887403416687359.

  • Norris, R. J., Weintraub, J. N., Acker, J. R., Redlich, A. D., & Bonventre, C. L. (forthcoming). The criminal costs of wrongful convictions: can we reduce crime by protecting the innocent? Criminology & Public Policy.

  • Nyman, S. (2016). Just how popular is ‘making a murderer’? Appleton Post-Crescent January 14. https://www.postcrescent.com.

  • Park, S., Holody, K. J., & Zhang, X. (2012). Race in media coverage of school shootings: a parallel application of framing theory and attribute agenda setting. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 89, 475–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peelo, M. (2006). Framing homicide narratives in newspapers: mediated witness and the construction of virtual victimhood. Crime, Media, Culture, 2, 59–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pew Research Center. (2017). Since 2015, sharp rise in share of Republicans saying colleges have a negative effect on the country. Retrieved from https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2017/07/20/republicans-skeptical-of-colleges-impact-on-u-s-but-most-see-benefits-for-workforce-preparation/ft_17-07-20_collegessince2015/. Accessed 16 May 2019

  • Peffley, M., & Hurwitz, J. (2007). Persuasion and resistance: race and the death penalty in America. American Journal of Political Science, 51, 996–1012.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, V., & Tewksbury, D. (1997). News values and public opinion: a theoretical account of media priming and framing. In G. Barnett & F. J. Boster (Eds.), Progress in communications Sciences. Greenwich: Ablex Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramirez, M. D. (2013). Punitive sentiment. Criminology, 51, 329–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramsey, R. J., & Frank, J. (2007). Wrongful conviction: perceptions of justice professionals regarding the frequency of wrongful conviction and the extent of system errors. Crime & Delinquency, 53, 436–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricciardelli, R., & Clow, K. A. (2012). The impact of an exoneree’s guest lecture on students’ attitudes toward wrongly convicted persons. Journal of Criminal Justice Education, 23, 127–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricciardelli, R., Bell, J. G., & Clow, K. A. (2009). Student attitudes toward wrongful conviction. Canadian Journal of Criminology and Criminal Justice, 51, 411–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarat, A., Kermes, R., Cambra, H., Curran, A., Kiley, M., & Pant, K. (2017). The rhetoric of abolition: continuity and change in the struggle against America’s death penalty, 1900-2010. The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 107, 757–780.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan, E. A., Jones, M. D., McBeth, M. K., & Radaelli, C. M. (2018). The narrative policy framework. In C. M. Weible & P. A. Sabatier (Eds.), Theories of the policy process. New York: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, A. D. (2017). Cultivating support for punitive criminal justice policies: news sectors and the moderating effects of audience characteristics. Social Forces, 96, 299–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slothuus, R., & de Vreese, C. H. (2010). Political parties, motivated reasoning, and issue framing effects. Journal of Politics, 72, 630–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, B., Zalman, M., & Kiger, A. (2011). How justice system officials view wrongful convictions. Crime & Delinquency, 57, 663–685.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soss, J., Langbein, L., & Metelko, A. R. (2003). Why do white Americans support the death penalty? The Journal of Politics, 65, 397–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, S.P. (2017). What happens if you’re wrongfully convicted in N.J.? Retrieved from https://www.nj.com/politics/index.ssf/2017/03/what_happens_if_youre_wrongfully_convicted_in_nj.html.

  • Tal-Or, N., Boninger, D. S., Poran, A., & Gleicher, F. (2004). Counterfactual thinking as a mechanism in narrative persuasion. Human Communication Research, 30, 301–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Unnever, J. D., & Cullen, F. T. (2005). Executing the innocent and support for capital punishment: implications for public policy. Criminology & Public Policy, 4, 3–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Unnever, J. D., & Cullen, F. T. (2007). Reassessing the racial divide in support for capital punishment: the continuing significance of race. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 44, 124–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warden, R. (2012). How and why Illinois abolished the death penalty. Minnesota Journal of Law and Inequality, 30, 245–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, T., & Leo, R. A. (2008). The wrong guys: murder, false confessions, and the Norfolk Four. New York: The New Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., Small, M., Penrod, S., Malpass, R. S., Fulero, S. M., & Brimacombe, C. A. E. (1998). Eyewitness identification procedures: recommendations for lineups and photospreads. Law and Human Behavior, 22, 603–647.

  • Zalman, M., Larson, M. J., & Smith, B. (2012). Citizens’ attitudes toward wrongful convictions. Criminal Justice Review, 37, 51–69.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert J. Norris.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(DOCX 163 kb)

APPENDIX

APPENDIX

Table 5 Sample demographics

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Norris, R.J., Mullinix, K.J. Framing innocence: an experimental test of the effects of wrongful convictions on public opinion. J Exp Criminol 16, 311–334 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-019-09360-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-019-09360-7

Keywords

Navigation