Abstract
This paper investigates the effect of volunteering on quality of life (QoL) in 50+ populations across European countries and Israel. We analyzed data from the Survey of Health, Aging and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). Using the Kendall tau-b correlation coefficients, we show that the extent of effect volunteering has on quality of life is nonlinearly related to the prevalence of volunteering in a given country. The relationship follows an inverted-U-shaped curve. In countries where volunteering is the most popular (Denmark, Switzerland, and Belgium) and in countries with the lowest rates (Poland, Greece, the Czech Republic, and Spain), the correlation between volunteering and one’s quality of life is low. The correlation is high in countries with medium levels of volunteering (Austria, Italy, and Israel). Moreover, volunteering affects more internal than external domains of QoL. These new insights extend the discussion started by Haski-Leventhal (Voluntas Int J Volunt Nonprofit Organ 20:388–404, 2009). Our study is correlational, and we do not claim causality.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
Quality aging depends on social participation. Volunteering enables older adults to remain connected to society (Morrow-Howell 2010). It helps them form social bonds and overcome adverse events in life (Ehlers et al. 2011).
Given the societal significance of volunteering among older adults, this study examined the relationship between participation in volunteering and quality of life for European and Israelian volunteers aged 50 and above. Specifically, we focus on how this relationship changes along with the volunteering rate across the countries. This issue was previously raised by Haski-Leventhal (2009), who analyzed data collected in 2004 in 11 countries from Western and Northern Europe and Israel, and found a positive correlation between volunteering and perceived health, satisfaction with life, and self-reported life expectancy among Europeans 50+.
Importantly, Haski-Leventhal (2009) found a relatively low impact of volunteering on well-being in countries with high volunteering rates. Precisely, the relationship between participation in volunteering and subjective dimensions of well-being depends upon the popularity of volunteering in a given country. Her result was obtained by Pearson’s linear correlation approach for a geographically limited group of countries. However, Niebuur et al. (2018) criticized most studies on volunteering for being conducted in the US or selected Western European countries and postulated to diversify studies in terms of geographical coverage. New evidence has supported the positive relationship between volunteering and well-being (Wilson 2012; Haski-Leventhal et al. 2016; Hansen et al. 2018), and availability of new data from European countries makes it worth to re-examine the aforementioned relationship.
The study at hand was inspired by Haski-Leventhal (2009) findings. We test validity (robustness) of the above results by applying Pearson’s linear correlation and the Kendall tau-b correlation to a much larger set of European countries and data collected during 2015. It extends previous analyses to include post-communist countries from Central and Eastern Europe (CEE) that were not included in the earlier studies. This approach provides us with greater variation in the studied variables.
Researchers have established high variability of volunteering rates across countries (OECD 2016; Hank and Erlinghagen 2010a; Hank and Stuck 2008; Haski-Leventhal 2009; Plagnol and Huppert 2010). Volunteering rates increase alongside with economic development, which is consistent with the proposition that the rates should be higher in wealthier countries supporting higher human capital, liberal democracies supporting higher social capital, and in countries with higher levels of religiosity (Parboteeah et al. 2004; Ziemek 2006).
The role theory provides a rationale for our interest in the relationship between volunteering and quality of life across countries. It proposes that people accumulate roles to enhance their power and status, which then translates into greater well-being. However, too many roles may lead to strain and burden. A person in older age does not typically perform too many roles, and volunteering may provide a useful role enhancement. Such role enhancement makes one’s life more meaningful and leads to multiple positive outcomes such as increased quality of life (Moen et al. 1995; Musick et al. 1999).
The main hypothesis relates to the Haski-Leventhal finding (2009, p. 394) concerning a relatively weak association between volunteering and well-being in countries where the volunteering rates are high and a relatively stronger association between volunteering and well-being in countries with lower rates. This interesting result could be explained by the volunteers’ sense of necessity for satisfaction (Cnaan and Cascio 1999) and the Social Origin TheoryFootnote 1 (Salamon and Anheier 1998). The finding is also in line with a wider phenomenon known as the “crowding-out” effect (Day and Devlin 1996; Bredtmann 2016), which proposes that when a public sector provides most of the essential services then what remains for volunteers is not seen as the crucial contribution to public life.
Arguably, it is important to investigate whether adding the countries of Central and Eastern Europe (CEE) will change the relationship between volunteering and the quality of life among older adults. It is hard to predict the relation for those countries due to historical reasons (Salamon and Sokolowski 2001). Prior studies suggest a strong positive relation since the crowding effect is rather weak, while the importance of a sense of necessity is strong.
In the following parts of the paper, we start with a review of the current literature on volunteering and quality of life among the elderly. Next, we present the methodology used in this research, which is followed by data analysis. The final part provides conclusions and a discussion of potential implications.
Literature
Elderly volunteering is motivated mainly by the expected psychological benefits, rather than biological (e.g., sleep, food, free time), economic (e.g., paid work, housework), or political benefits (Smith 1981). Older adults tend to volunteer because it gives them the opportunity to participate in meaningful activities (Haski-Leventhal 2009).
Previous research presents ample evidence that helping others can have a positive effect on subjective well-being (e.g., Haski-Leventhal 2009; Morrow-Howell et al. 2003; Thoits and Hewitt 2001; Meier and Stutzer 2008). The positive effects of volunteering on the elderly are well known and commonly accepted (Morrow-Howell 2010). For instance, Thoits and Hewitt (2001) examined how volunteering affects six different dimensions of well-being: levels of happiness, life satisfaction, self-esteem, sense of control over life, physical health, and depression. The scholars found that volunteering has a positive impact on both mental and physical health. People who volunteer are happier and experience better physical health and are less prone to depression. Volunteering provides a sense of purpose, encourages concern for others, and generates a sense of personal fulfillment (Stebbins and Graham 2004). Wilson (2012) identified a number of benefits from volunteering such as improvement in mental health and protection against symptoms of mental illness, lower levels of morbidity and mortality, increase in chances of obtaining a better education and a better job, accomplishment, social interaction and belongingness.
Jenkinson et al. (2013) examined forty experimental and cohort studies comparing the physical and mental health outcomes and mortality of a volunteering group against a non-volunteering group. They found that volunteering had a positive effect on people with depression, increased life satisfaction, and improved perceived well-being. A positive association between volunteering and subjective health was reported in Van Willigen (2000), Greenfield and Marks (2004), Li and Ferraro (2006), Borgonovi (2008), Anderson et al. (2014), and Detollenaere et al. (2017).
Volunteering may improve the well-being of the elderly by engaging them in socially meaningful roles and providing the meaning and purpose in life (Greenfield and Marks 2004; Prouteau and Wolff 2008). It helps to establish new relationships (Ehlers et al. 2011). Volunteering supports meaningful activities in a person’s life after adverse events such as the death of a close family member. A positive relationship between social activities and the quality of life of older people has been repeatedly confirmed in research (Bath and Deeg 2005; Di Gessa and Grundy 2014; Fernández-Mayoralas et al. 2015; Litwin and Stoeckel 2013; Kehl and Stahlschmidt 2016; Youssim et al. 2015).
The positive effects of volunteering are amplified if volunteers feel reciprocity (i.e., their help is appreciated) and contribute their time for prosocial causes (Anderson et al. 2014). Intrinsically motivated volunteering activities benefit a person more than extrinsically motivated ones. Volunteering is not only positively related to desirable outcomes such as well-being, but it also buffers against the negative effects of low self-esteem (Russell et al. 2019).
As our study covers diverse historical, cultural, and economic settings,Footnote 2 benefits from helping others are observed in diverse contexts (Aknin et al. 2013). The elderly are likely to feel the need to volunteer–volunteering substitutes for their prior roles in society and provides opportunities for social contact and gives meaning to their lives (Sherman and Shavit 2012). On the other hand, certain country-specific customs strengthening family bonds make elderly take care of grandchildren, which limits their availability to be involved in formal volunteering activities. This may explain low rates in Southern and Eastern European countries (Wilson and Musick 1997; Dykstra and Fokkema 2011; Hank 2007). Moreover, the elderly from Eastern European countries experienced forced “volunteering” during socialism when people were required to devote their time to social, cultural, and political causes. This life-course experience may lower their desire to provide assistance to others (Kuti 2004; Anheier and Salamon 1999).
Research Hypotheses
Based on the literature review, we expect that the rate of formal volunteering among people aged 50+ in a country is largely related to the level of economic development. We also expect that the impact of volunteering on the quality of life of volunteers depends on the volunteering rate in a given country. Therefore, we hypothesize:
H1
The rates of formal volunteering among older people vary from country to country: the rates are high in developed European countries and low in countries of Central and Eastern Europe (CEE).
H2
The impact of volunteering on quality of life (QoL) is decreasing in accordance to volunteering rates: It is high in countries with low volunteering rates, and it is low if the rates are high.
Data and Methods
In this paper, we focus on the specific relationship between participation in volunteering and the quality of life (QoL) among older adults (50+). We use data from the wave 6 (release 6.0.0) of the Survey of Health, Aging and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) (Börsch-Supan 2017). SHARE has been used to study volunteering by: Erlinghagen and Hank (2006), Hank and Stuck (2008), Haski-Leventhal (2009), Hank and Erlinghagen (2010a, b), Prinicpi et al. (2012), Hansen et al. (2018) and Dávila (2017). The dataset provides information on the socioeconomic status, health, and family relationships of people over 50 years old. The dataset includes information from 68,231 interviews conducted in 2015 in 18 countries.Footnote 3 The generic questionnaire is translated into the national languages. In each participating country, a probability sample was drawn. The household level response rate ranged from 30.3% in Luxemburg to 69.3% in Greece (Bergmann et al. 2017).
A volunteer is defined as a person who gave an answer “Done voluntary or charity work.” to a question: “Have you done any of these activities in the last month?” The same identification of formal volunteering was used in Haski-Leventhal (2009), Hank and Erlinghagen (2010a) and Wahrendorf et al. (2016). We do not consider informal volunteering, such as providing unpaid help to a friend, neighbor, or family member. These types of activities are covered by different questions in SHARE. A detailed discussion of the challenges associated with the measurement of volunteering can be found in Salamon et al. (2017).
SHARE assesses the quality of life in two ways. First, a respondent is asked about the subjective assessment of life satisfaction on a scale from 0 to 10: “On a scale from 0 to 10 where 0 means completely dissatisfied and 10 means completely satisfied, how satisfied are you with your life?“. The second measure uses 12 questions developed for the purpose of CASP scale (Vanhoutte 2012, 2014). CASP stands for control, autonomy, self-realization, and pleasure, and it was constructed to measure the quality of life at the early old age as the dependent variable. The index was constructed based on the theory of satisfaction of needs (Maslow 1968; Doyal and Gough 1991). It assumes that quality of life depends on a degree of satisfaction of people’s needs. It uses four main domains that are important at the early old age for the positive experience of life: the possibility of influencing one’s own surroundings (control), the right of a person to be free from unwanted interference by others (autonomy), self-fulfillment (self-realization), and enjoying life (pleasure) (Hyde 2003). All of these domains are treated as equally important.
Even though CASP is referred to as a quality of life indicator, it may be also understood as a subjective well-being measure since it is purely based on subjective opinion and not on objective measures (Okulicz-Kozaryn 2013, 2016). Here, we use CASP-12 which is a shorter version of CASP-19 (see Table 4 in “Appendix”). The distribution of CASP among volunteers and non-volunteers across countries is provided in “Appendix.” A detailed analysis of CASP-12 in the SHARE is given by Vanhoutte (2012) who also considers CASP to be more like the subjective well-being measure than the quality of life measure. According to Gasper (2010), both terms overlap significantly and sometimes the biggest difference is due to the fact that “well-being” is more popular among psychologists and “quality of life” is more popular among other social scientists. Kim et al. (2015) lists studies in which CASP-19 was used.
To examine the association between participation in volunteering and quality of life, we first calculate the volunteering rates. Second, we measure the association between volunteering and the quality of life across countries using the Kendall’s tau-b correlation coefficient. It is a nonparametric measure based on the number of concordances and discordances in paired observations. It ranges between − 1 and 1. Kendall’s tau-b is an appropriate measure for two ordinal variables, while the Pearson correlation coefficient is more appropriate for continuous nominal data. Also, the Kendall coefficient is better for distributions with large skewness or excess kurtosis since it is less sensitive to outliers than the Pearson correlation. In the case of discrete binary variables in large samples, Pearson approach yields sensible results (Cox 1972). It is instructive to compare the results from both measures.
Our sample includes people between 50 and 90 years old. From that age group, we exclude those with severe mobility problems, more than 2 limitations with activities of daily living (about 6%), 4 and more symptoms of depression on the EURO-D scale (about 9%).Footnote 4 We excluded people living in households with income lower than 1st percentile or higher than 95th percentile at country level. The sample used for analysis has 50,874 observations.
Results
The volunteering rates in wave 1 (data collected in 2005) were noticeably lower than in the later surveys (Table 1). In the first wave, respondents were asked about volunteering in the last 4 weeks preceding interviews, while in the waves 5 and 6 the question was about activities in the last 12 months. Sweden is the only country with a lower rate in wave 6 than in wave 1. This is due to a change in translation of the term “voluntary” in Swedish questionnaire. Revised translation from wave 4 (and onwards) opted to more narrowly define volunteering.Footnote 5
As expected, there are high rates for northern and western European countries: the Netherlands (nearly 40%), Denmark, Switzerland, and Belgium (around 30%). The rates in eastern and southern Europe are lower. In all Central and Eastern European countries (except Slovenia), they are below 10%. The lowest rates in wave 6 are for Croatia (4.4%) and Poland (3.4%). Figure 1 shows a linear relation between a country’s economic wealth (GDP per capita PPP)Footnote 6 and the volunteering rate among older adults (50+). OLS coefficient of 0.23 is statistically significant at 1% level and R2 is 63.6%. Thus, we find support for hypothesis 1: volunteering rates are higher in more developed countries and lower in Central and Eastern Europe (CEE). The CEE rates are definitely lower than in more advanced Western and Northern European countries, but they are similar to Southern European countries.
Increasing heteroscedasticity of the volunteering rates along with economic wealth should be noted. Comparison of Denmark, Austria, and Sweden provides a good example.
The low rates of volunteering among elderly should be worrisome. First, volunteering is a productive activity that benefits society (Erlinghagen and Hank 2006; Hank and Erlinghagen 2010b; Prouteau and Wolff 2006). The monetary value of volunteering in the OECD25 group was estimated as 1.9% of GDP in purchasing power parity with the highest impact in Australia (4.7%), New Zealand (4.1%), the USA (3.7%), and Germany (3.3%) (OECD 2015).Footnote 7 Second, volunteering increases subjective well-being and quality of life through many channels that were mentioned above and that are included in the GDP measure.
The average values of CASP for volunteers are higher than among non-volunteers in all countries regardless of GDP (Table 2). The highest absolute differences are among low-rate countries (Poland, Estonia, Italy, and Croatia). The lowest differences are for countries with high rates of volunteering (Denmark, Sweden, and Switzerland). This pattern is consistent with the main hypothesis. However, using differences in means can lead to false conclusions in this case.
Next, we proceed to the main part of data analysis, and test hypothesis 2. Panel A of Fig. 2 shows the values of Pearson’s linear correlation coefficients. Panel B shows the values of Kendall’s tau-b coefficients. The highest value of the Kendall correlation coefficient was obtained for Israel: 0.15. Other high-impact countries were Italy and Austria, countries with average volunteering rates. Weaker associations were observed for countries with either low or high rates. Hence, we find only partial support for hypothesis 2.
To visualize the pattern, we fit linear regressions models with a quadratic term. Regression results are given in “Appendix” (Table 8). We find support for the finding in Haski-Leventhal (2009) that the high rates of volunteering are not associated with a strong positive relation of volunteering and well-being. It is interesting that dispersion of association decreases along with the volunteering rate. For countries with the rate above 20% the correlations follow the well-fitted path. We may speculate that for “rich” countries the country-specific factors are becoming less important in explaining the impact of volunteering on QoL.
Results from cross-sectional linear regression are very suggestive. However, a low number of observations (N = 18) may be problematic. Therefore, we make pairwise comparisons of tau-b coefficients (Table 6 in “Appendix” shows statistical tests). We show that some country differences in tau-b values for the association between participation in volunteering and CASP (QoL indicator) are not statistically different. For example, associations in Germany (tau-b = 0.10) and in Slovenia (tau-b = 0.11) are not statistically different, despite large differences in the rates of volunteering. At the same time, tau-b of 0.14 for Austria is higher than the tau-b values for Germany and Slovenia. There are many other examples of non-monotonic order. For example, volunteering effects on QoL in Spain, Portugal, Poland, and Greece are statistically the same as in Germany and Belgium, but the rates of volunteering are noticeably lower. Also, different volunteering effects are observed in countries with similar volunteering rates, e.g., Estonia and the Czech Republic. In Estonia, the volunteering effect on QoL is stronger (0.141) than in the Czech Republic (0.053), although the rates of volunteering are similar. We conclude that the inverted-U-type relation between volunteering rate and its impact on QoL is supported by two statistical approaches.
The weak association across highly developed countries is consistent with previous research. Generous welfare policies in such countries meet the demand for most social needs and thus reduce the demand for volunteering (Salamon and Anheier 1998; Stadelmann-Steffen 2011). External factors may be less important than internal factors in explaining the impact of volunteering on QoL.
In contrast, in less developed countries, with weaker social welfare systems, volunteers are probably more needed. Lack of generous social policy means that volunteers engage in socially significant issues and therefore experience a sense of real accomplishment, which increases their QoL.
Wiggins et al. (2004) notice that “self-realization” and “pleasure” dimensions included in CASP are associated with individual (internal) characteristics of a person. On the other hand, “control” and “autonomy” are related to prerequisites of free participation in society and they are, at least partially, dependent on external characteristics of the social environment. We repeated the previous exercise for external and internal dimensions of CASP. We calculated tau-b correlations for the dimensions of CASP (Table 7). For components related to internal dimension, the correlation is 0.13 for self-realization and 0.10 for pleasure. Correlations for components related to external dimension are lower: 0.004 for autonomy and 0.05 for control.
Figure 3 shows fitted values and predicted relation between volunteering rate and its association with external and internal dimensions of QoL. The positive impact of volunteering on QoL is more related to its impact on individual domains than on features of the external environment. This is true regardless of the volunteering rate. Also, the relative importance of the internal dimension in countries with high volunteering rates (Denmark and Switzerland) is higher than in countries with low rates. In Croatia, Greece, and Spain where the overall association is the same as in Denmark and Switzerland, the relative impact of external components is noticeably more important. The same association between volunteering and its impact on QoL in different countries may be explained by different mechanisms.
Conclusion and Discussion
We used data from wave 6 of the SHARE survey to examine the relationship between volunteering and quality of life of older adults (50+) across different countries. We calculated the correlation coefficients of Kendall’s tau-b between CASP-12 and volunteering. The highest correlation values were obtained for countries with medium volunteering rates. In countries where volunteering is the most popular (Denmark, Switzerland, and Belgium) and in those with the lowest rates (Poland, Greece, the Czech Republic, and Spain), the average relationship is weaker.
The paper was motivated by work of Haski-Leventhal (2009), who found a negative relationship between the volunteering rates at the country level and impact of volunteering on individual well-being. We demonstrated that the Haski-Leventhal (2009) result is not robust for the extended sample including countries of Central and Eastern Europe. We found low rates of formal volunteering among older people in CEE and low impact of formal volunteering on QoL in these countries. The low popularity of volunteering among the elderly may be due to a negative attitude toward volunteering caused by the experience of forced volunteering during socialism. Discussing low rates of volunteering in CEE, we need to remember about different life courses of the inhabitants of Western and Eastern Europe (Salamon and Sokolowski 2001). The extension of the research into questions about the role of life-course events requires much more detailed data than available in SHARE.
With a larger and more diverse set of countries, we found strong support for the inverted-U relationship between volunteering rates and its impact on QoL. We tested the statistical significance of differences in Kendall tau-b correlation coefficients between countries and fitted a simple linear regression model with the quadratic term. Different explanations for weak association for low and high rate countries are plausible. Both arguments, social norms and crowding out, contribute to a better understanding of why volunteering has a limited impact on the quality of life in affluent countries.
Our finding that “gains” from volunteering are more related to the internal dimension than to the external (social) conditions is relevant for policy design. “self-realization” and “pleasure” are more important than “control” and “autonomy” in all countries. This should be taken into consideration while debating on encouraging volunteering among the elderly. We propose a communication policy to highlight the positive impacts of volunteering on elderly volunteers. Of course, an ideal solution would be to direct such policy to older adults who view volunteering as a way to achieve individual goals.
The relatively stronger role of the external dimensions of the CASP index in countries with low volunteering rates is consistent with explanations based on feelings of necessity and lack of crowding out (Bredtmann 2016). Further investigations of factors connecting volunteering and QoL in countries with low volunteering rates are needed.
The exploratory analysis in this paper applies harmonized variables and large dataset to present new insights on volunteering. However, it is possible that volunteering effect on QoL in low-rate countries is underestimated due to selection bias. One may expect that if volunteering is not popular, only those who are most likely to benefit significantly in terms of their quality of life should be involved in such activity (Plagnol and Huppert 2010).
Our results do not allow for making causal conclusions. In the study, we use self-reports of volunteering as an independent variable and values of CASP index as a dependent variable. Self-reports of volunteering are relatively free of response bias since the self-reports consider an objective activity. The CASP index was chosen because it is less exposed to arbitrary assessments and less dependent on highly variable factors affecting the overall satisfaction rating, such as mood or time of year (White 2007). However, it is still possible that results may be prone to response bias.
We discuss empirical results without referring or making an attempt to formulate theory explaining the empirical pattern described in the paper. More information about cultural, historical, and economic connections with volunteering by older people is needed before such an attempt is made. We consider this paper as a stepping stone that might help to construct such a theory in future.
Notes
The theory is based on the notion of path-dependent development. Nonprofit regime types and associated policies and policy-making styles account for cross-national differences in nonprofit sector characteristics (Anheier 2010).
Volunteering attracts age groups differently across European countries. In the Netherlands, France, Denmark, or the UK, volunteering rates in the 50+ age group are higher than the rates among younger people. In Norway, Germany, Finland, Iceland, and Switzerland, volunteering among elderly is more popular than among the middle-age (30–50) individuals, but elderly volunteer less than people in the age 15–29. In the Central and East European countries (including Estonia, Latvia, Poland, Czech Republic, Lithuania, and Hungary) and Southern European countries (including Spain and Italy), people in the 50+ age group volunteer less than the other age groups, but also they are rarely involved in volunteering as compared to those in Western and Northern Europe (Hank and Stuck 2008; Hank and Erlinghagen 2010a, b; OECD 2016).
Austria, Belgium, Croatia, Czech Republic, Denmark, Estonia, France, Germany, Greece, Israel, Italy, Luxembourg, Poland, Portugal, Spain, Sweden, Switzerland, and Slovenia.
Dimensions of EURO-D scale: depression, pessimism, suicidality, guilt, sleep, interest, irritability, appetite, fatigue, concentration, enjoyment, and tearfulness.
Source: correspondence with Swedish SHARE Team.
Gross Domestic Product per capita Purchasing Power Parity adjusted.
Table 5.4. “Estimates of the economic value of volunteering in the OECD area” in “How’s life? Measuring Well-being” (OECD 2015, p. 211). The evaluation of economic value of volunteer work includes formal and informal activities. One may expect a different ranking when only formal volunteering is considered.
References
Aknin, L. B., Barrington-Leigh, C. P., Dunn, E. W., Helliwell, J. F., Burns, J., Biswas-Diener, R., et al. (2013). Prosocial spending and well-being: Cross-cultural evidence for a psychological universal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 635.
Anderson, N. D., Damianakis, T., Kröger, E., Wagner, L. M., Dawson, D. R., Binns, M. A., et al. (2014). The benefits associated with volunteering among seniors: A critical review and recommendations for future research. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1505–1533.
Anheier, H. K. (2010). Social origins theory. In H. K. Anheier & S. Toepler (Eds.), International encyclopedia of civil society. New York, NY: Springer.
Anheier, H. K., & Salamon, L. M. (1999). Volunteering in cross-national perspective: Initial comparisons. Law and Contemporary Problems, 62(4), 43–65.
Bath, P. A., & Deeg, D. (2005). Social engagement and health outcomes among older people: Introduction to a special section. European Journal of Ageing, 2(1), 24–30.
Bergmann, M., Kneip, T., De Luca, G., & Scherpenzeel, A. (2017). Survey participation in the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE), Wave 1–6. Working Paper Series, 31-2017.
Borgonovi, F. (2008). Doing well by doing good. The relationship between formal volunteering and self-reported health and happiness. Social Science and Medicine, 66, 2321–2334.
Börsch-Supan, A. (2017). Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) Wave 6. Release version: 6.0.0. SHARE-ERIC. Data set: https://doi.org/10.6103/SHARE.w6.600.
Börsch-Supan, A., Brandt, M., Hunkler, C., Kneip, T., Korbmacher, J., Malter, F., et al. (2013). Data resource profile: The Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(4), 992–1001.
Bredtmann, J. (2016). Does government spending crowd out voluntary labor and donations? IZA World of Labor. https://doi.org/10.15185/izawol.299.
Cnaan, R. A., & Cascio, T. A. (1999). Performance and commitment: Issues in management of volunteers in human service organizations. Journal of Social Service Research, 24(3), 1–30.
Cox, D. R. (1972). The analysis of multivariate binary data. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series C (Applied Statistics), 21(2), 113–120.
Dávila, M. C. (2017). The relationship between social networks and volunteerism among seniors. Journal of Social Service Research, 44(1), 38–49.
Day, M. D., & Devlin, R. A. (1996). Volunteerism and crowding out: Canadian econometric evidence. The Canadian Journal of Economics, 29(1), 37–53.
Detollenaere, J., Willems, S., & Baert, S. (2017). Volunteering, income and health. PLoS ONE, 12(3), e0173139. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0173139.
Di Gessa, G., & Grundy, E. (2014). The relationship between active ageing and health using longitudinal data from Denmark, France, Italy and England. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 68(3), 261–267.
Doyal, L., & Gough, I. (1991). A theory of human need. New York: Macmillan.
Dykstra, P., & Fokkema, T. (2011). Relationships between parents and their adult children: A West European typology of late-life families. Ageing & Society, 31(4), 545–569.
Ehlers, A., Naegele, G., & Reichert, M. (2011). Volunteering by older people in the EU. Luxembourg: European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions, Publications Office of the European Union.
Erlinghagen, M., & Hank, K. (2006). The participation of older Europeans in volunteer work. Ageing & Society, 26(4), 567–584.
Fernández-Mayoralas, G., Rojo-Pérez, F., Martínez-Martín, P., Prieto-Flores, P.-E., Rodríguez-Blázquez, C., Martín-García, S., et al. (2015). Active ageing and quality of life: Factors associated with participation in leisure activities among institutionalized older adults, with and without dementia. Aging & Mental Health, 19(11), 1031–1041.
Gasper, D. (2010). Understanding the diversity of conceptions of well-being and quality of life. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 39(3), 351–360.
Greenfield, E. A., & Marks, N. F. (2004). Formal volunteering as a protective factor for older adults’ psychological well-being. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59(5), S258–S264.
Hank, K. (2007). Proximity and contacts between older parents and their children: A European comparison. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69(1), 157–173.
Hank, K., & Erlinghagen, M. (2010a). Dynamics of volunteering in older Europeans. The Gerontologist, 50, 170–178.
Hank, K., & Erlinghagen, M. (2010b). Volunteering in ‘old’ Europe—Patterns, potentials, limitations. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 29(1), 3–20.
Hank, K., & Stuck, S. (2008). Volunteer work, informal help and care among the 50+ in Europe: Further evidence for “linked” productivity activities at older ages. Social Science Research, 37(4), 1280–1291.
Hansen, T., Aartsen, M., Slagsvold, B., & Deindl, C. (2018). Dynamics of volunteering and life satisfaction in midlife and old age: Findings from 12 European countries. Social Sciences, 7, 78.
Haski-Leventhal, D. (2009). Elderly volunteering and well-being: A cross-European comparison based on SHARE data. Voluntas: International Journal of Voluntary and Nonprofit Organizations, 20, 388–404.
Haski-Leventhal, D., Metz, E., Hogg, E., Ibrahim, B., Smith, D. H., & Wang, L. (2016). Volunteering in three life stages. In D. H. Smith, R. A. Stebbins, & J. Grotz (Eds.), The Palgrave handbook of volunteering, civic participation, and nonprofit associations, volunteering in three life stages. London: Palgrave Macmillan.
Hyde, M. (2003). A measure of quality of life in early old age: The theory, development and properties of a needs satisfaction model (CASP-19). Aging and Mental Health, 7(3), 186–194.
Jenkinson, C. E., Dickens, A. P., Jones, K., Thompson-Coon, J., Taylor, R. S., Rogers, M., et al. (2013). Is volunteering a public health intervention? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the health and survival of volunteers. BMC Public Health, 13, 773.
Kehl, K., & Stahlschmidt, S. (2016). The limits of monetizing and paying for volunteering in eldercare: A behavioral-economic approach. Voluntas: International Journal of Voluntary and Nonprofit Organizations, 27(2), 768–789.
Kim, G. R., Gopalakrishnan, N., Blane, D., Peasey, A., Malyutina, S., Simonova, G., et al. (2015). Psychometric properties and confirmatory factor analysis of the CASP-19, a measure of quality of life in early old age: The HAPIEE study. Aging & Mental Health, 19(7), 595–609.
Kuti, E. (2004). Civic service in Eastern Europe and Central Asia: From mandatory public work toward civic service. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly, 33(4), 79S–97S.
Li, Y., & Ferraro, K. F. (2006). Volunteering in middle and later life: Is health a benefit, barrier or both? Social Forces, 85, 497–519.
Litwin, H., & Stoeckel, K. (2013). Social networks and subjective wellbeing among older Europeans: Does age make a difference? Ageing & Society, 33(7), 1263–1281.
Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd ed.). New York: Van Nostrand.
Meier, S., & Stutzer, A. (2008). Is volunteering rewarding in itself? Economica, 75(297), 39–59.
Moen, P., Robinson, J., & Dempster-McCain, D. (1995). Caregiving and women’s well-being: A life course approach. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 36(3), 259–273.
Morrow-Howell, N. (2010). Volunteering in later life: Research frontiers. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 65B(4), 461–469. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbq024.
Morrow-Howell, N., Hinterlong, J., Rozario, P. A., & Tang, F. (2003). Effects of volunteering on the well-being of older adults. Journal of Gerontology Social Work, 58(3), 137–145.
Musick, M. A., Herzog, A. R., & House, J. S. (1999). Volunteering and mortality among older adults: Findings from a national sample. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 54(3), 173–180.
Niebuur, J., van Lente, L., Liefbroer, A. C., Steverink, N., & Smidt, N. (2018). Determinants of participation in voluntary work: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. BMC Public Health, 18, 1213.
OECD. (2015). How’s life 2015—Measuring well-being. Geneva: OECD.
OECD. (2016). Society at glance. Geneva: OECD.
Okulicz-Kozaryn, A. (2013). City life: Rankings (livability) versus perceptions (satisfaction). Social Indicators Research, 110, 433–451.
Okulicz-Kozaryn, A. (2016). Happiness research for public policy and administration. Transforming Government: People, Process and Policy, 10(2), 196–211.
Parboteeah, K. P., Cullen, J. B., & Lim, L. (2004). Formal volunteering: A cross-national test. Journal of World Business, 39, 431–441.
Plagnol, A. C., & Huppert, F.-A. (2010). Happy to help? Exploring the factors associated with variations in rates of volunteering across Europe. Social Indicators Research, 97(2), 157–176.
Prinicpi, A., Lindley, R., Perek-Białas, J., & Turek, K. (2012). Volunteering in older age: An organizational perspective. International Journal of Manpower, 33(6), 685–703.
Prouteau, L., & Wolff, F.-C. (2006). Does volunteer work pay off in the labor market? Journal of Socio-Economics, 35, 992–1013.
Prouteau, L., & Wolff, F.-C. (2008). On the relational motive for volunteer work. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29(3), 314–335.
Russell, A. R., Nyame-Mensah, A., de Wit, A., & Handy, F. (2019). Volunteering and wellbeing among ageing adults: A longitudinal analysis. VOLUNTAS: International Journal of Voluntary and Nonprofit Organizations, 30(1), 115–128.
Salamon, L. M., & Anheier, H. K. (1998). Social origins of civil society: Explaining the nonprofit sector cross-nationally. Voluntas: International Journal of Voluntary and Nonprofit Organizations, 9(3), 213–248.
Salamon, L. M., Haddock, M. A., & Sokolowski, W. S. (2017). Closing the gap? New perspectives on volunteering North and South. In J. Butcher & C. J. Einolf (Eds.), Perspectives on volunteering, nonprofit and civil society studies (chapter 2) (pp. 29–51). Berlin: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-39899.
Salamon, L. M., & Sokolowski, W. (2001). Volunteering in cross-national perspective: Evidence from 24 countries. Working Papers of the John Hopkins Comparative Nonprofit Sector Project, no. 40.
Sherman, A., & Shavit, T. (2012). How the lifecycle hypothesis explains volunteering during retirement. Ageing & Society, 32(8), 1360–1381.
Smith, D. H. (1981). Altruism, volunteers and volunteerism. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly, 10, 21–36.
Stadelmann-Steffen, I. (2011). Social volunteering in welfare states: Where crowding out should occur. Political Studies, 59(1), 135–155.
Stebbins, R. A., & Graham, M. (2004). Volunteering as leisure/leisure as volunteering: An international assessment. Wallingford: CABI.
Thoits, P. A., & Hewitt, L. N. (2001). Volunteer work and well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42(2), 115–131.
Van Willigen, M. (2000). Differential benefits of volunteering across the life course. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 55(5), 308–318.
Vanhoutte, B. (2012). Measuring subjective well-being: A review. CCSR Working Paper.
Vanhoutte, B. (2014). The multidimensional structure of subjective wellbeing in later life. Journal of Population Ageing, 7, 1–20.
Wahrendorf, M., Blane, D., Matthews, K., & Siegrist, J. (2016). Linking quality of work in midlife to volunteering during retirement: A European study. Population Ageing, 9, 113–130.
White, A. (2007). A global projection of subjective well-being: A challenge to positive psychology. Psychtalk, 56, 17–20.
Wiggins, R. D., Higgs, P. F. D., Hyde, M., & Blane, D. B. (2004). Quality of life in the third age: Key predictors of the CASP-19 measure. Ageing & Society, 24(5), 693–708.
Wilson, J. (2012). Volunteerism research: A review essay. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly, 41, 176–212.
Wilson, J., & Musick, M. (1997). Who cares? Toward an integrated theory of volunteer work. American Sociological Review, 5, 694–713.
Youssim, I., Hank, K., & Litwin, H. (2015). The role of family social background and inheritance in later life volunteering: Evidence from SHARE-Israel. Research on Aging, 37(1), 3–17.
Ziemek, S. (2006). Economic analysis of volunteers’ motivations—A cross-country study. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 35, 532–555.
Acknowledgements
The study was funded from a Grant 2016/21/B/HS4/03058 (Narodowe Centrum Nauki). This paper uses data from SHARE Wave 6 (https://doi.org/10.6103/share.w6.611), see Börsch-Supan et al. (2013) for methodological details. (1). The SHARE data collection has been primarily funded by the European Commission through FP5 (QLK6-CT-2001-00360), FP6 (SHARE-I3: RII-CT-2006-062193, COMPARE: CIT5-CT-2005-028857, SHARELIFE: CIT4-CT-2006-028812), and FP7 (SHARE-PREP: N°211909, SHARE-LEAP: N°227822, SHARE M4: N°261982). Additional funding from the German Ministry of Education and Research, the Max Planck Society for the Advancement of Science, the U.S. National Institute on Aging (U01_AG09740-13S2, P01_AG005842, P01_AG08291, P30_AG12815, R21_AG025169, Y1-AG-4553-01, IAG_BSR06-11, OGHA_04-064, HHSN271201300071C) and from various national funding sources is gratefully acknowledged (see www.share-project.org).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Appendix
Appendix
See Fig. 4 and Tables 3 and 4.
The descriptive analysis is complemented by a comparison of the conditional averages for the main personal determinants of QoL in Table 5. Results are as expected. In all countries, the average number of years of education for volunteers is higher than for non-volunteers. Education and health are presumably highly correlated among elderly, and they are usually identified as key predictors of QoL. The subjective health differs between volunteers and non-volunteers in highly developed countries. There is no clear volunteering pattern between males and females. In France and Germany, men are more involved in volunteering than women, while the opposite is true in Southern Europe (Greece, Portugal, Italy, and Spain). There is no gender effect among CEE countries. The impact of income on volunteering is mixed. In some countries (Denmark, Belgium, Austria, Portugal, Slovenia, the Czech Republic, and Poland), household income per person (in purchasing power parity units) is the same for volunteers and non-volunteers. In Germany, France, Israel, Italy, Estonia, and Spain, the income is higher in households with a volunteer, and in Greece, it is higher in households without a volunteer (Tables 6, 7, 8).
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Morawski, L., Okulicz-Kozaryn, A. & Strzelecka, M. Elderly Volunteering in Europe: The Relationship Between Volunteering and Quality of Life Depends on Volunteering Rates. Voluntas 33, 256–268 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11266-020-00267-w
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11266-020-00267-w