Skip to main content
Log in

The Origins of Multi-level Society

  • Published:
Topoi Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There is a very striking difference between even the simplest ethnographically known human societies and those of the chimps and bonobos. Chimp and bonobo societies are closed societies: with the exception of adolescent females who disperse from their natal group and join a nearby group (never to return to their group of origin), a pan residential group is the whole social world of the agents who make it up. That is not true of forager bands, which have fluid memberships, and regular associations with neighbouring bands. They are components of a larger social world. The open and fluid character of forager bands brings with it many advantages, so the stability of this more vertically complex form of social life is not difficult to explain, once it establishes. But how did it establish, if, as is likely, earlier hominin social worlds resemble those of our close pan relatives in the suspicion (even hostility) of one band to another? How did hominin social organisation transition from life in closed bands, each distrustful of its neighbours, to the much more open social lives of foragers? I will discuss and synthesise two approaches to this problem, one ecological, based on the work of Robert Layton and his colleagues, and another that is organised around an expansion of kin recognition, an idea primarily driven by Bernard Chapais. The paper closes by discussing potential archaeological signatures both of more open social worlds, and of the supposed causal drivers of such worlds.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. That said, it is important to recognise the considerable variation in group size, in both the pan and the hominin lineages.

  2. Ritual connection can be surprisingly important. For example, in many Australian aboriginal cultures, connection through the same Dreamtime figure is an important social tie, for it counts as one way of being from the same place (Meggitt 1962).

  3. The far western Tai community seems to be a partial exception. Males patrol, but lethal violence between groups is much rarer. Patrols sometimes seem targeted on establishing an association with females from a neighbouring group, and there is just a hint of the bonobo practice of sex as a way of managing intercommunity tensions in these patrol to female encounters: see Stanford (2018, p. 78).

  4. Sometimes equally distant kin, with parallel cousins blocked, and cross-cousins preferred.

  5. In some cases in seasons of plenty; in other, more arid habitats, around permanent water and the resources that water supports.

  6. It should be noted though that these ideas remain controversial: (Collard et al. 2016; Vaesen et al. 2016).

  7. The Tiwi provide an example of this in miniature, as the large households of polygynous men provide those males with the opportunity to specialise in the production of elaborated carved ritual objects, and in the composition of new songs and dances (Hart and Pilling 1960).

  8. From a low of 10% (though only in one season) through to 87%.

  9. Somewhat counter-intuitively, such environments are often quite good for foragers, since plants invest more of their biomass in parts of the plant humans can consume, such as “USOs”; i.e. Underground Storage Organs like tubers and corms.

  10. Indeed, Opie and Power are rather sceptical about this possibility, because they do not see how paternity could be certain enough for this to be a reasonable deal for males. That, however, depends on the character of male foraging: if they forage in a single group, or a couple of groups, they automatically police one another. Moreover, despite the threat, at least some ethnographically known foragers keep the risks to within tolerable limits.

  11. This is true even if we accept Chapais’ elaboration of his model, with affine kin recognition leading to sister exchange between brothers-in-law. That might lead to stronger ties between nodes of kin in different bands. But it will still be true that while one affine-to-affine alliance links one kin node to the east, another will build links to the west. There is no mechanism that builds congruence in the out-connections of the different families in a single band.

  12. The same is true (as Ron Planer has pointed out to me) if there is evidence of regular repair on tools found far from their source materials.

References

  • Boehm C (2012) Moral origins: the evolution of virtue, altruism, and shame. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunn H, Gurtov A (2014) Prey mortality profiles indicate that Early Pleistocene Homo at Olduvai was an ambush predator. Quat Int 322:44–53

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunn H, Pickering TR (2010) Bovid mortality profiles in paleoecological context falsify hypotheses of endurance running–hunting and passive scavenging by early Pleistocene hominins. Quat Res 74(3):395–404

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapais B (2008) Primeval kinship. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapais B (2013) Monogamy, strongly bonded groups and the evolution of human social structure. Evol Anthropol 22:52–65

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapais B (2014) Complex kinship patterns as evolutionary constructions, and the origins of sociocultural universals. Curr Anthropol 55(6):751–783

    Google Scholar 

  • Collard M, Vaesen K, Cosgrove R, Roebroeks W (2016) The empirical case against the ‘demographic turn’in Palaeolithic archaeology. Philos Trans R Soc Ser B 371(1698):20150242

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond J (2005) Collapse: how societies choose to fail or survive. Penguin-Allen Lane, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans N (2017) Did language evolve in multilingual settings? Biol Philos 32(6):905–933

    Google Scholar 

  • Fry DP (ed) (2013) War, peace and human nature. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Furuichi T (2011) Female contributions to the peaceful nature of Bonobo Society. Evol Anthropol 20:131–142

    Google Scholar 

  • Gamble C (2013) Settling the earth: the archaeology of deep human history. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Gamble C, Dunbar R, Gowlett J (2014) Thinking big: how the evolution of social life shaped the human mind. Thames and Hudson, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Gowlett J, Wrangham R (2013) Earliest fire in Africa: towards the convergence of archaeological evidence and the cooking hypothesis. Azania 48(1):5–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Grueter C, Chapais B, Zinner D (2012) Evolution of multilevel social systems in nonhuman primates and humans. Int J Primatol 33:1002–1037

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart CW, Pilling A (1960) The Tiwi of North Australia. Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J (2004) Demography and cultural evolution: why adaptive cultural processes produced maladaptive losses in Tasmania. Am Antiq 69(2):197–221

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Boyd R, Derex M, Kline M, Mesoudi A, Muthukrishna M, Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG (2016) Appendix to understanding cumulative cultural evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2798257

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill K, Walker R, Božičević M, Eder J, Headland T, Hewlett B, Hurtado M, Marlowe FW, Wiessner P, Wood B (2011) Co-residence patterns in Hunter-Gatherer Societies show unique human social structure. Science 331:2286–2289

    Google Scholar 

  • Jablonka E (2017) Collective narratives, false memories, and the origins of autobiographical memory. Biol Philos 32(6):839–853

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan FM, van Schaik C, Francois P, Gintis H, Haun DBM, Hruschka DH, Janssen MA, Kitts JA, Lehmann L, Mathew S, Richerson PJ, Turchin P, Wiessner P (2013) Cultural evolution of the structure of human groups. Cultural Evolution: Society, Technology, Language, and Religion P. Richerson and M. Christiansen. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 88–116

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly R (2000) Warless societies and the origin of war. University of Michigan Press, Ann Abor

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly RK (2013) The lifeways of Hunter-Gatherers: the foraging spectrum. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn S (2019) The evolution of paleolithic technologies: a macroscopic perspective. Routledge, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Layton R (2008) What can ethnography tell us about human social evolution? In: Allen N, Callan H, Dunbar R, James W (eds) Early human kinship: from sex to social reproduction. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 113–128

    Google Scholar 

  • Layton R, Hara S (2010) Human social evolution: a comparison of hunter-gatherer and Chimpanzee Social Organization. In: Dunbar R, Gamble C, Gowlett J (eds) Social brain, distributed mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 83–113

    Google Scholar 

  • Layton R, O’Hara S, Bilsborough A (2012) Antiquity and social function of multilevel Social Organization among human Hunter-Gatherers. Int J Primatol 33(5):215–1245

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann J (2008) Meaning and relevance of kinship in great apes. In: Allen N, Callan H, Dunbar R, James W (eds) early human kinship: from sex to social reproduction. Wiley, New York, pp 160–167

    Google Scholar 

  • Luncz L, Boesch C (2014) Tradition over trend: neighboring chimpanzee communities maintain differences in cultural behavior despite frequent immigration of adult females. Am J Primatol 76:649–657

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe FW (2010) The Hadza: hunter-gatherers of Tanzania. University of California Press, Berkeley

    Google Scholar 

  • Marwick B (2003) Pleistocene exchange networks as evidence for the evolution of language. Camb Archaeol J 13(1):67–81

    Google Scholar 

  • Meggitt MJ (1962) Desert people. Angus and Robertson, Sydney

    Google Scholar 

  • Muthukrishna M, Henrich J (2016) Innovation in the collective brain. Philos Trans R Soc Ser B 317(1690):20150192

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowell A, White M (2010) Growing up in the middle pleistocene. Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition. A. Nowell and I. Davidson. Boulder. University of Colorado Press, Boulder

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Driscoll C, Thompson J (2018) The origins and early elaboration of projectile technology. Evol Anthropol 27:30–45

    Google Scholar 

  • Ofek H (2001) Second nature: economic origins of human evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Opie K, Power C (2008) Grandmothering and female coalitions: a basis for matrilineal priority? In: Allen N, Callan H, Dunbar R, James W (eds) Early human kinship: from sex to social reproduction. Oxford, Blackwell, pp 168–185

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering TR (2013) Rough and tumble: aggression, hunting, and human evolution. University of California Press, Los Angles

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering TR, Bunn H (2012) Meat foraging by Pleistocene African hominins: Tracking behavioral evolution beyond baseline inferences of early access to carcasses. stone tools and fossil bones. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 152–173

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas M (2009) Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324:298–1301

    Google Scholar 

  • Premo LS, Kuhn S (2010) Modeling effects of local extinctions on culture change and diversity in the Paleolithic. PLoS ONE 5(12):e15582

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodseth L (2012) From bachelor threat to fraternal security: male associations and modular organization. Int J Primatol 33:1194–1214

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott J (2017) Against the grain. Yale University Press, New Haven

    Google Scholar 

  • Seabright P (2010) The company of strangers: a natural history of economic life. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheehan O, Watts I, Gray RD, Atkinson QD (2018) Coevolution of landesque capital intensive agriculture and sociopolitical hierarchy. Proc Natl Acad Sci 115(14):3628–3633

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanford C (2018) The new chimpanzee: a twenty-first century portrait of our closest kin. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2014) A paleolithic reciprocation crisis: symbols, signals, and norms. Biol Theory 9(1):65–77

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2016) Cooperation, culture, and conflict. Br J Philos Sci 67(1):31–58

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2018) Religion re-explained. Relig Brain Behav 8(4):406–425

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiner MC (2002) Carnivory, coevolution, and the geographic spread of the genus Homo. J Archaeol Res 10(1):1–63

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaesen K, Collard M, Cosgrove R, Roebroeks W (2016) Population size does not explain past changes in cultural complexity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113(16):E2224–E2241

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner PW (2002) “Hunting, healing, and hxaro exchange.” Evolution and human behavior: A long-term perspective on !Kung (Ju/’hoansi) large-game hunting. Evol Human Behav 23(6):407–436

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner PW (2014) Embers of society: firelight talk among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111(39):14027–14035

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins J, Chazan M (2012) Blade production ~500 thousand years ago at Kathu Pan 1, South Africa: support for a multiple origins hypothesis for early Middle Pleistocene blade technologies. J Archaeol Sci 39:883e1900

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins J, Schoville B, Brown K, Chazan M (2012) Evidence for early hafted hunting technology. Science 338:942–946

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham R (1999) Evolution of coalitionary killing. Yearb Phys Anthropol 42:1–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham R (2017) Control of fire in the Paleolithic: evaluating the cooking hypothesis. Curr Anthropol 58(S16):S303–S313

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham R (2019) The goodness paradox: how evolution made us more and less violent. Profile Books, London

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thanks to Anton Killin, Ron Planer and the audience of the evolution of kinship workshop at the ANU for feedback on an earlier version of this paper. I would also like to thank the referees of the last version for their very constructive feedback. As always, it is a pleasure to acknowledge the Australian Research Council for their support for my research on human cognitive and social evolution.

Funding

Australian Research Council Grant FL13 130 100 141.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kim Sterelny.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

I declare that I have no conflict of interest.

Research Involving Human and Animal Rights

No animal nor human subjects were used in this research.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sterelny, K. The Origins of Multi-level Society. Topoi 40, 207–220 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-019-09666-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-019-09666-1

Keywords

Navigation