Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Attributional gender bias: teachers’ ability and effort explanations for students’ math performance

  • Published:
Social Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research is presented on the attributional gender bias: the tendency to generate different attributions (explanations) for female versus male students’ performance in math. Whereas boys’ successes in math are attributed to ability, girls’ successes are attributed to effort; conversely, boys’ failures in math are attributed to a lack of effort and girls’ failures to a lack of ability. This bias has been shown in previous research to be committed by teachers, parents, and students themselves. The present work sought to investigate whether this bias among secondary school math teachers might be reduced over time through adoption of an incremental theory of intelligence. Findings revealed at baseline, teachers committed the expected bias in reference to their high-achieving students’ math performance. Following exposure to stimuli, teachers in both experimental and control conditions reduced this bias. Unexpectedly, teachers across conditions showed a type of compensation for the bias by reversing stereotypical attributions for girls’ and boys’ successes and failures in math. Further, participants relapsed to the original bias nearly a year later. Findings indicate the potential to modify attributional gender bias, but also the challenges for achieving long-term changes within school contexts and for emphasizing effort beyond ability in math performance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113–125. doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. L., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78(1), 246–263. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00995.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bleeker, M. M., & Jacobs, J. E. (2004). Achievement in math and science: Do mothers’ beliefs matter 12 years later? Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 97–109. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolyard, J. J., & Moyer-Packenham, P. S. (2008). A review of the literature on mathematics and science teacher quality. Peabody Journal of Education, 83(4), 509–535. doi:10.1080/01619560802414890.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornholt, L., & Möller, J. (2003). Attributions about achievement and intentions about further study in social context. Social Psychology of Education, 6, 217–231. doi:10.1023/a:1024715609124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouchey, H. A., & Harter, S. (2005). Reflected appraisals, academic self-perceptions, and math/science performance during early adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(4), 673–686. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.4.673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, R. (2000). Making judgments about ability: The role of implicit theories of ability in moderating inferences from temporal and social comparison information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 965–978. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.965.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ceci, S. J., Williams, W. M., & Barnett, S. M. (2009). Women’s underrepresentation in science: Sociocultural and biological considerations. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 218–261. doi:10.1037/a0014412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheryan, S., & Plaut, V. C. (2010). Explaining underrepresentation: A theory of precluded interest. Sex Roles, 63, 475–488. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9835-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deemer, S. (2004). Classroom goal orientation in high school classrooms: Revealing links between teacher beliefs and classroom environments. Educational Research, 46(1), 73–90. doi:10.1080/0013188042000178836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickhäuser, O., & Meyer, W. (2006). Gender differences in young children’s math ability attributions. Psychology Science, 48(1), 3–16. Retrieved from http://www.pabst-publishers.de/psychology-science/1-2006.

  • Dweck, C. S. (2002a). The development of ability conceptions. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 57–88). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (2002b). Messages that motivate: How praise molds students’ beliefs, motivation, and performance (in surprising ways). In J. Aronson (Ed.), Improving academic achievement (pp. 37–60). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S. (2007). Where are all the women? Gender differences in participation in physical science and engineering. In S. J. Ceci, S. J. Ceci, W. M. Williams, & W. M. Williams (Eds.), Why aren’t more women in science: Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 199–210). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Espinoza, P. P., Quezada, S. A., Rincones, R., Strobach, E. N., & Gutierrez, M. A. E. (2012). Attributional bias instrument (ABI): Validation of a measure to assess ability and effort explanations for math performance. Social Psychology of Education, 15(4), 533–554. doi:10.1007/s11218-012-9201-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fennema, E., & Sherman, J.A. (1976). Fennema-Sherman mathematics attitudes scales: Instruments designed to measure attitudes toward the learning of mathematics by females and males. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 7(5), 324–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fennema, E., Peterson, P. L., Carpenter, T. P., & Lubinski, C. A. (1990). Teachers’ attributions and beliefs about girls, boys, and mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 21, 55–65. doi:10.1007/bf00311015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Findley, M. J., & Cooper, H. M. (1983). Locus of control and academic achievement: A literature review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(2), 419–427. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.2.419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgasz, H. J., Leder, G. C., & Kloosterman, P. (2004). New perspectives on the gender stereotyping of mathematics. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 6(4), 389–420. doi:10.1207/s15327833mtl0604_2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York, NY: Bantam Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Good, C., Aronson, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2003). Improving adolescents’ standardized test performance: An intervention to reduce the effects of stereotype threat. Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 645–662. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2003.09.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). The role of parents and teachers in the development of gender-related math attitudes. Sex Roles, 66(3), 153–166. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-9996-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). The science of sex differences in science and mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8(1), 1–51. doi:10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heck, R. H. (2007). Examining the relationship between teacher quality as an organizational property of schools and students’ achievement and growth rates. Educational Administration Quarterly, 43(4), 399–432. doi:10.1177/0013161x07306452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y., Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M.-S., & Wan, W. (1999). Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 588–599. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, J. N. (2011). Longitudinal effects of teacher and student perceptions of teacher-student relationship qualities on academic adjustment. The Elementary School Journel, 112(1), 38–60. doi:10.1086/660686.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, J. N., Luo, W., Kwok, O. M., & Loyd, L. K. (2008). Teacher-student support, effortful engagement, and achievement: A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 1–4. doi:10.1037/0022-0663-100.1.1.

  • Hyde, J. S., & Mertz, J. E. (2009). Gender, culture, and mathematics performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(22), 8801–8807. doi:10.1073/pnas.0901265106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, J. E., & Eccles, J. S. (1992). The impact of mothers’ gender-role stereotypic beliefs on mothers’ and children’s ability perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 932–944. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.932.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 15, 192–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28(2), 107–128. doi:10.1037/h0034225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. R., Stroessner, S. J., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Stereotype formation and endorsement: The role of implicit theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1421–1436. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mangels, J. A., Butterfield, B., Lamb, J., Good, C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Why do beliefs about intelligence influence learning success? A social cognitive neuroscience model. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(2), 75–86. doi:10.1093/scan/nsl013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, M. F., & Morris, M. W. (2010). Culture, attribution and automaticity: A social cognitive neuroscience view. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2–3), 292–306. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq034.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Midgley, C., Anderman, E., & Hicks, L. (1995). Differences between elementary and middle school teachers and students: A goal theory approach. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 15(1), 90–113. doi:10.1177/0272431695015001006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 33–52. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mulhern, F., & Rae, G. (1998). Development of a shortened form of the Fennema–Sherman mathematics attitudes scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 58(2), 295–306. doi:10.1177/0013164498058002012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niiya, Y., Crocker, J., & Bartmess, E. N. (2004). From vulnerability to resilience: Learning orientations buffer contingent self-esteem from failure. Psychological Science, 15, 801–805. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00759.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaks, J. E., Stroessner, S. J., Dweck, C. S., & Sherman, J. W. (2001). Person theories and attention allocation: Preferences for stereotypic versus counterstereotypic information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(6), 876–893. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.876.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, E. M., & Dong, W. (2006). Effects of mothers’ perceptions of children’s competence: The moderating role of mothers’ theories of competence. Developmental Psychology, 42(5), 950–961. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.950.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Räty, H., Vänskä, J., Kasanen, K., & Kärkkäinen, R. (2002). Parents’ explanation of their child’s performance in mathematics and reading: A replication and extension of Yee and Eccles. Sex Roles, 46, 121–128. doi:10.1023/a:1016573627828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph, K. D., Lambert, S. F., Clark, A. G., & Kurlakowsky, K. D. (2001). Negotiating the transition to middle school: The role of self-regulatory processes. Child Development, 72(3), 929–946. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryckman, D. B., & Peckham, P. D. (1987). Gender differences in attributions for success and failure. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 7(1), 47–63. doi:10.1177/0272431687071006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salovey, P., & Grewal, D. (2005). The science of emotional intelligence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 281–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scafidi, T., & Bui, K. (2010). Gender similarities in math performance from middle school through high school. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 37(3), 252–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sekaquaptewa, D., Espinoza, P., Thompson, M., Vargas, P., & von Hippel, W. (2003). Stereotypic explanatory bias: Implicit stereotyping as a predictor of discrimination. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 75–82. doi:10.1016/s0022-1031(02)00512-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slack, N., & Norwich, B. (2007). Evaluating the reliability and validity of a learning styles inventory: A classroom-based study. Educational Research, 49(1), 51–63. doi:10.1080/00131880701200765.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28. doi:10.1006/jesp.1998.1373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D. J., & Gralinski, J. H. (1991). Gender differences in children’s achievement-related beliefs and emotional responses to success and failure in mathematics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 361–371. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.83.3.361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swinton, A. D., Kurtz-Costes, B., Rowley, S. J., & Okeke-Adeyanju, N. (2011). A longitudinal examination of African American adolescents. Child Development, 82(5), 1486–1500. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01623.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tiedemann, J. (2000). Gender-related beliefs of teachers in elementary school mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 41(2), 191–207. doi:10.1023/a:1003953801526.

  • Tiedemann, J. (2002). Teachers’ gender stereotypes as determinants of teacher perceptions in elementary school mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 50, 49–62. doi:10.1023/a:1020518104346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tom, D., & Cooper, H. (1986). The effect of student background on teacher performance attributions: Evidence for counterdefensive patterns and low expectancy cycles. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 7, 53–62. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp0701_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (2005). Motivation from an attribution perspective and the social psychology of perceived competence. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 73–84). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitley, B. E., & Frieze, I. H. (1985). Children’s causal attributions for success and failure in achievement settings: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(5), 608–616. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.77.5.608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yee, D., & Eccles, J. S. (1988). Parent perceptions and attributions for children’s math achievement. Sex Roles, 19, 317–333. doi:10.1007/bf00289840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This manuscript is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. HRD-0624670. Thank you to Kimberly Carrillo and Yvette Baca for contributing to data collection for this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Penelope Espinoza.

Appendix

Appendix

1.1 ABI sample items

Success item for a high-achieving student

A.B. might score high on the end-of-course exam because....

  • __ she easily mastered the course material.

  • __ she studied hard before the exam.

  • __ she is a relatively good test taker.

  • __ she asked for help in preparing for the exam.

Failure item for a high-achieving student

A.B. might drop the advanced math class because....

  • __ she sometimes has a difficult time mastering concepts quickly.

  • __ she did not want to put extra effort into an advanced course.

  • __ she had trouble integrating the concepts from basic math courses.

  • __ she was bothered by the amount of homework involved with the course.

Success item for a low-achieving student

A.B. might raise the final grade to a “B” average because....

  • __ she found the assignments interesting and was able to focus.

  • __ she began meeting with a peer tutor.

  • __ she has the ability to do well in the subject.

  • __ she joined a study group of “A” students.

Failure item for a low-achieving student

A.B. might receive failing grades on two assignments because....

  • __ she had difficulty with the unit being covered.

  • __ she rushed through the assignments.

  • __ she generally struggles with math as a whole.

  • __ she did not review the sample problems.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Espinoza, P., Arêas da Luz Fontes, A.B. & Arms-Chavez, C.J. Attributional gender bias: teachers’ ability and effort explanations for students’ math performance. Soc Psychol Educ 17, 105–126 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-013-9226-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-013-9226-6

Keywords

Navigation