1 Introduction

Contemporary societies are becoming increasingly ethnically diverse. Diversity levels are likely to continue to increase as migration continues. Researchers have also observed that migration flows have become highly diverse with migrants originating from an increasingly diverse array of countries (Castles et al., 2014; Vertovec, 2007). The societal consequences of these rising diversity levels have become the topic of heated debate. Scholars and policymakers have directed their attention in particular to the question of whether and how diversity complicates social relationships. Putnam’s (2007) study on the eroding effect of diversity on social cohesion has been especially influential in this regard. Empirical evidence suggests that ethnic diversity is mainly associated with decreasing levels of intra-neighborhood cohesion.Footnote 1 Criminologists have also examined the role of ethnic diversity in local communities. Here, diversity is considered a relevant predictor of fear of crime. Research demonstrates that greater ethnic diversity in the residential context is associated with higher fear levels (Brunton-Smith & Sturgis, 2011; Covington & Taylor, 1991; Hooghe & De Vroome, 2016; Liu & Polson, 2016).

Our study challenges the ways in which ethnic diversity has been generally understood and measured in most previous research. We argue that commonly-used measures insufficiently reflect the enormous diversity of modern societies and propose a modified and refined operationalization of diversity, which involves an adapted version of the classical Herfindal-Hirschman-Index (HHI). The HHI is a widely used diversity index to measure ethnic heterogeneity in neighborhoods.Footnote 2 Our modified HHI measure is group-specific and assesses the level of diversity among members of the out-group (i.e. the group an individual does not belong to) from the perspective of the in-group (i.e. the group an individual does belong to). This approach differs from how scholars are currently using the HHI, where all habitants from the same neighborhood receive an identical diversity score, regardless of their ethnicity. The current study follows up on Koopmans and Schaeffer (2015) who introduced a similar innovation to test diversity effects on neighborhood cohesion among German respondents (N = 7500). We use the modified, group-specific HHI to estimate diversity effects on individuals’ perceptions of neighborhood cohesion of and fear of crime, relying on large-scale survey data from the Dutch Safety Monitor 2014 (N = 71,760) combined with individual-level register data from Statistics Netherlands. From this point forward, we refer to individuals’ perceptions of neighborhood cohesion also as ‘neighborhood cohesion’; the same holds for individuals’ perceptions of fear of crime, which we will refer to as ‘fear of crime’. We estimate diversity effects on neighborhood cohesion and fear from the perspective of Dutch natives, mainly for practical reasons.Footnote 3 Consequently, non-native Dutch are considered the out-group in this study and native Dutch the in-group. One of the main aims of the current research is to improve our understanding of the social implications of living in diversity.

Not only do we consider the effect of diversity on patterns of cohesion and fear, but we also examine the role of out-group size. Taken together, this approach allows for a more adequate analysis of the mechanisms that help to explain why and how a neighborhood’s ethnic composition affects cohesion and fear levels. We identify two mechanisms (Van der Meer & Tolsma, 2014). The first mechanism considers how diversity causes feelings of anomie, which ultimately result in social isolation and fear. The second mechanism is informed by conflict theory, and specifies that a large out-group induces feelings of threat which negatively affect cohesion and perceived safety. Most previous studies are unable to differentiate between these two mechanisms as existing measures of diversity and out-group size appear to be highly collinear (Koopmans & Schaeffer, 2016; Laurence et al., 2019; Schaeffer, 2013; Uslaner, 2011). In the current study, we distinguish between diversity and out-group size (both conceptually and empirically).

We also examine potential interrelationships between fear of crime and neighborhood cohesion, and aim to explore to what extent neighborhood cohesion mediates the relationship between ethnic diversity and fear of crime. Theoretically, there are two opposing views on how neighborhood cohesion and feelings of unsafety may relate to each other: attachment in the neighborhood is expected to either reinforce or weaken feelings of safety (Boessen et al., 2017; Kanan & Pruitt, 2002; Yuan & McNeeley, 2016). The empirical evidence is somewhat mixed, although most studies have found a negative relationship between cohesion and fear (Franklin et al. 2008). These associations are most often empirically tested at the individual level (e.g. Adams & Serpe, 2000; Drakulich, 2015; Gibson et al., 2002, see for an exception Yuan & McNeeley, 2015). Our research design, however, makes it possible to assess the relationship between fear and cohesion at both the individual level and the neighborhood level.

Overall, this paper contributes to the literature in the following ways. First, by using an alternative measure to study diversity effects on fear of crime and neighborhood cohesion and second, by examining whether the relationship between diversity and fear of crime is mediated by neighborhood cohesion. We apply multilevel equation modeling techniques to study the relationships and to test the potential mediation effect. The main research questions we aim to answer are:

  1. 1

    To what extent does ethnic diversity in the neighborhood relate to neighborhood cohesion and fear of crime?

  2. 2

    Is the association between ethnic diversity and fear of crime mediated by neighborhood cohesion?

This article proceeds as follows. Firstly, we elaborate on the role of ethnic diversity (versus out-group size) and why diversity in a neighborhood may impact levels of cohesion and fear. Secondly, we look more closely at how to measure diversity. Next, we consider the relationship between fear and cohesion. We then describe the data and our modeling strategy in more detail, before presenting the results of the statistical analyses. We conclude with a discussion of the results and their implications, and address the study’s limitations.

2 Theoretical Framework

The current paper distinguishes two dimensions of ethnic composition: ethnic diversity and out-group size (i.e. the share of the group a person does not belong to). Diversity refers more specifically to the distribution of inhabitants among different groups. The level of diversity is determined by two conditions: the number of groups present and the relative size of each group. A neighborhood is highly diverse if there is a large variety of groups and if the population is evenly distributed across these groups (Blau, 1977, p. 77–78).

2.1 Empirical Findings Linking Ethnic Composition to Cohesion and Fear of Crime

A lot of studies have empirically tested the relationship between ethnic composition and social cohesion. Scholars have looked at the impact of diversity on generalized trust (e.g. Hooghe et al., 2009), citizen participation (e.g. Gijsberts et al., 2012) and intra-neighborhood cohesion (e.g. Bécares et al., 2011). In most studies, these relationships were examined within the residential context of neighborhoods. More recently, researchers have also considered other contexts such as the workplace (Laurence et al., 2018). Van der Meer and Tolsma’s (2014) meta-analysis demonstrated that ethnic diversity in the residential environment is most consistently associated with decreases in intra-neighborhood cohesion. Unfortunately, these results tell us little about diversity effects per se because of the previously mentioned entanglement of diversity and out-group measures (Koopmans & Schaeffer, 2016; Schaeffer, 2013; Tolsma & Van der Meer, 2017; Uslaner, 2011).

As for fear of crime, most empirical research has been conducted within the context of American neighborhoods. Those studies are primarily restricted to examining the effect of minority concentration on feelings of safety and have repeatedly shown that the presence of African American residents is related to more fear, in particular amongst white Americans (Chiricos et al., 1997; Moeller, 1989; Pickett et al., 2012). This relationship is possibly caused by stereotypes depicting African Americans as being prone to crime and violence (Eitle & Taylor, 2008). Research examining contextual effects of ethnic composition on feelings of unsafety outside the United States is rather limited (for exceptions see Brunton-Smith & Sturgis, 2011; Glas et al., 2019; Hooghe & De Vroome, 2016). In the next section, we will discuss the mechanisms that help explain why we expect that diversity and out-group size have an impact on fear and cohesion.

2.2 Mechanisms Linking Ethnic Composition to Cohesion and Fear of Crime

Scholars examining diversity effects generally do so on the basis of the constrict hypothesis (Putnam, 2007) or the sub-cultural diversity hypothesis (Merry, 1981). The former hypothesis relates greater diversity to decreasing levels of social cohesion, while the latter predicts a positive relationship between diversity and fear. Both propose that diversity complicates social relationships, because shared values and a shared language are less likely to be present in a diverse setting. Inhabitants of these settings may therefore feel they lack knowledge of how to socially interact with each other (Van der Meer & Tolsma, 2014). Feelings of anomie and uncertainty emerge, causing inhabitants to avoid interaction and socially isolate themselves. Fear-of-crime scholars emphasize that people living in diverse settings find it more difficult to interpret each other’s behaviors and manners, which makes the presence of other ethnic groups “fear-inspiring” (Covington & Taylor, 1991, p. 232). This argument centers on the difference between one’s own ethnicity and that of one’s surrounding neighbors and does not link ethnicity to fear per se (Bennett & Flavin, 1994; Liu & Polson, 2016; Merry, 1981).

Based on conflict and threat theory, scholars have pointed out how increases in out-group size may spur competition between ethnic groups for scarce resources such as jobs and housing, and non-material resources, such as morality and identity (Van der Meer & Tolsma, 2014). Out-group size is thus considered a crucial indicator of inter-group competition (Schlueter & Scheepers, 2010). Processes of competition improve in-group solidarity, but at the same time increase out-group distrust and prejudice (Blalock, 1967; Quillian, 1995). Inhabitants who feel threatened interact less with their out-group neighbors and experience more fear. The extent to which an individual perceives an out-group as threatening, may depend on his or her own socio-economic status (Laurence et al. 2019). Studies show that in particular the native majority feels unsafe when more ethnic minorities are present (Hooghe & De Vroome, 2016; Pickett et al., 2012). This line of research distinguishes between members belonging to the ‘dominant’ majority and those belonging to ‘subordinate’ minority groups, and how the presence of the latter causes feelings of threat among majority group members (Quillian, 1995). These feelings easily result in prejudice and stereotyping and may, in turn, negatively affect perceptions of neighborhood safety and cohesion (Pickett et al. 2012; Quillian & Pager, 2001).

2.3 Measuring and Testing the Role of Ethnic Diversity

This section looks more closely at the measurement of ethnic diversity (for a more elaborate discussion of population diversity see Lieberson, 1969). The HHI is a commonly used diversity index. The same underlying formula is used to measure ethnic fractionalization within countries (Kolo, 2012). The height of the HHI depends on (1) the number of ethnic groups present in a neighborhood and (2) their relative size. The HHI score increases when a neighborhood’s population is composed of a relatively large number of groups that are equal in size.Footnote 4 In the current paper, we calculate diversity levels based on an adjusted version of the HHI. This version is group-specific and assesses diversity among members of the out-group in each neighborhood. Those living in the same neighborhood and sharing the same ethnicity obtain a similar score. This differs from the traditional HHI where all habitants from the same neighborhood receive an identical score, regardless of their ethnicity.

The formulas of the traditional HHI and group-specific HHI are largely comparable:

$${\text{ traditional HHI}} = 1 - \mathop \sum \limits_{i = 1}^{N} s_{i}^{2}$$
$${\text{group - specific HHI}} = 1 - \mathop \sum \limits_{j = 1}^{G} s_{j}^{2}$$

In the traditional formula, \(s_{i}\) is the share of ethnic group i (of the total population) and N stands for the total number of groups. For the group-specific formula, \(s_{j}\) denotes the share of out-group j (of the out-group population) and G the number of out-groups.

For both versions of the HHI, the researcher decides how many groups are distinguished and included in the calculations. Most Dutch scholars calculate the HHI based on a distinction of seven groups or less (Gijsberts et al., 2012; Lancee & Dronkers, 2011; Tolsma et al., 2009; Vervoort et al., 2011).Footnote 5 We argue that a HHI based on only seven groups fails to capture the “enormous diversity of ethnic groups” present in European cities (Crul, 2016, p. 57), resulting in underestimated diversity levels. In contrast, Koopmans and Schaeffer (2015), who study group-specific diversity effects on neighborhood cohesion, do not use any clustering and include every ethnic group separately. Here, the level of diversity may be overestimated. In the current study, we distinguish between 18 groups to do justice to the diversity within Dutch neighborhoods.Footnote 6 The categorization is a geo-linguistic classification, predominantly based on language and religion and refined with information on the political system of the country of origin (Jennissen et al., 2018).Footnote 7

The group-specific HHI is preferred over the ‘traditional’ HHI for two reasons. First, it makes it possible to test the effects of diversity and out-group size simultaneously because correlations between the two measures are reduced. Secondly, the adjusted HHI is a less ambiguous diversity measure. Most existing diversity indices implicitly assume that all inhabitants experience ‘diversity’ in their neighborhood in a similar way. This is, however, unlikely: for native inhabitants, living in an area with high diversity means living amongst fewer co-ethnics. For non-natives, the reverse tends to be true (Abascal & Baldassarri, 2015; Schaeffer, 2013). We observe these patterns as well in Dutch neighborhoods.Footnote 8 Because people are more likely to interact with similar others (i.e. members of their in-group), native Dutch living in relatively homogeneous neighborhoods are more likely to have contacts with other natives than those living in diverse neighborhoods (McPherson et al., 2001). The opposite is the case for non-natives residing in a diverse neighborhood. It follows that we cannot conclude whether a possible diversity effect–when measured by the traditional HHI–is the result of actual diversity or of living with fewer co-ethnics. The group-specific HHI, in sum, enables us to better locate diversity effects since the share of the in-group and hence the role of in-group contact is left out. The group-specific diversity measure ensures that the effect of diversity is now disentangled from the effect of not living with co-ethnics.

To better illustrate how out-group size, the group-specific HHI and the traditional HHI relate, we visualize their relationships in the scatterplots below (Fig. 1). The measures were calculated for Dutch natives as they are the focus of the current study. It follows that, in the current research, the out-group consists of all non-native Dutch inhabitants. Whether a group belongs to the out-group depends on which is group is chosen as the in-group. The points in the scatterplots of Fig. 1 represent the neighborhoods included in the current study. The left plot (Fig. 1a) shows that there is little distinction between out-group size and the traditional HHI, indicating that these variables are empirically almost entirely similar (see also Gijsberts et al., 2012, p. 536). This is confirmed by the high correlation coefficient (r = 0.98). The right plot (Fig. 1b), based on the group-specific HHI, shows a much more dispersed pattern; out-group size and diversity are now two empirically distinct dimensions of ethnic composition. The measures are only weakly correlated (r = 0.24). Figure 1b further suggests that there is a relatively large share of neighborhoods with a small out-group that is highly diverse. Approximately one-fourth of the neighborhoods fall into this category (compared to zero when diversity is measured in the traditional way).Footnote 9 This is not necessarily surprising because the share of native Dutch–which would bring down a neighborhood’s diversity level when included–is no longer part of how the diversity score is calculated. The specific characteristics raise the question of whether the hypothesized diversity effects operate similarly or differently depending on out-group size. We examine this option more closely in the statistical analyses.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Measuring ethnic composition for Dutch natives. The dots represent all neighborhoods included in the study a Out-group size and the traditional HHI b Out-group size and the group-specific HHI

2.4 An (Mediating) Effect of Neighborhood Cohesion on Fear of Crime

In this last theoretical section, we further elaborate on the potential dependency between cohesion and fear, and on whether perceptions of cohesion mediate the relationship between ethnic diversity and fear. We focus on individuals’ perceptions of fear and cohesion and how these perceptions vary within and between neighborhoods. Note that the hypothesized mediation can only occur at the neighborhood level (i.e. between level) since ethnic diversity is a contextual characteristic that does not vary within neighborhoods. It follows that diversity levels cannot account for individual (i.e. within) differences in cohesion and fear (Laméris et al., 2018; Preacher et al., 2010). The potential mediating role of cohesion will thus be modeled and assessed at the between (or: neighborhood) level. At the within level, we only consider the association between cohesion and fear. We analyze the different levels and pathways within the framework of multilevel structural equation modeling (MSEM) (Preacher et al., 2010). Further details can be found in Sect. 3.5 (analytical strategy). The main model is illustrated in Fig. 2

Fig. 2
figure 2

Main conceptual model

The mediating role of cohesion has already been well documented in studies aimed at understanding neighborhood crime patterns. Based on collective efficacy theory, scholars found that the relationship between certain neighborhood characteristics (including ethnic composition) and violent crime is largely mediated by the aggregated level of cohesion and informal social control in a neighborhood (Morenoff et al., 2001; Sampson et al., 1997). This line of research suggests that inhabitants living in cohesive neighborhoods are more likely to use informal means to control their neighborhood with the aim of reducing problems related to crime and disorder. The presence of these social informal controls in a neighborhood may result (both directly and indirectly) in increased feelings of safety (Scarborough et al., 2010). Based on this literature, we expect that cohesion (at least) partially mediates the hypothesized relationship between diversity and fear. The empirical evidence on the association between cohesion and fear is, however, rather limited and mixed. A study by Yuan and McNeeley (2015) demonstrated that the strength of social networks in a neighborhood was not associated with inhabitants’ perceived risk or worries about victimization. Rountree and Land (1996) showed that social integration (at the neighborhood level) diminishes residents’ perceptions of the neighborhood as being unsafe, but simultaneously results in higher levels of burglary-specific fear.

At the individual level, there are at least three pathways that link cohesion to fear. The first two hypothesize that cohesion and feelings of safety are positively related. Cohesion contributes to more safety either by providing inhabitants with a greater sense of familiarity or by providing them with better access to support resources (Boessen et al., 2017). Being familiar with the neighborhood and the inhabitants makes it easier to ‘place’ each other in public space, which in turn contributes to an improved sense of safety (Blokland, 2017). Besides, neighborhood ties may function as potential resources for support on which residents can rely if necessary. Inhabitants who feel more supported will experience less vulnerability and, consequently, less fear (Boessen et al., 2017; Kanan & Pruitt, 2002). The positive relationship between neighborhood cohesion and feelings of safety is supported by various studies. It has been shown that residents who feel more attached to the neighborhood report lower levels of fear of crime (Adams & Serpe, 2000; Riger et al., 1981). Yuan and McNeeley (2015) found that people are less likely to consider their neighborhood as being unsafe when they have more friends or family living nearby.

Scholars have also hypothesized that cohesion may harm feelings of safety. In this case, neighborhood ties are expected to increase fear levels because residents with more ties are more likely to be exposed to crime-related news and information. It is predicted that increased exposure enhances the effect of indirect victimization and causes more concern about crime in the neighborhood, resulting in higher levels of fear (Covington & Taylor, 1991; Yuan & McNeeley, 2015). Other researchers have argued that the relationship between social ties and fear may be reciprocal, implicating that diminished social ties are both a cause and a consequence of fear (Kanan & Pruitt, 2002; Liska et al. 1988). We cannot empirically rule out the reverse causality because our paper relies on cross-sectional data.

2.5 Hypotheses

Based on the literature outlined above, we formulate the following four hypotheses:

Hypothesis 1:

Residents living in neighborhoods with more diversity experience more fear of crime.

Hypothesis 2:

Residents living in neighborhoods with more diversity experience less neighborhood cohesion (2a), which in turn increases fear of crime (2b).

Hypothesis 3:

Residents living in neighborhood with a larger out-group experience more fear of crime (3a) and less neighborhood cohesion (3b).

Hypothesis 4:

Residents who report higher levels of neighborhood cohesion experience less fear of crime.

All hypotheses are tested at the between level, with the exception of the fourth hypothesis which is examined at the within level.

3 Research Design

3.1 Data

The current study uses data from the Dutch Safety Monitor 2014 and individual register data from Statistics Netherlands. The Safety Monitor is a nationwide survey on crime-related feelings of unsafety and victimization. It is a high-quality national survey with a large and representative sample. Respondents were sampled from the municipality population register. The size of the sample was large enough to ensure that the respondents are equally distributed across the entire country. The overall response rate was 38.8%. In total, 86,382 respondents completed the self-administered questionnaires, either online (47.9%) or through a written questionnaire (52.1%).Footnote 10 The respondents live across the entire Netherlands, in areas with varying levels of ethnic diversity. Only the native Dutch respondents (N = 71,760) were selected for this study.Footnote 11 Unfortunately, our sample is too small to test our hypotheses separately for other ethnic groups. On average, approximately 8.3 native respondents share the same neighborhood. This is sufficient to correctly estimate regression coefficients and variance components, as well as the corresponding standard errors (Maas & Hox, 2005). The selected respondents live across the entire Netherlands and reside in 8,621 neighborhoods; 72% of all Dutch neighborhoods are included in the survey. The neighborhoods are administratively defined and have an average population size of 1,400 inhabitants. We merged the survey data with non-public individual register data (‘microdata’) from Statistics Netherlands. Access to microdata was granted under specific conditions.Footnote 12 We used data on the ethnicity of all Dutch inhabitants and information on the economic situation of all Dutch households.

3.2 Neighborhood cohesion and fear of crime

In our analysis, two outcome variables are distinguished: perceptions of neighborhood cohesion and fear of crime. The former is measured through a set of four items. These items are people in this neighborhood socialize pleasantly; I live in a cozy neighborhood where people help each other out and do things together; I feel at home with the people living in this neighborhood; and I have a lot of contact with other neighbors (answer categories: agree completely, agree, neither agree nor disagree, disagree and disagree completely). A factor analysis indicated that all four items load onto a single factor (see Table 5 in Appendix for details). The four items also appear to form a unidimensional scale, accounting for 71.10% of the variance. The scale is based on the average of at least three valid answers and is internally consistent with a Cronbach’s α of 0.86. This measure of neighborhood cohesion will be used to model cohesion both at the individual level and the neighborhood level.

To measure of fear of crime, three sub-dimensions are combined that relate to elements of behavior, threat and risk (Skogan, 1999). We constructed a scale consisting of five items. Respondents were asked how often they do not answer the door during evening hours; avoid certain areas in their neighborhood; feel unsafe walking in their neighborhood or being home alone during the evening; and are afraid of being victimized (answer categories: seldom or never, occasionally and frequently). A factor analysis resulted in one single factor (see Table 6 in Appendix for details). The items also proved to form a unidimensional and internally coherent scale, explaining 52.91% of the variance with a Cronbach’s α of 0.85. The fear of crime scale represents the average of at least three valid answers. A higher score on the scale indicates more fear of crime.

3.3 Ethnic Diversity and the Out-Group

The diversity measure is based on the distribution of out-group members among (a maximum of) 17 groups. Because only native Dutch were included in the analysis, the out-group consists of inhabitants with a migration background (or, in other words, non-native Dutch). The out-group variable is calculated as the number of ‘out-group’ inhabitants divided by the total number of inhabitants. The diversity measure and out-group variable are based on the ethnic composition of neighborhoods in 2014.

3.4 Control Variables

We control for several variables at the neighborhood level and individual level. Previous research has identified these controls as relevant determinants of neighborhood cohesion and/or fear of crime. At the neighborhood level, measures of economic disadvantage and crime are included. To measure the degree of economic disadvantage, an index was constructed that combines the percentage of low-income households, the average income of the context and the percentage of households for which social security is the main source of income (e.g. Vervoort, 2012). Before constructing the index, the distribution of average income was reversed and all indicators were standardized. Lastly, the mean of the standardized indicators was calculated. To control for crime, we included the registered number of reported burglaries and the registered number of violent crime incidents.Footnote 13 These variables represent the incidence of either burglary or violent crimes per 1000 members of the population in 2014 in a neighborhood. The descriptive statistics for the neighborhood-level variables (all measured continuously) are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics neighborhood-level variables

The individual control variables are age (in decades), gender, education level, presence of children in the household, whether social benefits are the main source of income of the household, income level and length of residence in the neighborhood (in years). When analyzing fear, we also controlled for burglary victimization. This variable is self-reported and was measured by asking respondents whether they had been a victim of burglary during the past five years. The descriptives (minimum and maximum values, mean, standard deviation and measurement scale) for the individual-level variables are included in Table 7 in the Appendix.

3.5 Analytical Strategy

We applied multilevel structural equation modeling (MSEM) to test the proposed pathways. MSEM is considered the most appropriate method to examine mediation in nested data because it explicitly separates the between level (neighborhoods) from the within level (individuals).Footnote 14 We follow the approach introduced by Preacher and colleagues (2010; 2011) to test mediation in a 2–1-1 design, with neighborhood diversity as a level 2 characteristic and cohesion and fear as level 1 characteristics. As mentioned previously, the mediation is examined at level 2 (the neighborhood level) because diversity is a neighborhood-level variable (for an illustration see Laméris et al., 2018). Based on the null model, we calculated intraclass correlations (ICCs) for an indication of how much variance is located at the neighborhood level. The ICCs vary between 0.07 (fear) and 0.09 (cohesion). These ICC values indicate that only a low proportion of the variance can be attributed to the neighborhood level. We obtained Akaike information criterion (AIC) and Bayesian information criterion (BIC) values to compare the model fit between the null model, the model with individual-levels variables and the ‘full’ model with both individual and neighborhood-level variables.Footnote 15 AIC and BIC are both criteria methods for model selection and to assess model fit. Lower values indicate a better model fit. Our full model proved to have the best model fit. We specify random intercepts and fixed slopes and perform our analyses in R, using the lavaan package (version 0.6–4). The random intercepts allow that the (individual) scores for fear of crime and neighborhood cohesion are predicated by the level of ethnic diversity which varies between neighborhoods. The slopes are fixed, meaning that it is assumed that the size of the potential diversity effect is the same for every individual (regardless of the neighborhood).

4 Results

4.1 Results from the Main Model

The results of our main model are shown in Table 2. All control variables are included. Most effects of the individual-level controls are in line with previous research (see Table 8 in the Appendix for details). This is also the case for the results regarding the neighborhood-level control variables. We find that in economically disadvantaged neighborhoods, cohesion levels are lower (b = −0.040) on average. There is, however, no significant association between economic neighborhood status and reported fear. In addition, it is shown that higher burglary rates and a higher incidence of violent crimes are significantly associated with more fear in the neighborhood (b = 0.003 and b = 0.001, respectively).

Table 2 Results path analysis

Next, we considered the diversity effects more closely. We observed that the diversity effect on fear of crime is not in line with our expectations: surprisingly, a higher level of diversity in the neighborhood is related to less fear. More specifically, inhabitants experience 0.01 (b = -0.105 × 0.10) less fear if diversity increases by 10% points. A possible explanation is that in highly diverse neighborhoods the out-group is more fragmented and, as a result, there is less threat because there is no dominant majority. The observed diversity effect–assessed at a 3-point scale–is, however, relatively small. The societal significance of this finding should therefore not be overestimated (Bernardi et al. 2017). Moreover, in an additional robustness analysis the diversity effect on fear was no longer significant.Footnote 16 It follows that hypothesis 1, predicting more fear in more diverse neighborhoods, is rejected.

The model also shows that more diversity in the neighborhood is related to less neighborhood cohesion. This diversity effect is in line with Putnam’s constrict hypothesis (2007) and most previous research on this relationship (e.g. Gijsberts et al., 2012; Lancee & Dronkers, 2011). In a neighborhood with 10% points more diversity, the average level of cohesion decreased by 0.04 (b = −0.389 × 0.10), measured on a 5-point scale. Hypothesis 2a–more diversity is associated with less neighborhood cohesion–is thus confirmed.Footnote 17 It was found that, at the between-level, higher levels of cohesion are accompanied by less fear (b = −0.204). This result supports hypothesis 2b. The indirect effect of diversity on fear is 0.079 (b = −0.389 × −0.204), indicating that inhabitants of ethnically diverse neighborhoods experience more fear because diversity has weakened their social relationships in the neighborhood. It also demonstrates that the level of perceived safety is higher in more socially cohesive neighborhoods, which is consistent with the literature on collective efficacy and the previous findings of Rountree and Land (1996).

On the basis of the third hypothesis, we expect that a larger out-group in the neighborhood is related to more fear (3a) and less cohesion (3b). The results indicate that a larger out-group is indeed associated with 0.03 more fear if the out-group increases by 10% points (b = 0.003 × 10). This finding confirms previous research showing that natives feel more unsafe in neighborhoods inhabited by a relatively large out-group. Furthermore, the out-group effect on cohesion is also in line with our expectations. If the size of the out-group increases by 10% points, the respondents experience 0.06 (b = −0.006 × 10) less cohesion. Consequently, hypotheses 3a and 3b are both supported.

At the within level, we also address the association between cohesion and fear. Our fourth hypothesis predicts a negative relationship between a resident’s level of cohesion and sense of unsafety. Contrary to the hypothesis, we find that more cohesion is associated with slightly higher fear levels (b = 0.006). This result seems to suggest that the negative aspects of being more socially integrated in a neighborhood outweigh potential positive effects. Hypothesis 4 remains unconfirmed. All our hypotheses and their results are summarized in Table 3.

Table 3 Schematic overview of results

4.2 Robustness and Additional Checks

We carried out three additional analyses to evaluate the robustness of our results and to explore whether we can further improve our understanding of the observed effects. First, we wanted to consider the possibility that the detected diversity effect on neighborhood cohesion depends on the size of the out-group. The expectation is that the effect of diversity is stronger when the share of the out-group increases. To test this, a model was estimated including interaction effects between diversity and out-group size. The interactions turned out not to be significant, suggesting that diversity effects do not depend on out-group size. As a second robustness check, we explored whether different outcomes are produced by excluding respondents who live in a relative diverse neighborhood (with a HHI larger than 0.8) with only a small out-group (less than 10%). This model, which included 52.098 respondents, was estimated to assess whether our main model was biased by the characteristics of this specific group. The diversity effects did not, however, change much. The results of these two models are available upon request.

In our third model, we examined the role of cultural distance and whether the effects of diversity and the out-group are stronger when resulting from non-European/Anglo-Saxon (‘non-Western’) groups (in contrast to European/Anglo-Saxon, or ‘Western’, groups).Footnote 18 The idea is that anomie and threat are more likely to occur when the perceived cultural distance between different group members is larger. Dutch natives consider the non-European/Anglo-Saxon group to be the most culturally distant (Schlueter & Scheepers, 2010). The results of this additional analysis are reported in Table 4. Our ideas are not supported: we find that diversity based on the share of European/Anglo-Saxon groups even has a slightly larger effect on cohesion. Besides, it seems to make (almost) no difference whether the out-group measures are based on the composition of European/Anglo-Saxon or non-European/Anglo-Saxon groups.

Table 4 Results path analysis. (Non-)European/Anglo-Saxon diversity and out-groups

5 Discussion and Conclusion

The current study examined to what extent the ethnic composition of Dutch neighborhoods is associated with residents’ perceptions of cohesion and fear. We considered the role of two different composition aspects: (1) the level of ethnic diversity and (2) the size of the out-group. Previous scholars analyzing similar relationships have often failed to specify whether it is expected that a neighborhood’s ethnic composition of a neighborhood will affect perceptions due to either diversity or due to out-group size. We argue that it is necessary to empirically distinguish between these two aspects: they are theoretically distinct and relate to different explanatory mechanisms. We did so by using group-specific measures of diversity and out-group and simultaneously analyzed their impact on both cohesion and feelings of unsafety among Dutch natives. Furthermore, in an additional analysis, we distinguished between different ‘sorts’ of diversity and out-groups to shed more light on the role of cultural distance and similarity. No support was found for the expectation that natives’ levels of cohesion and unsafety are particularly affected by the presence of more culturally distant groups, although this is often implicitly assumed by researchers.

Our results showed that the hypothesized effect of diversity is most consistently found in relation to neighborhood cohesion. We observed that more diversity in the neighborhood is related to less cohesion. Our study is one of the first to detect a diversity effect on cohesion based on this modified measure. It is possible that the found diversity effect on cohesion is slightly underestimated since the level of diversity was not calculated on the basis of the entire neighborhood population (but only on the different shares of non-native groups). In prior research, scholars have often been unable to conclude whether reduced levels of cohesion were better explained by ethnic diversity or rather the concentration of specific minority groups (e.g. Dinesen & Sønderskov, 2015; Glas et al., 2019; Laurence et al., 2019; Sluiter et al., 2015). We add to this literature by providing empirical support for Putnam’s constrict hypothesis. Despite this contribution, two limitations should be noted here: only natives were included in the analyses and, secondly, we narrowed our focus to neighborhood cohesion, whereby we did not consider other elements of cohesion that are not neighborhood specific. Another limitation of the current study that should be mentioned is the use of cross-sectional data. We therefore cannot control for selection processes or assume causality. In order to make actual causal claims, longitudinal data are necessary.

No support was found for the diversity hypothesis regarding fear of crime. This outcome contradicts the results of a recent study that found a positive association between ethnic diversity and fear at various spatial levels, including the neighborhood (Glas et al., 2019). In this earlier study, ethnic diversity was measured using the conventional HHI. In addition, no distinction was made between the different aspects of ethnic composition. These differences may explain why contradicting results were found. The current research shows that differences in fear are better explained by the size of the out-group. We therefore conclude that diversity may induce negative effects, but that these effects are limited to neighborhood cohesion and do not affect fear of crime. The results suggest that feelings of anomie–which explain why cohesion levels are lower in ethnically diverse neighborhoods–do not spill-over into feelings of unsafety.

Based on the threat mechanism, we also researched the role of out-group size in relation to levels of cohesion and fear. The idea that living in a neighborhood with ethnic ‘others’–regardless of their internal diversity–may be threatening, gained support. We found that a larger out-group in the neighborhood is associated with less cohesion and more fear. It follows that both the level of diversity and the size of the out-group in a neighborhood are associated with lower cohesion levels. The out-group effect on fear confirms previous research showing a positive association between the presence of minority groups and fear among members of the (traditional) majority (Chiricos et al., 1997; Moeller, 1989).

This study also examines the interrelationship between neighborhood cohesion and feelings of unsafety. We found a specific pattern for each level of analysis, indicating that the relationship between cohesion and fear operates differently at different levels. At the individual level, cohesion and fear are positively related: inhabitants who experience more cohesion report slightly higher fear levels. This supports the idea that local social ties may elevate fear levels because people who are socially integrated in the neighborhood are more exposed to crime related news and stories (Boessen et al., 2017; Covington & Taylor, 1991). Our results also demonstrate that living in a neighborhood that is considered cohesive decreases fear levels at the neighborhood level.

Overall, we showed in our analyses which elements of the ethnic composition of a neighborhood play a role in explaining patterns of fear of crime and neighborhood cohesion and, by doing so, provide a more nuanced understanding of the diversity debate. We agree with Koopmans and Schaeffer (2015) who urged scholars to move away from using conventional diversity measures in order to “move ahead” the research field (p. 173). Our study illustrates what can be gained from using a more innovative method to measure the ethnic composition of neighborhoods when researching neighborhood cohesion and fear of crime patterns.