Skip to main content
Log in

Does Relative Deprivation Matter in Developing Countries: Evidence from Six Transition Economies

  • Published:
Social Indicators Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Existing evidence on whether relative status is an important determinant of well-being has two key features: it is mainly derived from high income countries, and it relies on relative deprivation measures constructed by the researchers, rather than being reported by the respondents. The need to construct relative deprivation measures imposes strong assumptions with respect to obvervability of relative deprivation. This paper adds evidence on the importance of social comparisons based on self-reported relative status assessments, which obviates the need to impose observability assumptions. The underlying survey data has the added benefit of coming from six transition economies at different levels of economic development, making it possible to explore the role of social comparisons at low income levels. Interviewer’s observations of the household’s relative deprivation are also employed to address the endogeneity concerns associated with using self-reported relative status measures. The results suggest that relative deprivation is indeed a welfare-relevant concern, even in the poorest countries in Eastern Europe. Among multiple reference groups available in the data, local social comparisons appear to be most salient.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Note that unlike other studies, Senik (2004) finds reference group income to exert a positive effect on individual satisfaction, which the author interprets in the spirit of Hirschman’s “tunnel effect” (Hirschman and Rothschild 1973), i.e. the reference group income is treated more like an indication of their own future income, rather than of their inferior relative status.

  2. For an overview of the history of the theory of social comparison, see Suls and Wheeler (2000).

  3. In tables, the countries are labeled by their acronyms, respectively, MKD, KAZ, MLD, SRB, TAJ, and UKR.

  4. In Serbia the sample size is 2,401 households, as data for the Roma booster were not part of the publicly available dataset.

  5. The results in Sect. 5 are not affected if the asset index is replaced in regressions with the set of assets that are used in its construction by means of a set of dummy variables.

  6. Note that because of the high share of respondents who were not able to rank their standard of living in comparison to that of their grandparents, the empirical analysis in Sect. 5 omits this comparison group.

  7. One may worry that the interviewer’s assessment of the respondent’s relative deprivation may be influenced by the mood of the respondent. There are two factors mitigating this possibility. First, the interviewer is not asked to provide a direct assessment of relative deprivation, rather the assessment of the neighbourhood and the assessment of the property are provided separately, and these two questions are not consecutive. Secondly, in the questionnaire the respondent’s assessment of relative status is in the first half of the interview, whereas the respondent’s assessment is provided at the end of the interview, after attitudinal questions that do not probe the respondent’s welfare in either absolute or relative terms.

  8. Ferrer-i-Carbonell and Frijters (2004) examine the more stringent assumption of cardinality, i.e. that the difference between responses 2 and 3 on the satisfaction scale is the same, for instance, as the difference between 6 and 7. Relying on data from the German Socio-Economic Panel (GSOEP) they look at differences between ordinal and cardinal models of life satisfaction using the 11-step response to the following question: “How happy are you at present with your life as a whole? Please answer by using the following scale in which 0 means totally unhappy, and 10 means totally happy.” They find that results are largely unaffected by the choice of cardinal vs. ordinal specification.

  9. I am grateful to an anonymous referee for pointing this out.

  10. I am grateful to Andrew Clark for pointing this out.

  11. It should be noted that ’neighbours’ was not among the possible answers in the survey question ’Whose income would you be most likely to compare your own with?’ Certainly there can be considerable overlap between neighbours and friends/co-workers. Note also that Clark and Senik do not have data on the incomes or consumption levels of these reference groups, such that they are not able to directly test for the effect of the individual’s status relative to her reference group on her well-being.

References

  • Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2004). Well-being over time in Britain and the USA. Journal of Public Economics, 88, 1359–1386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P. H., Bulte, E., & Zhang, X. (2011). Positional spending and status seeking in rural China. Journal of Development Economics, 96, 139–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, X., & Zhang, X. (2009) Blood for social status: Preliminary evidence from rural China. In Annual meeting, July 26–28, 2009, Milwaukee, Wisconsin No. 49411, Agricultural and Applied Economics Association, May.

  • Clark, A. E., Frijters, P., & Shields, M. A. (2008). Relative income, happiness, and utility: An explanation for the Easterlin paradox and other puzzles. Journal of Economic Literature, 46, 95–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., & Oswald, A. J. (1996). Satisfaction and comparison income. Journal of Public Economics, 61, 359–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., & Senik, C. (2010). Who compares to whom? The anatomy of income comparisons in Europe. The Economic Journal, 120, 573–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cojocaru, A., & Diagne, M. (2013) How reliable and consistent are subjective measures of welfare in Europe and Central Asia? Evidence from the second life in transition survey. Policy research working paper 6359, The World Bank.

  • Deaton, A. (2001). Relative deprivation, inequality, and mortality. NBER working paper No. 8099, National Bureau of Economic Research.

  • Di Tella, R., Haisken-DeNew, J., & MacCulloch, R. (2010). Happiness adaptation to income and to status in an individual panel. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 76(3), 834–852.

  • Di Tella, R., MacCulloch, R. J., & Oswald, A. J. (2003). The macroeconomics of happiness. Review of Economics and Statistics, 85, 809–827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Seidlitz, L., & Diener, M. (1993). The relationship between income and subjective well-being: Relative or absolute? Social Indicators Research, 28, 195–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Diener, M., & Diener, C. (1995). Factors predicting the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 851–864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Tay, L., & Oishi, S. (2013). Rising income and the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 267–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. (2001). Income and happiness: Towards an unified theory. Economic Journal, 111(473), 465–484.

  • Eibner, C., & Evans, W. N. (2005). Relative deprivation, poor health habits, and mortality. Journal of Human Resources, XL, 591–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fafchamps, M., & Shilpi, F. (2008). Subjective welfare, isolation, and relative consumption. Journal of Development Economics, 86, 43–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A. (2005). Income and well-being: An empirical analysis of the comparison income effect. Journal of Public Economics, 89, 997–1019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A., Frijters, P. (2004). How important is methodology for the estimates of the determinants of happiness? Economic Journal, 114(497), 641–659.

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Filmer, D., & Pritchett, L. H. (2001). Estimating wealth effects without expenditure data—or tears: An application to educational enrollments in states of India. Demography, 38, 115–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. (2007). Falling behind: How rising inequality harms the middle class. The Aaron Wildavsky forum for public policy. Oakland: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H., & Levine, A. S. (2007). Expenditure cascades. Ithaca: Cornell University Mimeograph.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, S., & Loewenstein, G. (1999). Hedonic adaptation. In D. Kahneman & E. Diener (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 302–329). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2001). Happiness and economics: How the economy and institutions affect human well-being. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goethals, G., & Darley, J. (1977). Social comparison theory: An attributional approach. In J. Suls & R. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison processes: Theoretical and empirical perspective (pp. 259–278). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, C., & Felton, A. (2006). Inequality and happiness: Insights from Latin America. Journal of Economic Inequality, 4, 107–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschman, A. O., & Rothschild, M. (1973). The changing tolerance for income inequality in the course of economic development; with a mathematical appendix. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 87, 544–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–291.

  • Knight, J., Song, L., & Gunatilaka, R. (2009). Subjective well-being and its determinants in rural China. China Economic Review, 20, 635–649.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenberg, J. (1996). Consuming because others consume. Social Theory and Practice, 22, 273–297.

  • Luttmer, E. F. P. (2005). Neighbors as negatives: Relative earnings and well-being. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 120, 963–1002.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride, M. (2001). Relative-income effects on subjective well-being in the cross-section. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 45, 251–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melenberg, B. (1992). Micro-economic models of consumer behaviour and welfare. PhD dissertation, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands.

  • Ravallion, M., & Lokshin, M. (2002). Self-rated economic welfare in Russia. European Economic Review, 46, 1453–1473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravallion, M., & Lokshin, M. (2010). Who cares about relative deprivation? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 73, 171–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roh, J. (2011). Essays on the political economy of welfare and redistribution. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Senik, C. (2004). When information dominates comparison: Learning from Russian subjective panel data. Journal of Public Economics, 88, 2099–2123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stutzer, A. (2004). The role of income aspirations in individual happiness. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 54(1), 89–109.

  • Senik, C. (2009). Direct evidence on income comparisons and their welfare effects. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 72, 408–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suls, J., & Wheeler, L. (2000). Handbook of social comparison: Theory and research (1st ed.). Berlin: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • van de Stadt, H., Kapteyn, A., & van de Geer, S. (1985). The relativity of utility: Evidence from panel data. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 67, 179–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (1991). Is happiness relative? Social Indicators Research, 24, 1–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank Andrew Clark, Bob Rijkers, Branko Milanovic, Carol Graham, Madiha Afzal, Michael Lokshin, Peter Murrell, as well as two anonymous referees for comments on an earlier version of this paper. All remaining errors are mine alone. The views expressed in this paper are those of the author and should not be attributed to the World Bank Group or any affiliated organization.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alexandru Cojocaru.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cojocaru, A. Does Relative Deprivation Matter in Developing Countries: Evidence from Six Transition Economies. Soc Indic Res 125, 735–756 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-015-0864-2

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-015-0864-2

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation