Sex Roles

pp 1–13 | Cite as

Gender Labels on Gender-Neutral Colors: Do they Affect Children’s Color Preferences and Play Performance?

Original Article

Abstract

Gender-typed color preferences are widely documented, and there has been increasing concern that they affect children’s play preferences. However, it is unclear whether such color preferences exist across cultures, how they have emerged, and how gender color-coding affects performance. Chinese preschoolers (n = 126) aged 59 to 94 months were tested. First, we assessed their gender-typed color preferences using forced-choice tasks with color cards and pictures of neutral toys in gender-typed colors. Second, we tested if gender labels could affect color preferences by labeling two gender-neutral colors as gender-typed and assessed children’s liking for them using a rating task and a forced-choice task with pictures of neutral toys in the labeled colors. Third, we assigned children a tangram puzzle (i.e., a puzzle using geometric pieces) painted either in the gender-appropriate or gender-inappropriate color and measured the number of pieces they completed and their speed. Results showed that Chinese children exhibited the same gender-typed color preferences as Western children did. Moreover, applying gender labels amplified a gender difference in color preferences, thus providing direct and strong evidence for the social-cognitive pathway underlying gender-typed preferences. Finally, color-coding as gender-appropriate or -inappropriate had no impact on performance but the gender labels improved boys’ performance. These results add to knowledge on how gender-related information affects children’s responses to the social world and suggest that the current gender color divide should be reconsidered.

Keywords

Gender labels Gender color-coding Color preferences Play performance Gender differences 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

This project was approved by the Psychology Departmental Research Ethics Committee at the University of Hong Kong. All children participated with the written consent of their parent. Children’s verbal assent was also obtained prior to the experiment.

Supplementary material

11199_2017_875_MOESM1_ESM.docx (384 kb)
ESM 1 (DOCX 384 kb)

References

  1. Alexander, G. M. (2003). An evolutionary perspectives of sex-typed toy preferences: Pink, blue and the brain. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 7–14.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021833110722.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Ambady, N., Shih, M., Kim, A., & Pittinsky, T. L. (2001). Stereotype susceptibility in children: Effects of identity activation on quantitative performance. Psychological Science, 12, 385–390.  https://doi.org/10.1111/ 1467-9280.00371.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Antill, J. K., Goodnow, J. J., Russell, G., & Cotton, S. (1996). The influence of parents and family context on children’s involvement in household tasks. Sex Roles, 34(3), 215–236.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01544297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Armenta, B. E. (2010). Stereotype boost and stereotype threat effects: The moderating role of ethnic identification. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16(1), 94–98.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017564.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Arthur, A. E., Bigler, R. S., & Ruble, D. N. (2009). An experimental test of the effects of gender constancy on sex typing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 427–446.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.08.002.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Auster, C. J., & Mansbach, C. S. (2012). The gender marketing of toys: An analysis of color and type of type on the Disney store website. Sex Roles, 67, 375–388.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0177-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Basow, S. A. (1992). Gender stereotypes and roles (3rd ed.). Pacific Grove: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  8. Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.4.354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Black, R. W., Tomlinson, B., & Korobkova, K. (2016). Play and identity in gendered LEGO franchises. International Journal of Play, 5, 64–76.  https://doi.org/10.1080/21594937.2016.1147284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Blakemore, J. E. O., & Centers, R. E. (2005). Characteristics of boys' and girls' toys. Sex Roles, 53(9–10), 619–633.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-7729-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Caldera, Y. M., Huston, A. C., & O’Brien, M. (1989). Social interactions and play patterns of parents and toddlers with feminine, masculine and neutral toys. Child Development, 60, 70–76.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1131072.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Casey, B. M., Andrews, N., Schindler, H., Kersh, J. E., Samper, A., & Copley, J. (2008). The development of spatial skills through interventions involving block building activities. Cognition and Instruction, 26, 269–309.  https://doi.org/10.1080/07370000802177177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chattopadhyay, A., Gorn, G. J., & Drake, P. (2010). Differences and similarities in hue preferences between Chinese and Caucasians. In A. Krishna (Ed.), Sensory marketing: Research on the sensuality of products (pp. 219–239). New York: Taylor and Francis Group.Google Scholar
  14. Chen, E. S. L., & Rao, N. (2011). Gender socialization in Chinese kindergartens: Teachers’ contributions. Sex Roles, 64, 103–116.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9873-4.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Cherney, I. D., & Dempsey, J. (2010). Young children’s classification, stereotyping, and play behavior for gender neutral and ambiguous toys. Journal of Educational Psychology, 30(6), 651–669.  https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2010.498416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cherney, I. D., & London, K. (2006). Gender-linked differences in the toys, television shows, computer games, and outdoor activities of 5-to 13-year-old children. Sex Roles, 54, 717–726.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-006-9037-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Chiu, S. W., Gervan, S., Fairbrother, C., Johnson, L. L., Owen-Anderson, A. F. H., Bradley, S. J., … Zucker, K. J. (2006). Sex-dimorphic color preference in children with gender identity disorder: A comparison to clinical and community controls. Sex Roles, 55, 385–395.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-006-9089-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cohen, P. N. (2013). Children’s gender and parents’ color preferences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 393–397.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-012-9951-5.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., & Pfäfflin, F. (2003). Transgenderism and intersexuality in childhood and adolescence. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  20. Cunningham, S. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2011). The colour of gender stereotyping. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 598–614.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02023.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Davies, D. R. (1986). Children’s performance as a function of sex-typed labels. British Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 173–175.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1986.tb00717.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Davies, D. R. (1989). The effects of gender-typed labels on children’s performance. Current Psychology: Research & Reviews, 8, 267–272.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02686725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Del Giudice, M. (2012). The 20th century reversal of pink-blue gender coding: A scientific urban legend? [letter to the Editor]. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 1321–1323.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-012-0002-z.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Del Giudice, M. (2017). Pink, blue, and gender: An update. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 46, 1555–1563.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-017-1024-3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Ellis, L., & Ficek, C. (2001). Color preferences according to gender and sexual orientation. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 1375–1379.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00231-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., & Buchner, A. (1996). GPower: A general power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28(1), 1–11.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Fagot, B. I., & Leinbach, M. D. (1989). The young child’s gender schema: Environmental input, internal organization. Child Development, 60, 663–672.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1130731.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Fulcher, M., & Hayes, A. R. (2017). Building a pink dinosaur: The effects of gendered construction toys on girls’ and boys’ play. Sex Roles. Advance online publication.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-017-0806-3.
  29. Gold, D., & Berger, C. (1978). Problem solving performance of young boys and girls as a function of task-appropriateness and sex-identity. Sex Roles, 4, 183–195.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00287499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Goldberg, S. (2017, January). Why we put a transgender girl on the cover of National Geographic. National Geographic: Gender Revolution. Retrieved from http://www.nationalgeographic.com/magazine/2017/01/editors-note-gender/.
  31. Green, V. A., Bigler, R., & Catherwood, D. (2004). The variability and flexibility of gender-typed toy play: A close look at children’s behavioral responses to counterstereotypic models. Sex Roles, 78, 371–386.  https://doi.org/10.1023/B:SERS.0000049227.05170.aa.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Halim, M. L., & Ruble, D. (2010). Gender identity and stereotyping in early and middle childhood. In J. Chrisler & D. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook of gender research in psychology (pp. 495–525). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hargreaves, D. J., Bates, H. M., & Foot, J. M. C. (1985). Sex-typed labeling affects task performance. British Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 153–155.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1985.tb00674.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Hidi, S. (2000). An interest researcher’s perspective: The effects of extrinsic and intrinsic factors on motivation. In S. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 309–339). San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hines, M. (2010). Sex-related variation in human behavior and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 448–456.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.07.005.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  36. Hurlbert, A. C., & Ling, Y. (2007). Biological components of sex differences in color preference. Current Biology, 17, 623–625.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hyde, J. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60, 581–592.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Ickes, W., Gesn, P. R., & Graham, T. (2000). Gender differences in empathic accuracy: Differential ability or differential motivation? Personal Relationships, 7, 95–109.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201276007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Jadva, V., Hines, M., & Golombok, S. (2010). Infants’ preferences for toys, colors and shapes: Sex differences and similarities. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 1261–1273.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-010-9618-z.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Jirout, J. J., & Newcombe, N. S. (2015). Building blocks for developing social skills: Evidence from a large, representative U. S. Sample. Psychological Science, 26, 302–310.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614563338.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Kane, E. W. (2006). “No way my boys are going to be like that!”: Parents’ responses to children’s gender nonconformity. Gender and Society, 20, 149–176.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0891243205284276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children’s sex-role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 82–173). Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  43. Lee, J. F. K., & Collins, P. (2008). Gender voices in Hong Kong English textbooks—Some past and current practices. Sex Roles, 59, 127–137.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9414-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Lee, J., Lee, J. O., & Collins, D. (2009). Enhancing children's spatial sense using tangrams. Childhood Education, 86(2), 92–94.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00094056.2010.10523120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Leinbach, M. D., Hort, B. E., & Fagot, B. I. (1997). Bears are for boys: Metaphorical associations in young children’s gender stereotypes. Cognitive Development, 12, 107–130.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(97)90032-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Li, R. Y. H., & Wong, W. I. (2016). Gender-typed play and social abilities in boys and girls: Are they related? Sex Roles, 74, 399–410.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0580-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–1498.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1130467.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2011). Pretty in pink: The early development of gender-stereotyped colour preferences. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 656–667.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02027.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  50. Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  51. Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119–1134.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1129498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues: Cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 67–70.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00276.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 903–933.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.903.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Martin, C. L., DiDonato, M. D., Clary, L., Fabes, R. A., Kreiger, T., Palermo, F., … Hanish, L. (2012). Preschool children with gender normative and gender non-normative peer preferences: Psychosocial and environmental correlates. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 831–847.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-012-9950-6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  55. Masters, J. C., Ford, M. E., Arend, R., Grotevant, H. D., & Clark, L. V. (1979). Modeling and labeling as integrated determinants of children’s sex-typed imitative behavior. Child Development, 50, 364–371.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1129411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Montemayor, R. (1977). Children’s performance in a game and their attraction to it as a function of sex-typed labels. Child Development, 45, 152–156.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1127761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Mulvey, K. L., Miller, B., & Rizzardi, V. (2017). Gender and engineering aptitude: Is the color of science, technology, engineering, and math materials related to children’s performance? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 160, 119–126.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Paoletti, J. B. (1987). Clothing and gender in America: Children’s fashions, 1890-1920. Signs, 13, 136–143.  https://doi.org/10.1086/494390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Paoletti, J. B. (2012). Pink and blue: Telling the boys from the girls in America. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
  60. Picariello, M. L., Greenberg, D., & Pillemer, D. B. (1990). Children’s sex-related stereotyping of colors. Child Development, 61, 1453–1460.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1130755.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Pomerleau, A., Bolduc, D., Malcuit, G., & Cossette, L. (1990). Pink or blue: Environmental gender stereotypes in the first two years of life. Sex Roles, 22, 359–367.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00288339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. (1998). Gender development. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 933–1016). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  63. Ruble, D. N., Taylor, L. J., Cyphers, L., Greulich, F. K., Lurye, L. E., & Shrout, P. E. (2007). Effect of gender constancy on age-related changes in gender beliefs. Child Development, 78, 1121–1136.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01056.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Saito, M. (1994). A cross-cultural study on color preference in three Asian cities: Comparison between Tokyo, Taipei and Tianjin. Japanese Psychological Research, 36(4), 219–232.  https://doi.org/10.4992/psycholres1954.36.219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Saito, M. (1996). A comparative study of color preferences in Japan, China, and Indonesia, with emphasis on the preference for white. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 83, 115–128.  https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1996.83.1.115.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Serbin, L. A., Powlishta, K. K., & Gulko, J. (1993). The development of sex typing in middle childhood. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 58(2), 1–74.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1166118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Shih, M., Ambady, N., Richeson, J. A., Fujita, K., & Gray, H. M. (2002). Stereotype performance boosts: The impact of self-relevance and the manner of stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 638–647.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.83.3.638.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Shih, M., Pittinsky, T. L., & Ambady, N. (1999). Stereotype susceptibility: Identity salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Psychological Science, 10, 80–83.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women's math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28.  https://doi.org/10.1006/jesp.1998.1373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Sprafkin, C., Serbin, L. A., Denier, C., & Connor, J. M. (1983). Sex-differentiated play: Cognitive consequences and early interventions. In M. B. Liss (Ed.), Social and cognitive skills: Sex roles and child’s play (pp. 167–192). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  71. Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Stein, A., Pohly, S., & Mueller, E. (1971). The influence of masculine, feminine and neutral tasks on children’s achievement behavior, expectancies of success and attainments values. Child Development, 42, 195–207.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1127075.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Sweet, E. V. (2013, August). Same as it ever was? Gender and children's toys over the 20th century. Paper presented at the 108th annual American Sociological Association meeting in New York, NY.Google Scholar
  74. Taylor, C., Clifford, A., & Franklin, A. (2013). Color preferences are not universal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1015–1027.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Thomas, R. M. (1999). Human development theories: Windows on culture. Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
  76. Van Yperen, N. W. (2003). Task interest and actual performance: The moderating effects of assigned and adopted purpose goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1006–1015.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A metal-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.117.2.250.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Wachman, R. (2012, March). Lego’s profits rise as it thinks pink. The Guardian. Retrieved from https://www.theguardian.com/business/2012/mar/01/lego-profits-rise-pink.
  79. Weisgram, E. S., Fulcher, M., & Dinella, L. M. (2014). Pink gives girls permission: Exploring the roles of explicit gender labels and gender-typed colors on preschool children’s toy preferences. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35, 401–409.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2014.06.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Wong, W. I., & Hines, M. (2015a). Effects of gender color-coding on toddlers’ gender-typical toy play. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44(5), 1233–1242.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0400-5.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Wong, W. I., & Hines, M. (2015b). Preferences for pink and blue: The development of color preferences as a distinct gender-typed behavior in toddlers. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44(5), 1243–1254.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-015-0489-1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Yu, L., Winter, S., & Xie, D. (2010). The child play behavior and activity questionnaire: A parent-report measure of childhood gender-related behavior in China. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 807–815.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9403-4.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Zosuls, K. M., Ruble, D. N., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shrout, P. E., Bornstein, M. H., & Greulich, F. K. (2009). The acquisition of gender labels in infancy: Implications for gender-typed play. Developmental Psychology, 45, 668–701.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyThe University of Hong KongHong KongPeople’s Republic of China

Personalised recommendations