Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of Gender Color-Coding on Toddlers’ Gender-Typical Toy Play

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Gender color-coding of children’s toys may make certain toys more appealing or less appealing to a given gender. We observed toddlers playing with two gender-typical toys (a train, a doll), once in gender-typical colors and once in gender-atypical colors. Assessments occurred twice, at 20–40 months of age and at 26–47 months of age. A Sex × Time × Toy × Color ANOVA showed expected interactions between Sex and Toy and Sex and Color. Boys played more with the train than girls did and girls played more with the doll and with pink toys than boys did. The Sex × Toy × Color interaction was not significant, but, at both time points, boys and girls combined played more with the gender-atypical toy when its color was typical for their sex than when it was not. This effect appeared to be caused largely by boys’ preference for, or avoidance of, the doll and by the use of pink. Also, at both time points, gender differences in toy preferences were larger in the gender-typical than in the gender-atypical color condition. At Time 2, these gender differences were present only in the gender-typical color condition. Overall, the results suggest that, once acquired, gender-typical color preferences begin to influence toy preferences, especially those for gender-atypical toys and particularly in boys. They thus could enlarge differences between boys’ and girls’ toy preferences. Because boys’ and girls’ toys elicit different activities, removing the gender color-coding of toys could encourage more equal learning opportunities.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The education levels were assessed using UNESCO classification, which defines tertiary education as education beyond post-secondary non-tertiary education. Tertiary education includes 1st stage (not leading directly to an advanced research qualification) and 2nd stage (leading to an advanced research qualification).

  2. More details about the analyses are available from the corresponding author upon request.

  3. This analysis came at the suggestion of a reviewer.

  4. Cohen’s d was calculated with the formula: t/√N.

References

  • Alexander, G. M. (2003). An evolutionary perspective of sex-typed toy preferences: Pink, blue, and the brain. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 7–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in response to children’s toys in nonhuman primates (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 467–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, G. M., Wilcox, T., & Woods, R. (2009). Sex differences in infants’ visual interest in toys. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 427–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S. A., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex- stereotyped toy preferences. Psychological Science, 3, 203–206.

  • Block, J. H. (1983). Differential premises arising from differential socialization of the sexes. Child Development, 54, 1335–1354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cabin, R. J., & Mitchell, R. J. (2000). To Bonferroni or not to Bonferroni: When and how are the questions. Bulletin of the Ecological Society of America, 81, 246–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caldera, Y. M., Huston, A. C., & O’Brien, M. (1989). Social interactions and play patterns of parents and toddlers with feminine, masculine and neutral toys. Child Development, 60, 70–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, S. W., Gervan, S., Fairbrother, C., Johnson, L. L., Owen-Anderson, A. F. H., Bradley, S. J., et al. (2006). Sex-dimorphic color preference in children with gender identity disorder: A comparison to clinical and community controls. Sex Roles, 55, 385–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, S. J., & MaCrae, C. N. (2011). The colour of gender stereotyping. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 598–614.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eliot, L. (2009). Pink brain, blue brain: How small differences grow into troublesome gaps—and what we can do about it. Oxford: Oneworld.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fagot, B. I. (1978). The influence of sex of child on parental reactions to toddler children. Child Development, 49, 459–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fagot, B. I. (1983). Interactive behavior code. Unpublished instrument.

  • Feinman, S. (1981). A status theory of the evaluation of sex-role and age-role behavior. Sex Roles, 10, 445–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fine, C. (2010). Delusions of gender. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golombok, S., Rust, J., Zervoulis, K., Croudace, T., Golding, J., & Hines, M. (2012). Continuity in sex-typed behavior from preschool to adolescence: A longitudinal population study of boys and girls aged 3–13 years. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 591–597.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halim, M. L., Ruble, D. N., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Zosuls, K. M., Lyrye, L. E., & Greulich, F. K. (2014). Pink frilly dresses and the avoidance of all things “girly”: Children’s appearance rigidity and cognitive theories of gender development. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1091–1101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy preferences parallel those of children. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 359–364.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hines, M. (2010). Sex-related variation in human behavior and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 448–456.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hurlbert, A. C., & Ling, Y. (2007). Biological components of sex differences in color preference. Current Biology, 17, 623–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60, 581–592.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Idle, T., Wood, E., & Desmarais, S. (1993). Gender role socialization in toy play situations: Mothers and fathers with their sons and daughters. Sex Roles, 28, 679–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jadva, V., Golombok, S., & Hines, M. (2010). Infants’ preferences for toys, colors and shapes: Sex differences and similarities. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 1261–1273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. (Producer). (2011). Fighting the power of pink [Radio broadcast]. Retrieved from http://www.bbc.co.uk/programmes/b010y39d.

  • Kusuoka, H., & Hoffman, J. I. E. (2002). Advice on statistical analysis for circulation research. Circulation Research, 91, 662–671.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leinbach, M. D., Hort, B. E., & Fagot, B. I. (1997). Bears are for boys: Metaphorical associations in young children’s gender stereotypes. Cognitive Development, 12, 107–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2011). Pretty in pink: The early development of gender-stereotyped colour preferences. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 656–667.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents’ differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masters, J. C., Ford, M. E., Arend, R., Grotevant, H. D., & Clark, L. V. (1979). Modeling and labeling as integrated determinants of children’s sex-typed imitative behavior. Child Development, 50, 364–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, R. G. (1981). Simultaneous statistical inference. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Orenstein, P. (2011). Cinderella ate my daughter. New York, NY: HarperCollins Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, S. E., & Schloss, K. B. (2010). An ecological valence theory of color preferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 8877–8882.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paoletti, J. B. (2012). Pink and blue: Telling the boys from the girls in America. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pasterski, V., Geffner, M. E., Brain, C., Hindmarsh, P., Brook, C., & Hines, M. (2005). Prenatal hormones and postnatal socialization by parents as determinants of male-typical toy play in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Child Development, 76, 264–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, A. M. (2011). Is pink necessary? The New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2011/01/23/books/review/Paul-t.html.

  • Perneger, T. V. (1998). What’s wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. British Medical Journal, 316, 1236–1238.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Picariello, M. L., Greenberg, D., & Pillemer, D. B. (1990). Children’s sex-related stereotyping of colors. Child Development, 61, 1453–1460.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, W. R. (1989). Analyzing tables of statistical tests. Evolution, 43, 223–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosnow, R. L., & Rosenthal, R. (1988). Focused tests of significance and effect size estimation in counseling psychology. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35, 203–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosnow, R. L., & Rosenthal, R. (1989). Definition and interpretation of interaction effects. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 143–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., Lurye, L. E., & Zosuls, K. M. (2010). Pink frilly dresses and early gender identity. Princeton Report on Knowledge, 2(2). Retrieved from http://www.princeton.edu/prok/issues/2-2/pink_frilly.xml.

  • Ruble, D. N., Taylor, L. J., Cyphers, L., Greulich, F. K., Lurye, L. E., & Shrout, P. E. (2007). Effect of gender constancy on age-related changes in gender beliefs. Child Development, 78, 1121–1136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schloss, K. B., Poggesi, R. M., & Palmer, S. E. (2011). Effects of university affiliation and “school spirit” on color preferences: Berkeley vs. Stanford. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 498–504.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Serbin, L. A., Connor, J. M., & Iler, I. (1979). Sex-stereotyped and nonstereotyped introductions of new toys in the preschool classroom: An observational study of teacher behavior and its effects. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 4, 261–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Serbin, L. A., Poulin-Dubois, D., Colburne, K. A., Sen, M. G., & Eichstedt, J. A. (2001). Gender stereotyping in infancy: Visual preferences for and knowledge of gender-stereotyped toys in the second year. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 7–15.

  • Servin, A., Bohlin, G., & Berlin, L. (1999). Sex differences in 1-, 3-, and 5-year-olds’ toy- choice in a structured play-session. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40, 43–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, M. E., Jacklin, C. N., & Maccoby, E. E. (1983). Sex-of-child differences in father-child interaction at one year of age. Child Development, 54, 227–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprafkin, C., Serbin, L. A., Denier, C., & Connor, J. M. (1983). Sex-differentiated play: Cognitive consequences and early interventions. In M. B. Liss (Ed.), Social and cognitive skills: Sex roles and child’s play (pp. 167–192). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vander Stoep, S. W., & Johnson, D. D. (2009). Research methods for everyday life: Blending qualitative and quantitative approaches. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zosuls, K. M., Ruble, D. N., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shrout, P. E., Bornstein, M. H., & Greulich, F. K. (2009). The acquisition of gender labels in infancy: Implications for gender-typed play. Developmental Psychology, 45, 688–701.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Gates Cambridge Trust. Data were presented at the 2012 annual meeting of the International Academy of Sex Research in Lisbon, Portugal. We would like to thank the parents, children, children’s centers and nurseries who contributed data to the research, and Mihaela Constantinescu who helped with data coding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wang I. Wong.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wong, W.I., Hines, M. Effects of Gender Color-Coding on Toddlers’ Gender-Typical Toy Play. Arch Sex Behav 44, 1233–1242 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0400-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0400-5

Keywords

Navigation