Introduction

During recent decades, enrolment in higher education has risen strongly in all industrialized countries (OECD, 2019: 50). This is also the case in Germany, where the share of the respective age cohort with a higher education entrance certificate increased from 26% in 1995 to 54% in 2017 (Statistisches Bundesamt, 2018). Despite this ongoing expansion in higher education, however, many students do not receive a degree in the major they originally enrolled in (OECD, 2019: 208). Estimates for Germany for 2010, for example, indicate that about 39% of university students and about 21% of students at universities of applied sciences either switched majors or dropped out from higher education altogether (Heublein et al., 2012). Within this group, up to 30% of all students switched from the major they had initially chosen (Heublein et al., 2008).

Previous research on student retention, however, has focused either exclusively on student dropout or jointly on dropout and switching majors, both in Germany (e.g. Blüthmann et al., 2008; Fellenberg & Hannover, 2006; Heublein, 2014; Heublein et al., 2012, 2017; Ishphording & Wozny, 2018) and elsewhere (e.g. Belloc et al., 2011 for Italy; Hovdhaugen, 2009 for Norway; Robinson, 2004 for Australia). In contrast, very few studies exclusively address switching majors. These studies mostly do not examine switching majors in general (as exceptions, see Astorne-Figari & Speer, 2019 for the US; Diem, 2016 for Switzerland), but either focus on particular groups of subjects (King, 2015; Ost, 2010), consider specific groups of students (Riegle-Crumb et al., 2016; Tieben, 2020), or examine single higher education institutions (Belloc et al., 2011; Bornmann & Daniel, 1999). As a consequence, most of these studies have theoretical explanations tailored to their specific research topic but do not develop a theoretical framework applicable to switching majors in general. Moreover, representative studies on the higher education system as a whole are largely missing, especially regarding the German case. This paper, therefore, investigates possible factors that may explain why students enrolled in a German higher education institution switch their initially chosen major.

Germany is an interesting case to examine on this topic since switching majors is institutionally more restricted than in more open higher education systems, such as the US (Wright, 2018). In German higher education, students enrol in a specific major or combination of majors and thus have to reapply again when switching their major. Moreover, there are restricted admissions for a lot of majors at German higher education institutions, which are, for example, based on specific grades obtained in the higher education entry qualification (Finger et al., 2018). Therefore, students have to comply with these admission criteria when selecting a new major. These restrictions are very similar to those in many Asian countries, Chile or some other European countries like Spain and Turkey (Bordon & Fu, 2015), but very different to open major choice systems, such as the US (Bordon & Fu, 2015), which allows students to start with an undeclared major and to easily switch their major during studies (Bridet & Leighton, 2015; Wright, 2018).

Accordingly, switching majors in Germany is likely to have negative consequences for students, such as extending their period of study, a probable loss of human capital investment, and an increased risk of dropout (Wolter et al., 2014). It might also entail macroeconomic costs, especially if higher education is mainly state financed (as is the case in Germany), and thus switching majors may lead to a loss of public investment (Diem, 2016). At the same time, switching majors might have positive consequences, since it gives students the opportunity to revise an unsuitable initial subject choice (Bornmann & Daniel, 1999; Diem, 2016). These positive consequences have been shown for open major systems such as the US, and indicate that students switching their major even show higher graduation rates than students persisting in their initially chosen major (Liu et al., 2021; Yue & Fu, 2017). For higher education systems where changing majors involves a reapplication there is, however, not yet any evidence on how these factors drive such decisions.

The consequences of switching majors may depend on the degree and timing of such switches, which is why it is important to differentiate between different types of major changes. Regarding the degree of switching, we can differentiate between “low-cost” and “high-cost” major switches (Arcidiacono, 2004). If students switch their major within broader categories of academic disciplines (for example from sociology to political sciences in the broader field of social sciences), this might involve less effort and fewer costs, since course requirements are similar to those of the initial major and achieved credit points can be transferred relatively easily to the new course of study. In contrast, if students completely revise their initial subject choice and switch between broader categories of academic disciplines (for example from physics to history), the loss of human capital investment is likely to be larger. This is in line with assumptions of Astorne-Figari and Speer (2019), who first examined the distance of major switches in order to distinguish between major changes and only smaller corrections of major choice.

The timing of switching refers to switches taking place at an early or late stage of higher education studies. In this regard, studies from the US indicate that early major switches have no negative impact on students’ success in higher education (Foraker, 2012; Liu et al., 2021), while switches taking place after the second year are correlated with lower grades and graduation rates, but also with longer times until graduation (Foraker, 2012). In Germany, the negative consequences of late switches might be aggravated due to the system of state subsidies for students of low-income families, since these are only paid if switching majors take place during the first three terms.Footnote 1 We thus contribute to the existing literature by systematically differentiating between the degree and timing of switching majors, as this latter aspect in particular has not been a focus of previous research.

In the following section (Sect. “Previous Research”), we briefly summarize the state of previous research on switching majors before presenting our theoretical considerations and hypotheses (Sect. “Theoretical Considerations”). These are examined empirically based on data from the student cohort of the German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS SC5), a nationwide representative study following first-year students during the first six years of their higher education trajectory. We describe the dataset, operationalizations, and methods applied in Sect. “Data, Operationalizations, and Methods” before presenting our results from logistic regression models in Sect. “Results”. The article concludes with a discussion on the empirical results and implications for further research (Sect. "Conclusion").

Previous Research

Very few prior studies have exclusively addressed the topic of switching majors in higher education. These studies mostly do not examine switching majors in general (for exceptions, see Astorne-Figari & Speer, 2019 for the US; Diem, 2016 for Switzerland), but either focus on particular groups of subjects, student groups, or higher education institutions. Two studies from the US examine switching majors from particular subject groups: King (2015) examines switching majors from STEM fields in comparison to other subjects and Ost (2010) looks at switching majors in life and physical sciences. Regarding student groups, Riegle-Crumb et al. (2016) investigate the switching of majors by US students in gender-atypical subjects, while Tieben (2020) examines the switching of majors by non-traditional students in Germany, who graduated from vocational training before entering higher education. Focusing on single higher education institutions, Belloc et al. (2011) examine switching majors in Italy and Bornmann and Daniel (1999) do so for Germany. So far, no representative study on switching majors exists for the German system of higher education, which is the focus of the present paper.

Generally, it is important to note that the definition of what is considered a major switch varies strongly between different studies. While some studies investigate switches between different higher education institutions (Hovdhaugen, 2009; Ishitani & Flood, 2018), others identify switching majors as a potential pathway to graduation (Robinson, 2004) or examine switches to other departments within the same higher education institution (Belloc et al., 2011; Bornmann & Daniel, 1999). Others investigate subject persistence rather than switching majors (Ost, 2010) or they investigate the degree of switching by considering aspects of the original major and the one the student has switched to (Astorne-Figari & Speer, 2019; Riegle-Crumb et al., 2016). In this study, we examine switching majors irrespective of whether the switch takes place within or between higher education institutions. Rather, we consider two different aspects related to switching majors: the degree and the timing of change. Regarding the degree of switching, we compare students switching their major within or across broad academic disciplines. The timing of switching differentiates between switches taking place at an early or at a late stage of the course of study.

As regards explanations for switching majors, three broad strands can be identified in the literature: aspects of individual achievement, the person-major fit, and social expectations of parents and peers. First, previous studies indicate that achievements in secondary schooling and in higher education seem to matter when switching majors. Students with better grades in high school, with a better performance in the course of higher education, or who are actively involved in their studies and show higher effort have a lower probability of switching faculty or their initial major (Astorne-Figari & Speer, 2019; Belloc et al., 2011; Hovdhaugen, 2009; King, 2015; Ost, 2010). We will therefore consider student achievement in high school as an explanatory factor. In particular, we consider the final grade of students’ higher education entry examination, which captures several aspects that are important for academic success, not only in terms of cognitive performance, but also as regards study skills and goals (e.g. Schlücker & Schindler, 2019).

Second, if the characteristics of the first and newly chosen major are considered, it seems that students generally seek to achieve a better person-major fit when switching their major. Astorne-Figari and Speer, (2019), for example, consider the gender and racial compositions of majors as well as their competitiveness and course requirements. They find that, in general, students switch to majors with course requirements that are a better match for their abilities and their own characteristics in terms of race and gender (see also Diem, 2016 for Switzerland). Moreover, women are more likely to leave more competitive majors than men (Astorne-Figari & Speer, 2019). Since individuals seek to find the best possible match between their own occupational interests and their environment (Holland, 1997), we will consider this further aspect of person-major fit as an explanatory point of reference. More specifically we investigate whether the possible (mis)match between students’ occupational interests and the content taught in their initial major explains switching.

Finally, significant others, especially parents and peers, play an important role when switching majors. First, the parental judgement of the subject choice seems to matter. For example, young women are more likely to switch their major should their parents disagree with their career aspirations (Li & Kerpelman, 2007). Furthermore, the social and academic integration with fellow students has a lasting impact on persisting within a major. Students who are better socially integrated in their degree courses are less likely to switch majors (Ishitani & Flood, 2018). In terms of academic integration, low-performing students are more likely to persist if they are surrounded by high-achieving peers (Ost, 2010).Footnote 2 Since the data used in this study do not allow us to examine the effect of social integration or the achievement of peers directly, we measure both, parental and peer influences by their (dis-)agreement with students’ original subject choice and how this affects students’ decision to switch majors.

In sum, we assume individual achievement, the (mis)match between students’ occupational interests and the content taught in their initial major, and the expectations of students’ parents and peers to have an impact on the switching of majors in higher education. Since none of the above-mentioned studies considers all of these aspects, we take them into account simultaneously when analysing switching majors. In addition, sociodemographic characteristics have been shown to matter, in particular gender, migration and parental education background, and having finished vocational training before studying. The results of previous research indicate that women (Astorne-Figari & Speer, 2018; Belloc et al., 2011) and students from low-income families (Belloc et al., 2011; Ishitani & Flood, 2018) have an increased risk of switching major, while students that graduated from vocational training before entering higher education (Tieben, 2020) are less likely to do so. Results on the effect of race/ethnicity are mixed, since a study on the US indicates that Black and Hispanic students are more likely to switch their major (Astorne-Figari & Speer, 2018), while other studies on the US (Ishitani & Flood, 2018), Italy (Belloc et al., 2011) and Switzerland (Diem, 2016) find that immigrant or foreign students switch less often. Since none of these studies offer a theoretical explanation for these results, we decided to take those sociodemographic aspects as control variables into account.

Theoretical Considerations

Since there are only few studies dealing with switching majors in higher education, we consider and apply the theoretical perspectives and existing research on two related topics to the phenomenon of switching majors: research on (initial) subject choice, and studies on higher education dropout. Theoretically, students may decide to switch majors because of bad study conditions and their dissatisfaction with aspects of their originally chosen major. Moreover, students compare the different characteristics of the initial major to what they expect to be the characteristics of their second choice of subject. In this paper, we focus on explanatory factors relating to the originally chosen major to compare students switching majors with students who are still enrolled in or who graduated from their originally chosen major.Footnote 3

To conceptualise how aspects of individual performance, the person-major fit, and perceived social expectations of parents and peers might affect switching majors, we apply a sociological rational choice frameworkFootnote 4 that has been developed for explaining educational choices (Erikson & Jonsson, 1996; Jonsson, 1999) and that has been applied to both subject choices and student dropout. From this perspective, when deciding whether to stay or to switch, students rationally consider their probabilities of success in the initial major and weigh the anticipated benefits and costs of staying versus leaving.Footnote 5 We will argue in the following that a student’s school achievement is one possible dimension affecting the probability of mastering the performance requirements of the initial major successfully. A good match between individual occupational interests and the interests met by particular subjects is considered a possible benefit of staying in the initially chosen subject. Finally, meeting parental and peer expectations are possible dimensions to lower the social costs when choosing a particular subject. Switching majors can thus be considered as revising the choice of the original field of study, which was made under incomplete information (Altonji et al., 2012). In the following, we elaborate these expectations in more detail and discuss whether the assumed mechanisms should have a stronger impact on students switching majors across or within subject groups (degree of switching) or at an early or late stage of their study (timing of switching).

Switching majors should be affected by a student’s expected probabilities of successfully graduating from the initially chosen major. From the perspective of rational utility maximization, students assess their school achievements in a particular domain both objectively through grades and by subjective evaluations and choose a major they assume they can successfully master, which thus offers the highest probability of success (Becker et al., 2010). In doing so, they consider their strengths in single subjects, such as mathematics or science, but also compare their relative abilities across domains (Jonsson, 1999). Previous research on initial subject choices has demonstrated that individuals use their objective schooling achievements in different domains to assess the probability of succeeding in particular subject groups (Becker et al., 2010; Hägglund & Lörz, 2020; Mann & DiPrete, 2016). Moreover, subjective evaluation of strengths and weaknesses in particular domains seems to matter for the initial subject choice (Hägglund & Lörz, 2020; Mann & DiPrete, 2013; Werfhorst et al., 2003). Finally, a large number of studies show that high achievement in secondary school and in higher education decreases the risk of dropping out from higher education (Isleib et al., 2019; Ishphording & Wozny, 2018; Robbins et al., 2004; Wolter et al., 2014).

If we apply this perspective to switching majors, we can derive two competing hypotheses. Pupils at the end of secondary school do not have precise information on the performance requirements of particular subjects and whether they will be able to meet them. Only after entering higher education in a particular subject can students have a better idea of what type of exams they have to take and how well they can perform in comparison to their fellow students. These experiences might change their initial evaluation of the probability of success. An objective evaluation in the sense of school grades might serve as a signal of how well performance requirements can be met. In the case of low final school grades, students not only have a higher probability of dropping out completely from higher education but also have a higher probability of switching majors (Astorne-Figari & Speer, 2019; Belloc et al., 2011; Hovdhaugen, 2009), possibly to subjects with lower performance requirements (Astorne-Figari & Speer, 2019). Therefore, we expect that lower achievement in secondary education increases students’ probability of switching majors (H1a).

However, due to the particular situation in the German higher education system, many subjects have a restricted admission based on students’ average achievements in secondary education (Finger et al., 2018). As a consequence, students with lower final average grades in secondary education have a smaller range of potential subjects in higher education to choose from—both at the moment of the initial decision and at the moment of revising this decision. In contrast, students with better average final grades have more options to choose from when intending to revise their initial subject choice. Accordingly, their evaluation of the criteria for success in a different major should be higher than for students with lower final grades. Therefore,—as a counter hypothesis to hypothesis 1a—we expect that higher achievement in secondary education increases a student’s probability of switching majors (H1b).

Regarding the degree and timing of switching majors, we assume that school achievements should matter more strongly for switches across rather than within broad disciplines and early in their studies. In both cases, students will already know whether they meet specific course requirements during the first two semesters. Since course requirements may be rather similar within broad subject groups, school achievements should matter in particular for switching majors across disciplines and switches taking place in the first two semesters.

The expected benefits of a subject comprise its suitability for life and career plans (Jonsson, 1999), but also the value young adults attach to specific occupational contents (Eccles, 1994). Building on Holland’s theory of vocational choices (1997), we assume that individuals seek to find the best possible match between their own occupational interestsFootnote 6 and their environment, i.e. the content taught in the particular major they are enrolled in. According to Holland (1997), individuals seek to achieve a match between their occupational interests and different occupational groups by utilizing occupational stereotypes and public discourses. This implies that members of an occupation or subject display a similar interest structure (Holland, 1997), but also that the content of studies matches some interest structures better than others (Nagy, 2007). Accordingly, a good match between individual occupational interests and the interests met by particular subjects can serve as one possible benefit of staying in the initially chosen subject.

Previous research shows that Holland’s occupational interests are a strong predictor of students’ major choices, both in Germany (Hägglund & Lörz, 2020; Nagy, 2007; Ochsenfeld, 2016) and elsewhere (Porter & Umbach, 2006). In the context of Germany, Nagy (2007) was the first to apply this concept to the transition from secondary to higher education and to demonstrate its importance for subject choices. Only a handful of studies investigate the role of occupational interests on student dropout and show that a good match between students’ occupational interests and the content of the subject enrolled in decreases the dropout risk (Allen & Robbins, 2008, 2010 for the US; Nagy, 2007 for Germany).

If we apply this perspective to switching majors, we can assume that pupils at the end of secondary school do not have precise information on the content of particular fields of study. Rather, they derive their knowledge from stereotypes about specific subjects and make their initial subject choices in higher education according to them. Only after entering higher education are they able to evaluate whether their individual occupational interests are actually met by the content taught in the respective major. Accordingly, we expect that students are more likely to switch majors if their individual interests do not match the dominant content taught in their initially chosen major (H2).

We again assume that a mismatch between individual occupational interests and the content of a particular subject should matter more strongly for switches across broad disciplines and for early switches. Regarding the degree of switches, students are more likely to find a better match if they decide to switch to a major that is not related to the one they originally chose and therefore belongs to a different subject group. Moreover, the longer students stay in their originally chosen major, the better they might accommodate their individual interests to the content of the subjects, thus lowering the likelihood of possible mismatches. As a consequence, mismatches between occupational interests and the content taught within the originally chosen major should increase the likelihood of switching majors, particularly during the first two semesters.

Finally, perceived costs should matter for explaining switches between majors. Theoretically, these costs might arise in the form of both monetaryFootnote 7 and social costs (Erikson & Jonsson, 1996; Jonsson, 1999). We focus here on the aspect of social costs deriving from significant others, such as parents and peers. In this regard, studies on initial subject choices point toward the influence of parents’ socioeconomic background and parental expectations (Lörz et al., 2011; Raabe et al., 2019; Werfhorst et al., 2001). Regarding peer influence, students in general adjust their subject preferences to those of their friends (Raabe et al., 2019). Moreover, good social integration with peers and faculty members (e.g. Ishphording & Wozny, 2018; Klein, 2019; Meyer & Strauß, 2019) or a perceived positive study culture (Severiens & Dam, 2012) reduce a student’s dropout risk.

When applying this perspective to switching majors, a student’s initial subject choice often follows the expectations of their parents, either in terms of the subject’s gender composition or in terms of the social status achieved by graduating in a specific subject (Lörz et al., 2011; Werfhorst et al., 2001). Yet if students do not choose a major in accordance with parental expectations, social costs might arise in the form of parental disagreement or low parental support, which may also include a lower level of financial support. This in turn might increase the likelihood of students switching majors in a direction more in line with parental expectations. In a similar vein, peers are assumed to affect social costs related to initial subject choices by supporting (or not supporting) the choices made. According to Raabe et al. (2019), young adults value their friends’ opinions and discuss their preferences for subjects. If peers disapprove of a student’s initial subject choice they might sanction such nonconforming behavior. Accordingly, we expect that students are more likely to switch majors if their initial subject choice does not conform to parental or peer expectations (H3).

These cost considerations should again matter more strongly for switches across broad subject categories and for early switches. Regarding the degree of switching, switching majors within broad categories might not suffice to regain parental or peer support if parents and peers strongly disagree with the initial subject choice. Moreover, students are likely to be more strongly focused on the approval of their parents and former school friends at the beginning of their studies. At a later stage, they are already socialized into their field of study and social integration in higher education should matter more strongly when deciding whether to leave their study programme. Thus, social costs should be particularly relevant for switches across broad subject categories and at an early stage of higher education studies.

To sum up, we expect the different aspects of individual performance, the person-major fit, and perceived social expectations of parents and peers to predominantly affect substantial switches taking place across broad subject groups and switches taking place at an early stage of the course of study.

Data, Operationalizations, and Methods

To test our hypotheses empirically, we use the German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS) (Blossfeld et al., 2011),Footnote 8 which follows the educational pathways of six different age cohorts from early childhood (Starting Cohort 1) to adulthood (Starting Cohort 6). The analysis is based on Starting Cohort 5, a cohort of first-year undergraduate students who enrolled for the first time in a German higher education institution in the winter term 2010/11 (FDZ-LIfBi, 2018). The survey started with a sample containing 17,910 students, with an oversampling of students in teacher education and those attending private higher education institutions (FDZ-LifBi, 2018). Our analyses include the first 11 waves taking place between the winter term 2010/11 and the summer term 2016, which means following first-year students for up to 6 years or 12 semesters during their higher education careers. As a consequence, most students finished at least their first degree during the period of observation.

Of these first-year students, 5704 left and did not return to higher education during the period of observation. These (temporary) dropouts were excluded from the analyses, since the data do not allow differentiating between dropping out temporarily or dropping out permanently (Ishphording & Wozny, 2018) and they are strongly biased because of high panel attrition rates. The sample of the remaining 12,206 students was further reduced due to missing values on important variables,Footnote 9 which were excluded by listwise deletion and resulted in an analytical sample of 9006 students in total. To make sure that our independent variables precede our dependent variable, all independent variables used in the models were measured in the first wave, i.e. the first semester in higher education. To deal with the problem of sample selection bias and panel attrition, we applied weights that take into account the sampling design and panel mortality (Zinn et al., 2017).

The dependent variable switching major is based on students’ self-reported information that they have switched majors since starting their study or since participating in the last survey wave.Footnote 10 Majors were measured using the classification of the German Federal Statistical Office (Statistisches Bundesamt, 2012: 443ff.), which means we are able to distinguish between 78 fine and nine broad subject categories. On the basis of these subject categories we are able to measure switching majors in two different ways. First, we define major switching as a transfer from the initially chosen major to any new major based on the 78 subject categories. Second, we differentiate between students switching majors within the same broad study group and those switching to another broad study group. To do so, we collapse the 78 subjects into nine broader study groups (for the assignment of the single subjects to the broad study groups, see Table 2 in the appendix) and identify whether students switched majors either within or across the broad categories. Since the information on switching majors was measured from Wave 2 up to Wave 11 in every second survey, we are also able to identify the timing of switching majors. We differentiate between students switching majors at an early stage of their study (in the first or second semester)Footnote 11 and those deciding to do so at a later stage (third semester or later). Since we have no information about the actual higher education institutions students are enrolled in, a major switch can take place within the same institution or after switching between institutions.

In our first model we compare students who switched majors in general (1, N = 1247) with students still enrolled in or having graduated from their originally chosen major (0, N = 7759). In our second and third model we compare students who switched majors across (N = 850) or within (N = 397) broad subject groups (1) with students still enrolled in or having graduated from their originally chosen major (0). In our fourth and fifth model we compare students switching majors in the first or second semester (N = 655) or in the third semester or later (N = 592) (1) with those still enrolled in or having graduated from their originally chosen major (0). Overall, 13.8% of the students switched majors during the six years following their initial enrolment in higher education, which is slightly higher than the estimated figures of Heublein et al., (2012: 46–50). Of those who switched their major, 47% did so within their first two semesters and about two-thirds of them switched to another broad subject group. Students enrolled in larger subject groups in particular (such as linguistics and cultural studies) often switch majors within the same subject group (see Table 3 in the appendix).

Our first independent variable, student’s achievement in secondary education, is measured by the average final grade of the higher education entry certificate in upper secondary education, which ranges in Germany from 1.0 (best mark) to 4.0 (lowest mark).Footnote 12 To facilitate interpretation of this variable, we rescaled the variable: higher values now point toward a better performance in secondary education.

Person–major fit is operationalized by a possible (mis)match between individual occupational interests and the content taught in the initially chosen major. Individual occupational interests are measured with the “Interest Inventory Life-Span” for adults (IILS-II), based on the instrument developed by Bergmann and Eder (1999), containing three items per interest domain. The resulting six indices range from 1 to 5 and measure a student’s realistic, investigative, artistic, social, enterprising, and conventional interests at the beginning of their higher education studies. To operationalize a possible (mis)match with the content of subjects, we compare those individual interests to the occupational interest domain most relevant in each subject category, as identified by Nagy (2007) and Bergmann and Eder (1999) (see Table 4 in the appendix). The derived variable includes three categories indicating whether a student’s occupational interest in the most relevant domain per subject is at a high, average, or low level. Since means and standard deviations vary strongly across interest domains, we define the interest level to be low if a student’s interest has a value below the 50th percentile in the sample, average for values between the 50th and 75th percentiles, and high for values above the 75th percentile (see Table 5 in the appendix).Footnote 13 The resulting variable comprises three categories, representing a bad (0, ref. category), good (1), or very good (2) match between students’ individual occupational interests and the content of their major.

Finally, perceived parental and peer expectations are operationalized with parents’ and peers’ (dis)agreement with students’ initial subject choice. For doing so, students were asked whether their parents or friends approved of their subject choice, with answers ranging from “1” (applies completely) to “5” (does not apply at all). Thus, higher values indicate stronger disapproval on the part of parents and peers regarding a student’s initial subject choice.

We further control for the following factors that have been shown to affect switching majors as well as our main explanatory variables (see Sect. “Previous Research”): student’s gender (1 = women, 0 = men), migration background (1 = up to the third generation, 0 = no migration background),Footnote 14parental education background (1 = at least one parent with tertiary education (ISCED-97 5a, 6), 0 = no parent with tertiary education), and previous vocational training qualification (1 = yes, 0 = no). Moreover, we control for the two types of higher education institutions existing in Germany (1 = universities, 0 = universities of applied sciences), since students drop out more often from universities (e.g. Isleib et al., 2019), which might also be the case for major changes. Finally, we consider whether students have enrolled in the subject of their choice (1 = yes, 0 = no), which has been shown to reduce students’ dropout risk (e.g. Ishphording & Wozny, 2018) and could thus also explain why students switch their major. A descriptive overview of all variables can be found in Table 1.Footnote 15

Due to the binary response variable for switching majors, we estimate logistic regression models. As described above, we use five different dependent variables, resulting in five different models: (1) switching majors in general; (2) switching majors across broad subject categories; (3) switching majors within broad subject categories; (4) switching majors at an early stage during the first two semesters; and (5) switching majors at a later stage (see Table 6 in the appendix for the full models). In each of these models, students switching majors are compared to students still enrolled in or having graduated from their initially chosen major, which results in different sizes of the analytic sample depending on the dependent variable. Coefficients are presented as average marginal effects (AMEs), which can be interpreted as percentage point differences in the probability of switching majors for a one-unit change of the predictors (Mood, 2010). To take different change rates per subject category and further unobserved subject characteristics into account, we include a dummy variable for each category of the fine subject classification (subject fixed effects) in all models.

Results

How do school grades, the fit between personal occupational interests and the content that is taught in the initially chosen field of subject, and parental and peer expectations affect switching majors? To answer these questions empirically, we first examine the distribution of the explanatory variables for the total sample and then separately for the four groups of switches (across/within broad categories, early/late switches) (Table 1).

Table 1 Description of variables (proportions and means)

For the final grades in upper secondary education we cannot observe considerable differences between students who switched majors and those who did not, nor can we observe considerable differences between the several groups of students switching majors (Table 1). The distribution of the individual occupational interests matching those most relevant for the originally chosen major indicates that 51.3% of the student population achieves a very good match (values above the 75th percentile). This implies that about half of the first-semester students chose majors in higher education that corresponded with their individual interests. However, we observe differences between the groups: students switching majors showed worse matches between their occupational interests and the content of their major (25.7%) than those staying in their initial major (19.8%). Among students switching majors, especially those switching at an early stage of study and those switching across subject groups, report bad matches between their individual interests and the one domain mostly taught in their initial major. Finally, parents and peers in general largely approve of the majors students have initially chosen (mean value of 1.8), although peer judgements are a bit more negative than those of parents (mean value of 2.2). Again, students changing their subject and especially those changing across broad subject categories and at an early stage of their studies receive lower levels of support from both parents and peers.

As a next step, we estimated logistic regression models on the five different dependent variables. Figure 1 presents the results on switching majors as a transfer to another major among any of the 78 subject categories, irrespective of whether the change took place within or across broad subject groups or at an early or late stage. In Fig. 2, we take the degree of switching majors into account and differentiate between switching majors across and within broad subject groups. Finally, Fig. 3 presents the models, distinguishing between those students switching majors at an early stage of study and those doing so at a later stage. Coefficients are displayed as average marginal effects (AMEs) with 95% confidence intervals. All models contain control variables (gender, migrant and educational background, vocational training, higher education institution, and subject of choice).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Data source: National Educational Panel Study (NEPS), Starting Cohort First-Year Students, https://doi.org/10.5157/NEPS:SC5:11.0.0, weighted estimates, N = 9006 (7759 non-switchers and 1247 subject-switchers)

Results from logistic regression: switching majors in general (AMEs with 95% confidence intervals).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Data source: National Educational Panel Study (NEPS), Starting Cohort First-Year Students, https://doi.org/10.5157/NEPS:SC5:11.0.0, weighted estimates, n = 8609 (7759 non-switchers, 850 subject-switchers across subject groups); n = 8156 (7,759 non-switchers, 397 subject-switchers within subject groups)

Results from logistic regression: switching majors within and across subject groups (AMEs with 95% confidence intervals).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Data source: National Educational Panel Study (NEPS), Starting Cohort First-Year Students, https://doi.org/10.5157/NEPS:SC5:11.0.0, weighted estimates, n = 8351 (7759 non-switchers, 592 subject-switchers at an early stage); n = 8414 (7759 non-switchers, 655 subject-switchers at a late stage)

Results from logistic regression: switching majors at an early or at a late stage (AMEs with 95% confidence intervals).

As we expected from Hypothesis 1b, better final grades in upper secondary education do increase the probability of switching majors (Fig. 1). This effect is even higher when students switch across broad subject groups (Fig. 2) and, other than expected, when students switch their major at a late stage of their study (Fig. 3). These results are in contrast with our Hypothesis 1a and previous findings for other countries (Astorne-Figari & Speer, 2019; Belloc et al., 2011; Hovdhaugen, 2009), according to which better academic performance lowers the probability of switching majors. On the one hand, we interpret this finding against the background of the German higher education system, where access to higher education often depends on the grades of the higher education entry qualification. As a consequence, students with higher achievement in secondary education have more options to choose from and thus better probabilities of success in a large variety of subjects (even at later points during their studies). On the other hand, this result might also stem from our operationalization, since we do not measure performance in higher education (as studies in other country contexts did). Accordingly, students with lower achievement in secondary education might perform well in their initially chosen subject and are thus less likely to switch their major.

Regarding the occupational interests of students, one can see that a (very) good match with the content taught in their originally chosen major is related to a decreased risk of switching majors. While the effect of this variable is not significant for subject changes in general (p < 0.05; Fig. 1), it increases and turns significant for subject changes across broad subject groups (Fig. 2). In contrast, (mis)matching occupational interest does not seem to matter for subject changes within broad disciplines and for the timing of switching majors (Fig. 3). Overall, these results indicate that a (very) good match between students’ occupational interests and the content taught in their major increases the chances of staying with their initially chosen subject, thus lowering the probability of switching majors. We can therefore confirm Hypothesis 2 for substantial switching of majors across broad subject groups.

Regarding social costs, we expected negative parental and peer judgements about the initially chosen subject to increase the probability of switching majors (Hypothesis 3). Our models support this hypothesis, since students show a higher probability of switching majors the more their parents and peers disapprove of their initial subject choice (Fig. 1). This is true for both changes within and across subject groups (Fig. 2) but stronger effects are found for changes across academic disciplines, which implies that the possible sanctions of such nonconforming behavior might be stronger than for subject changes within subject groups. Moreover, results confirm that parental and peer disapproval of the originally chosen major is particularly important for changing majors at an early stage of higher education (Fig. 3). At a later stage, parental and peer evaluations might lose their importance due to social networks formed within higher education itself, which are not captured by the variables. The results of the stepwise inclusion of variables (see Table 7 in the Online appendix) indicate that models on parental and peer expectations exhibit better model fits, which implies that these factors are more important for understanding major switches than individual achievements or person–major fits.

The results of the control variables (see Table 6 in the appendix) indicate that women have a higher probability of switching majors than men do, which supports the results of Astorne-Figari and Speer (2018) for the US and Belloc et al. (2011) for Italy. Having completed vocational training before studying and a migrant background decrease the risk of switching majors, which is also in line with previous research (Belloc et al., 2011; Ishitani & Flood, 2018; Tieben, 2020). Interestingly, first-generation students (i.e. neither parent holds a tertiary education degree) also have a lower probability of switching majors, especially across subjects and at later stages, which contrasts with previous findings for the US (Ishitani & Flood, 2018) and Italy (Belloc et al., 2011). This might either be due to different measures of parental background, since both other studies use parental income instead of education. However, the result might also be specific to the German context, since state subsidies for students of low-income families are only paid if switching majors takes place during the first three terms, which makes especially later switches costlier. Finally, students at universities have a higher probability of switching majors than students studying at a university of applied sciences, while students enrolled in their preferred major are less likely to do so, which supports findings on students’ dropout (Ishphording & Wozny, 2018; Isleib et al., 2019).

Conclusion

In this article, we examined how aspects of individual achievement, person–major fit, and parental and peer expectations affect students switching majors in German higher education and whether these effects vary between the degree and timing of such switches. Theoretically, we applied a rational choice approach according to which students consider their probabilities of success in the initial major and weigh the anticipated benefits and costs of staying versus leaving. Thus, we perceive switching majors as a revision of the initial subject choice, which was made under incomplete information. Empirically, however, we were not able to measure the broad concepts of probability of success, benefits, and costs. Rather, the data used allowed us only to consider certain aspects of these concepts: student’s school achievement to affect the probability of success, a good match between individual occupational interests and the interests met by particular subjects as a possible benefit, and parental and peer approval of the initial subject as lowering social costs.

Overall, the empirical analyses based on a representative sample of German students starting higher education in 2010/11 support our theoretical considerations. First, students’ individual performance evaluations in the form of the final grades received in upper secondary education matter in this regard. In contrast to previous studies in different country contexts (Astorne-Figari & Speer, 2019; Belloc et al., 2011; Hovdhaugen, 2009), our results indicate that better academic performance increases the probability of switching majors. This might be particularly the case in the German higher education system, where access to many subjects depends on a student’s achievements in secondary education. As a consequence, high-performing students have more options to choose from and thus have a better probability of success in a large variety of subjects, while students with lower grades have to stick to their initial choice. Since we were only able to measure performance in terms of high school grades, further research should establish whether performance in higher education produces similar results in the German context, or whether it is more in line with previous international research suggesting a higher inclination for students with lower performance to switch majors.

Regarding the person-major fit as an aspect of benefits, we investigated the (mis)match between individual occupational interests and the content of the subject initially chosen. A better match should increase the benefit of studying a particular subject, making switches less likely. We confirmed this assumption for switching majors across broad subject groups, but it did not matter for changes within subject groups; nor did it matter for the timing of switching major. This finding points toward the importance of differentiating between the degrees of change, as we did in our study. Subject characteristics may not only matter regarding the initial subject; they may also matter regarding the major students choose to switch to.

A limitation in this regard is the operationalization of the match between the occupational interests of the student and the contents taught in the field of study. We followed Bergmann and Eder (1999) and Nagy (2007), who identified the occupational interest which is the most relevant in each field of study. Since the substantive focus of a field of study can differ between institutions of higher education and can therefore cater to occupational interests at different levels, a precise measurement of the environment characteristics would have been a better indicator.

Finally, perceived parental and peer support of the initial subject choice as an aspect of the social environment lowers the risk of switching majors, especially when those switches happen at an early stage of study and correlate with a transfer across subject groups. This confirms earlier findings (Raabe et al., 2019) that young adults value their friends’ opinions and discuss their subject preferences and points toward the importance of costs relating to switching majors.

Due to our study design, we could only consider perceived parental and peer expectations at higher education entry, while social processes taking place in the course of higher education studies could not be investigated. Accordingly, future research should examine further social influences that might be useful for understanding later switches, such as social integration with fellow students or teaching personnel. These aspects have been shown to matter for student dropout (e.g. Klein, 2019), while the relevance for switching majors is yet to be established.

Overall, this study makes three important contributions to understanding the under-researched topic of switching majors in higher education. It is the first study that systematically integrates explanations relating to the individual, the subject, and the social level into one theoretical framework and that simultaneously examines their effect on switching majors. Results indicate that all of these aspects matter, with parental and peer judgement being slightly more important than individual achievement and person-major fit. But our findings highlight as well that results might be sensitive to the country context. Thus, in countries where subject choices are bound to institutional restrictions, such as in Germany (Finger et al., 2018), major switches might be driven by different factors than in countries with open major choice systems, such as the US (Bordon & Fu, 2015; Bridet & Leighton, 2015; Wright, 2018). The effect of individual achievements is one example of how explanatory factors might vary, which calls for more comparative research on this topic.

Second, our results demonstrate that it is important to differentiate between the degree and the timing of switching majors, since not all changes seem to be major changes (Astorne-Figari & Speer, 2019). Our research confirms that the phenomenon of switching majors is highly heterogeneous in the sense that it comprises minor adjustments within the same area but also major changes between different groups of study. Also, the timing of switching majors seems to matter in the sense that early decisions regarding changing majors seem to be driven by other factors than decisions for late changes. Mixing up such different phenomena is likely to lead to contradictory research results.

Finally, we can show that, although switching majors is driven partly by forces similar to those driving dropout rates, it is a distinct phenomenon in terms of explanatory factors and further research should treat it as such. Overall, we can conclude that the practice of switching majors (other than dropout from higher education) has not yet received enough research attention to understand this diverse phenomenon sufficiently. We hope, however, that this study contributes to increasing the understanding of this complex issue.

Appendix

See Tables 26

Table 2 Fine and broad subject groups
Table 3 Switching majors within or across subject groups
Table 4 Attribution of occupational interests to subjects
Table 5 Distribution of occupational interests
Table 6 Results from the logistic regression models (AMEs)