1 Introduction

Increasingly, scientists from different disciplines are studying how childhood conditions contribute to individual development regarding educational attainment, social behaviour, labour market prospects and health conditions. Both families and school play a vital role in this process by bolstering or harming children’s skills, which are predictors of later outcomes in several domains (Lundberg, 1993; Hill et al., 2001; Case et al., 2005; Leschied et al., 2008; Francesconi et al., 2010).

The effects of attending formal childcare on younger children have been thoroughly investigated, as have the effects of school quality on older children.Footnote 1 However, children are also exposed to other development opportunities. The hours between the end of school and bedtime are often filled with various activities that can promote different skills. These activities may be more or less structured (e.g. participating in team sports versus playing freely in the park), geared towards educational enrichment or social activities, and they can be performed alone, with other children (friends, siblings, cousins) or adults (parents, grandparents, instructors). Understanding the effects of such activities is important because they contribute to children’s development and may be possible sources of inequality among children from different socio-economic backgrounds. Notably, it has been shown that inequality in socio-emotional skills has increased in the UK over the past 30 years, with the socio-economic status of mothers being a significant contributing factor (Attanasio et al., 2020).

As part of the broader discussion of the importance of childhood conditions for an individual’s development, the key question central to our analysis is how time allocation influences children’s non-cognitive development. Particularly, we study the effects of time use on emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems and prosocial behaviour at ages 7 and 11, using data from the Millennium Cohort Study (MCS) (UK). We consider time spent outside regular school time: with parents, in extra-curricular activities (music, sports, extra classes) as well as less structured activities, such as going to the library, attending religious services, reading, watching TV, playing electronic games and performing small chores around the house, such as tidying up or caring for pets.

To study the effects of time allocation on children’s non-cognitive outcomes at ages 7 and 11, we apply a cumulative value-added model. Subsequently, we test the sensitivity of our results to possible endogeneity issues resulting from unobserved variables, reverse causality and measurement error due to the inclusion of past behavioural indicators in the model. To handle the omitted variable bias, we apply a method developed by Oster (2019) to produce bias-adjusted estimates and bind the coefficients of interest in the presence of such omitted variable bias. To address reverse causality, we leverage the panel dimension of the data, including only lagged activities. The bias due to the inclusion of lag outcomes is handled through an instrumental variable approach. Finally, we complement our analysis with a fixed-effects approach, which allows us to consider unobservable time-invariant characteristics but does not permit estimation of age-specific effects. The results show that time spent on sports, studying, reading, doing small chores around the house and time spent actively with parents are beneficial for the development of children’s socio-emotional skills, while video-screen time and extra hours spent at school are detrimental. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study showing that the allocation of children’s time is important not only for cognitive but also for non-cognitive development, in line with some of the results on parental inputs (Moroni et al., 2019) and with studies on the impact of individual activities (Hille & Schupp, 2015).

Although we explored possible heterogeneities in the effects of some child characteristics (socio-economic status, gender, nationality and family composition), we found no significant differences.

This study contributes to the literature in three specific ways: focusing on non-cognitive outcomes (rather than cognitive ones); studying the effects of several activities (rather than a single activity); and considering activities conducted not only with parents but also independently and with other children.

Focusing on non-cognitive development is crucial and forms the basis of this study. The literature documents that these skills are at least as important as cognitive ones not only for future educational and labour market outcomes but also risky adolescent behaviour and health-related outcomes (Heckman & Rubinstein, 2001; Cunha & Heckman, 2008; Prevoo & ter Weel, 2015; Attanasio et al., 2020). Non-cognitive skills also influence learning abilities and cognitive development (Almlund et al., 2011). The seminal work by Heckman & Rubinstein (2001) led empirical studies to investigate the impact of childhood conditions on non-cognitive development. Particularly, it has been shown that non-cognitive skills are improved by good parental investments such as income and material resources, cognitive stimulation, parental interpersonal skills, parenting style and breastfeeding (Cunha & Heckman, 2008; Borra et al., 2012; Cunha et al., 2013; Heckman & Kautz, 2013; Fletcher & Wolfe, 2016; Doyle et al., 2017; Moroni et al., 2019).

Studying the relationship between time allocation and non-cognitive development is important because non-cognitive skills can be highly sensitive to the impact of extra-curricular activities. Qualitative studies have suggested that since the emphasis at school is on academic attainment, out-of-school activities provide children who perform poorly at school with opportunities to feel capable, thus increasing their self-esteem and wellbeing (Callanan et al., 2016). It also allows children to make and interact with new friends, and this may have possible consequences on social outcomes. Therefore, understanding if there is a link between time allocation and non-cognitive development is highly relevant, as it could shape the way policy-makers and educational institutions intervene in designing the supply of such activities. Moreover, the increasing importance of extra-curricular activities may have different impacts according to socio-economic background, and thus can be a source of increased socio-emotional inequalities that call for intervention by policy-makers.

The second focus of this study is on the effects of several activities, whereas it is more common in the literature to find studies considering a single activity (e.g. reading, sport, music or computer and TV use).Footnote 2 A few studies used data from children’s time diaries to explore the full range of activities, such as the one by Hofferth & Sandberg (2001), Fiorini & Keane (2014) and Caetano et al. (2019). Hofferth & Sandberg (2001) used data from the 1997 US Child Development Supplement to the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID) (around 2000 children aged 0–12) and found that time devoted to learning activities such as reading is positively correlated with high school achievement, as is structured time spent playing sports or on social activities. Additionally, time spent eating meals with the family is associated with fewer behavioural problems. Fiorini & Keane (2014) used time use diaries from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (around 1300 children aged 4–9) to consider the overall impact of time use and to study the trade-off between the benefits of alternative activities. The result of their study is a ranking of activities from the most to the least beneficial: time spent on educational activities, particularly with parents, was the most productive for cognitive skill development. However, they found that non-cognitive skills are insensitive to alternative time allocations. Caetano et al. (2019) also used the time diaries from the Child Development Supplement of the 2002 and 2007 PSID to estimate the effect of family time inputs on cognitive skills, applying an exogeneity test developed by Caetano (2015).Footnote 3 Their study provided a different classification of activities from those in previous studies (and our own), aggregating them into active and passive time with different individuals. They reported that active time with an adult family member (parents or grandparents) induces an increase in cognitive skills.Footnote 4 Hofferth & Sandberg (2001), Fiorini & Keane (2014) and Caetano et al. (2019) represented the ideal benchmark for our study, but unluckily we do not have time use diaries at our disposal. This means that—despite considering a large set of different activities—we lack the data to evaluate the trade-off between them. The differences between our results and previous findings in the literature are discussed in the Results section.

The third contribution of this study is to consider the effects of time allocation beyond parental time on child development. Todd & Wolpin (2007) used data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth to estimate the effect of parental and school inputs on child cognitive abilities. Parental input was represented by an index that considers parental stimulation and involvement and the toys and learning materials available. The results show that parental inputs have positive effects on children’s cognitive development. The effect of parental time is also the focus of three studies using data from the Child Development Supplement to the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (US). Carneiro & Rodriguez (2009) found that children (especially ages 3–6) who spend more time with their mothers perform better on cognitive tests. Hsin & Felfe (2014) found that working mothers spend less time with their children, but only on unstructured activities, and what counts instead for child cognitive development and positive behaviour is the time spent engaging in educational activities together. Finally, Del Boca et al. (2017) considered the combined effect of spending time doing beneficial activities independently or together with their parents on children of different ages. They found that time spent independently on these activities in adolescence has a positive effect on cognitive outcomes, whereas time spent with the mother is more important for younger children.

Among the studies of parental time inputs, the most similar to this study regarding data and analysis techniques is Del Bono et al. (2016). Their study aimed to identify the effect of time spent with the mother (divided into recreational time and educational time) at the ages of 3, 5 and 7 on children’s cognitive skills and behavioural difficulties. The goal of our study is to investigate the effect of different uses of time, grouped into seven categories (among which is time spent with parents) on the non-cognitive development of older children aged 7 and 11, ages at which time with parents is reduced and time increases in other activities. Furthermore, non-cognitive skills are treated separately and prosocial behaviour is also considered.

This study is organised as follows: Section 2 describes the MCS, the selection of the sample and the variables used throughout the analyses. Section 3 presents the methods employed for the empirical analysis. Section 4 contains the results and the robustness checks. Section 5 presents the conclusions.

2 Data, sample selection and main variables of interest

The MCS is a longitudinal survey that tracks the lives of a sample of about 19,000 babies born in the UK in 2000/2001. The survey was conducted in different waves: we use information from surveys for cohort-children aged about 9 months, 3, 5, 7 and 11 years old. The dataset has two considerable advantages: first, many of the questions and child indicators are repeated over time; second, it provides ample information about the child and the child’s family from birth, which may provide important data to control for.

The initial wave one sample comprises 18,818 children, but around 10% of the sample is lost due to attrition at each new wave. We only consider children in families participating in the survey up to wave 4 or 5, when the children are 7 and 11 years old. Additionally, we exclude twins due to the possibility of different timings in their development with respect to single-birth children (Mowrer, 1954; Mittler, 1971). The sample is further restricted to children with non-missing information on the dependent variables. Our final samples comprise 10,570 children in wave 4 (children aged 7) and 9438 in wave 5 (children aged 11). Table 9 in the Appendix shows how the final samples analysed differ from the initial sample in wave one, due to attrition and sample selection. It turns out that the final samples include more educated and work-attached parents than the general population interviewed in wave one.

The MCS has repeated measurements of a child’s non-cognitive outcomes and contains rich information about parental socio-economic background, employment status, childcare arrangements and specific parental inputs at various points in time. Of particular interest to this study are the variables reporting extra-curricular activities and indicators of the child’s development and wellbeing when s/he is 5, 7 and 11 years old. We focus on non-cognitive outcomes, specifically child socio-emotional skills derived from the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) included in the MCS, which highlights both positive and undesirable behaviours.Footnote 5 As with most of the variables, the respondent to questions regarding activities and child socio-emotional skills is virtually always the mother.Footnote 6

Ideally, one would like to investigate the impact of activities on both cognitive and non-cognitive outcomes; however, in the MCS, no cognitive indicator is measured over the three waves, and between ages 7 and 11, none is measured over the two waves. Therefore, the empirical strategies would be different from the one implemented in the study, limiting the scope of comparison between results on cognitive and non-cognitive outcomes. Considering the dearth of studies on the relationship between activities and socio-emotional development, we found it more valuable and interesting to focus on non-cognitive outcomes.

The 25 items on the SDQ ask parents about the behavioural attributes of their child and measure five children’s socio-emotional dimensions (emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems and prosocial behaviour).Footnote 7 Each dimension is derived from five items, such as ‘Shares readily with other children (treats, toys, etc.)’ (Goodman, 1997). For each item, the possible answers are ‘not true’ (0 points), ‘somewhat true’ (1), and ‘certainly true’ (2). The groups of five answers are combined in a total score for each socio-emotional dimension, ranging from 0 to 10. Lower scores identify positive traits for the first four dimensions, while a higher score identifies more positive traits regarding prosocial behaviour. Table 1 summarises the dependent variables for children aged 7 and 11 years old, while the distribution of their outcomes is presented in Fig. 1.Footnote 8

Table 1 Children’s socio-emotional skills (main outcomes)

The main independent variables in our analysis are the activities undertaken by children in their free time. The data provides information about an extensive range of activities, including playing a musical instrument, going to the library, attending religious services and classes, participating in sports, reading, watching TV and playing electronic games. In waves 3 and 4 (at ages 5 and 7), we also have information about other activities conducted with the parents: parents reading to the child, playing music with the child and drawing with the child. Unfortunately, the data does not provide the number of minutes/hours spent on each activity, only a measure of frequency (e.g. Every day/Several times a week/Once or twice a week/Once or twice a month/Less often/Not at all). We recode the frequency of most activities as dummy variables where 1 indicates that the activity is conducted at least once a week. The only exceptions are represented by homework and video-screen activities, where 1 indicates at least one hour per day. All the activities are listed and described in Table 2. We report activities at ages 7 and 11, ages at which we study their effects on socio-emotional skills, but also at age 5, since we will use past activities as further controls (see Section 3). The first three columns of Table 2 (columns 1 to 3) provide an overview of both the activities recorded over time and the more age-specific ones. Among the activities recorded over time, we see an increase in the time devoted to sport (without parents) and computer use. Columns 4 and 5 in the Table report the proportion of children changing their participation in each activity between the different waves: switching from doing the activity in wave w to not doing it in wave w + 1, or vice versa.Footnote 9

Table 2 Activities

With so many variables of interest, interpreting the results can be quite challenging, especially as some of the reported variables are likely to capture types of activities that are relatively similar to each other. We therefore implement a principal component analysis (PCA) aimed at developing better insight into the number of common latent dimensions that the different activities may share.Footnote 10

Tables 1012 in the Appendix report the PCA. In wave 3, we obtain four components, while in waves 4 and 5 we obtain seven components. In Table 3, we summarise the grouping of activities in the different components: (1) activities with parents; (2) sports; (3) library and religious activities; (4) video-screen time; (5) reading, caring and tidying-up (which covers reading, caring for pets and looking after an elderly household member and tidying up); (6) Extra hours at school (including after school and before school classes); and (7) school-related activities (including homework and extra classes). No components express extra hours at school, school-related activities or reading/caring activities for children when they are 5 years old. Notably, in applying the PCA, similar activities are grouped together, as one would do without statistical methods.

Table 3 Principal component analysis for activities in the three waves

Some activities are clearly related to one particular component, and are always associated with the same component over the three waves, like watching TV or playing with the computer. The same happens for the four activities related to the extra-hours at school and school-related activities. Other activities are highly related with more than one component, such as ‘parents play active games’.Footnote 11 Importantly, the activities, which are present both when the child is 7 and when the child is 11 (our main ages of interest), are allocated to the same components in the two waves.

3 Empirical methods

Our aim is to estimate the effects of children’s time allocation on five socio-emotional outcomes. For the main specification, we chose a cumulative value-added (CUVA) model (Section 3.1), whose results are shown for outcomes at ages 7 and 11. To test their robustness to possible endogenous issues, we handled the risk of omitted variables bias in Section 3.2, the risk of reverse causality in Section 3.3 and the risk of measurement error bias due to the inclusion of past values of the dependent variable in Section 3.4. Finally, in Section 3.5, we propose a fixed-effect model (FE), which allowed us to consider unobservable time-invariant characteristics, although it does not permit estimation of age-specific effects.

3.1 The cumulative value-added model

With the CUVA specification for each child’s socio-emotional skill, we estimated the following linear equation with OLS, once for outcomes at age t = 7 and once for outcomes at age t = 11:

$$Y_{i,t} = \alpha _t + A_{i,t}^\prime \beta _{1t} + A_{i,t - m}^\prime \beta _{2t} + \beta _{3t}Y_{i,t - m} + Z_{i,t}^\prime \beta _{4t} + \varepsilon _{i,t}$$
(1)

where Y represents one of the five child socio-emotional outcomes for child i at age t, vector A indicates the components expressing different uses of time and vector Z indicates the control variables of child i at or before age t. The subscript m is equal to 2 when we estimate the effects at age 7, including time-use components and outcomes measured at age 5, and is equal to 4 when we estimate the effects at age 11, including time-use components and outcomes measured at age 7.Footnote 12β1 is the coefficient of interest. In this model, we included information regarding the past use of the child’s time (allowing for a ‘lagged’ effect) and information on the child’s non-cognitive outcome in the previous wave, which can control for most of the differences across children.Footnote 13 The inclusion of past values of the output in the model should capture all unobservable past inputs and unobservable characteristics of the child, e.g. her/his temperament, talents and preferences. This model is equivalent to comparing the socio-emotional skills of two children at age 7 (11) who used to have the same skill indicator and time inputs at age 5 (7), but may have used their time in a different way between ages 5 and 7 (7 and 11).

The assumptions underlying model (1) include the following: the information contained in vector Z and Yi,t − m is a good proxy of any unobserved inputs and unobservable characteristics of the child; the effect of unobserved inputs and child’s characteristics declines with age at the rate β3; and there is no remaining unobserved heterogeneity that correlates with activities at age t (Todd & Wolpin, 2003, 2007; for details about these assumptions).

Examples of variables contained in vector Z are personal characteristics, parents’ and family characteristics and socio-economic circumstances. The detailed descriptive statistics are reported in Table 13 (panel A to C). First, we considered variables that describe the environment/context that children are faced with, referred to as environmental variables (Table 13, panel A). They were measured in the same waves as the main outcomes (at ages 7 and 11) and are related to the household dimension (mother, father, siblings and grandparents’ presence in the household; parental hours of work). A second set of variables account for previous parental investments (before age 7), and are fixed over time (Table 13, panel B): whether the child was breastfed, how long the mother stayed at home after birth, type of childcare when the child was 30 months old, father’s involvement with the child when the child was 9 months old and parental education. Third, we included some socio-demographic control variables concerning the child, parents and household (Table 13, panel C) that may correlate with the use of time and may affect the outcomes. Control variables about the child were all measured in the first wave: gender, nationality, birth weight, age, number of siblings at birth, hospitalisations, accidents and the three indicators of child development in the first year of life,Footnote 14 which capture child endowments at an early age and are known to be predictive of later development (Hernández-Alava & Popli, 2017). We included the following variables concerning the parents: quality of the child–mother relationship,Footnote 15 locus of control of the mother,Footnote 16 mother’s personality typeFootnote 17 and parents’ mental wellbeing.Footnote 18,Footnote 19 Regarding the household, we included the presence of new-borns, household equivalent income (both measured at the current wave), household location (England, Scotland, Wales and Northern Ireland) and whether the child had been on holidays outside the UK in the past year.

Finally, in addition to the child’s development at nine months, to consider the correlation between the different abilities of the child as s/he grows older, we included one variable measuring the child’s cognitive ability during the previous wave, which was derived through PCA of the cognitive items available in the survey (see Table 15 in the Appendix).Footnote 20 At age 7, we considered past measures of abilities in naming objects, coordinating figures in the spatial dimension and problem-solving (measured at age 5). For age 11, we considered past measures of abilities in reading, math and coordinating figures in the spatial dimension (measured at age 7).

Although we can control for a large set of variables using the CUVA specification, causal interpretation of the results remains tentative, as discussed and addressed hereafter.

3.2 Risk of variable omission

Although the model included the lag of the dependent variable and a large set of control variables, there may still be unobservable characteristics of the child/family that correlate with time use and child behaviours. For example, a young boy might be very shy and therefore be both less likely to engage in sports and more likely to be bullied by school-mates, without less sport directly causing more peer problems. Or, strict parents may require the child to tidy up the room and also directly affect the child’s level of obedience.

We handled the risk of omitted variable bias by applying a method designed to assess the stability of coefficients in the presence of unobservable selection (Oster, 2019) to the CUVA specification. This method, building on the previous study by Altonji et al. (2005), evaluated the robustness of results against omitted variable bias, assuming that the relationship between the treatment and the unobservables can be recovered from the relationship between the treatment and the observables and allowed the coefficient of interest to be bound in the presence of such omitted variable bias. Consequently, we need to choose a level of Rmax that corresponds with the R-squared from a hypothetical regression of the outcome on the treatment and both the observed and unobserved controls. If the outcome could be fully explained by the treatment and full control set, then Rmax would be 1; however, in many empirical settings, it seems likely that the outcome cannot be fully explained, even if the full control set is included (e.g. due to measurement error). Therefore, one needs to choose a bound of Rmax, and Oster (2019) proposed focusing on bounds that are a function of the observed R-squared of the regression with a full set of observable controls. We chose Rmax = 1.3 R-squared, as suggested by Oster (2019). We subsequently calculated the bounds of the estimated coefficients for different values of the relative degree of the selection on the observed and unobserved variables (δ). We focused on δ = 0, corresponding with the original estimates, and δ = 1 as the upper bound, which corresponds with the assumption of equal selection between observed and unobserved variables, as suggested by Oster (2019).

3.3 Risk of reverse causality

Regarding reverse causality, all the estimates control for past socio-emotional problems, so this issue was already partially solved. However, it is still possible that even under the same value of socio-emotional skills at age 7, reverse causality emerges in the relationship between socio-emotional skills and activities at age 11. Thus, to test our results against the risk of reverse causality, we decided to include only the value of past engagement in the different activities on current socio-emotional skills in the regressions.

3.4 Risk of measurement errors

The estimated model, which includes lagged values of the dependent variables, implicitly considers omitted past inputs and controls for unobservable characteristics of the child, e.g. her/his temperament, talents and preferences. However, there could be a problem if past outcomes are measured with error, as this can induce biased estimates. Therefore, to address measurement error in the lagged outcomes, we used the instrumental variable method: using a two-period lagged outcome as an instrument for the one-period lagged outcome is an acknowledged solution to measurement error in value-added models (Arellano & Bond, 1991; Andrabi et al., 2011; Del Bono et al., 2016).

3.5 Fixed-effect model

To consider the unobservable characteristics fixed over time, an alternative strategy is to estimate the model with individual fixed effects (FE). The FE specification is useful when we want to relax the assumption about no unobserved heterogeneity that correlates with extra school activities at age t. For this specification, using data from both waves, we estimated the following equation:

$$Y_{i,t} = \alpha _t + A_{i,t}^\prime \beta _t + Z_{i,t}^\prime \theta _t\upsilon _i + e_{i,t}$$
(2)

With this model, including child fixed-effects υi, we can observe whether a change in the frequency of activities conducted between ages 7 and 11 explains part of the difference in the child’s socio-emotional skills over time, eliminating the effect of the unobservable characteristics fixed over time as well as other unobserved family characteristics fixed over time. In this model, vector Z includes only time-varying covariates, i.e. only the controls presented in panel A of Table 15 and the time-varying controls in panel C: the presence of new-borns, household equivalent income, holidays outside the UK in the past year and the child’s cognitive ability at the previous wave.

CUVA and child FE specifications rely on different assumptions about the relationship between the child’s time use and outcomes. In the first case, the model allowed for a temporal adjustment because the present effect of an activity can be different from the effect of the same activity in the past. With the child FE, we assumed instead that the input effects are age invariant.

4 The effects of time allocation

4.1 Main results

The effects of time use—represented as different components—on the five socio-emotional skills are presented in Tables 48. For each outcome, and separately at ages 7–11, the tables report the effects of the activities obtained with the CUVA specification (columns 1 and 5), the Oster bounds (columns 2 and 6), the effects of the lagged activities (columns 3 and 7) and the effects of the activities when dealing with the endogeneity of the lagged dependent variable (columns 4 and 8). The last column reports the effects estimated when employing FE and the whole sample. A negative sign of the coefficient indicates that the activity reduces that behavioural problem and thus has a ‘beneficial’ effect, or vice versa. The only exception is the prosocial dimension, which must be read backwards (a negative sign of the coefficient indicates a detrimental effect). When reading the effects of the lagged activities, we need to remember that some activities are not observed at age 5, and therefore, their effect at age 7 cannot be estimated (column 3).

Table 4 The effects of children’s allocation of time on prosocial behaviour
Table 5 The effects of children’s allocation of time on conduct problems
Table 6 The effects of children’s allocation of time on peer relationship problems
Table 7 The effects of children’s allocation of time on emotional symptoms
Table 8 The effects of children’s allocation of time on hyperactivity/inattention

Overall, we find that time spent with parents and time on sports, school-related activities, reading and caring/tidying up have beneficial effects, while extra hours at school and video-screen time have harmful effects. No effects were found for participating in religious activities or going to the library. Prosocial behaviour, which improves children’s ability to share with others and be helpful, proves to be the dimension most responsive to time allocation. All non-cognitive dimensions are strongly correlated over time.

We now comment on the strongest results, namely, those found in our main specification and robust against endogeneity issues. It may happen that they are not confirmed (models to test against omitted variable bias, reverse causality, or measurement error are more restrictive): if that is the case, such results are not presented in this discussion. Both the dependent and independent variables are standardised, which makes it easier to read the results.

Prosocial behaviour is influenced positively by several activities (Table 4): time spent with parents, reading and caring/tidying up, conducting school-related activities at both ages and doing sports at the age of 11.

Conduct problems are reduced at both ages by reading and caring/tidying up (also confirmed by the FE model) and at age 11 by time spent with parents and on school-related activities (Table 5). Peer problems are reduced at both ages by sports activities, as also confirmed by the FE (Table 6). Emotional symptoms were mitigated at age 7 by reading and caring/tidying up and at age 11 by sport activities (Table 7). However, screen time has a detrimental effect on a child’s emotional sphere at age 11.

Both reading and caring/tidying up decreased inattention problems at both ages, as also confirmed by the FE model. Children aged 11 who spend more active time with their parents present fewer hyperactivity/inattention problems (Table 8). Instead, spending more time at school at younger ages increases this type of problem.

Overall, time spent with parents reading, playing games has a beneficial effect on prosocial behaviour, conduct and inattention problems. Reading and caring/tidying up positively influence prosocial behaviour and decrease inattention, conduct and emotional problems. Time spent on homework and extracurricular activities at school improves prosocial behaviour and reduces conduct problems. Sport has beneficial effects on prosocial behaviour, peer problems and emotional symptoms. In contrast, video-screen time has a detrimental effect on the emotional sphere of older children, and extra time spent at school may increase hyperactivity issues in younger children.

In terms of size, the estimated effects indicate that one standard deviation (s.d.) of difference in activities leads to 0.03–0.11 s.d. in difference in behavioural dimensions. By means of comparison, studies documenting the impacts of ad hoc interventions targeting socio-emotional skills find effects of 0.15–0.30 s.d., depending on the program, the outcome, and the time elapsed between the intervention and observed outcome.Footnote 21 Compared to these findings, results for the allocation of time appear relatively small, but one needs to keep in mind that this is ‘standard’ use of time and not an ad hoc program. Instead, the magnitude is comparable to estimates presented in studies on the effect of time use on cognitive skills. For instance, according to Fiorini & Keane (2014) an additional hour of educational activities with parents improves cognitive skills by 0.02–0.04 s.d., and according to Del Bono et al. (2016) educational time with the mother improve verbal skills by 0.04–0.05 s.d. at age 7 (contemporaneous and CUVA models respectively).

To better understand the effect sizes in our setting, we make some predictions. We need to remind that the dependent variable is the sum of the mother’s answers, which can take the value 0, 1, or 2; lower scores identify positive traits for the first four socio-emotional dimensions, whereas higher scores identify positive traits regarding prosocial behaviour (see Section 2 for the details). Compare, for example, an 11-year-old child who does sports and goes biking at least once a week with a child with the same characteristics but who does not perform these activities or does them less often. The mother of the first child will provide fewer (negative) answers when asked about the child’s emotional sphere, inducing a reduction in the average emotional symptom score of 0.51 (from 2.19 to 1.68 score); she will also give fewer negative answers when asked about peer problems, yielding an average reduction of 0.80 (from 1.87 to 1.07). Instead, she will answer positively more often when asked about prosocial behaviour, yielding an increase of 0.23 points (from 8.68 to 8.91). Conversely, the mother of a child who both watches TV and uses a PC more than an hour per day will answer more often that her child has emotional problems, with an increase of 0.10 (from 1.73 to 1.83) in the score.Footnote 22,Footnote 23

We also investigated the possible heterogeneous effects of time use according to socio-economic status, ethnic background, gender, and family composition, but we found no strong evidence for any heterogeneous effect.Footnote 24 However, if children from different context have different likelihood of spending time on the activities, the overall benefits may be unequally distributed across them. The findings, available as online supplemental material, show that children from more advantaged backgrounds are more exposed to enriching uses of their time and less exposed to detrimental ones. Consequently, differences in the use of time by children from different family backgrounds should be considered as an additional source of inequality.

Most of the beneficial effects we find on the child’s socio-emotional skills follow previous findings on the cognitive dimensions of children. In addition to the positive influence of participating in sports and activities with parents on many educational outcomes found in previous studies, here we also find positive effects on non-cognitive outcomes. New evidence is then provided for the beneficial effects of time spent on activities performed at home, such as reading, doing homework and caring for pets and other people.

Our results seem to differ from those of Fiorini & Keane (2014) and Del Bono et al. (2016), since neither of those studies found effects on non-cognitive outcomes. Differences with results in Fiorini & Keane (2014) may be due to institutional differences across the two countries, different sample sizes, the age-range considered and different econometric specifications. On the other hand, while the use of the same data and the same econometric specification in Del Bono et al. (2016) question the causes of different results, there are two major differences which motivate them: the inclusion of prosocial behaviour as an outcome—which is not included in their estimates and turns out to be very responsive to time allocation—and the analysis of different categories of time use, not only time with parents. For the major overlap between the two studies (parental time at age 7), we find that time spent with parents has a positive effect only on prosocial behaviour.Footnote 25

The results follow psychological studies on child non-cognitive development, which underline the beneficial effects of active and dynamic uses of time versus the detrimental effects of passive activities. Sports, active time with parents and doing things at home are better than spending time watching TV or using PC and tablets. While dynamic uses of time imply effort and perseverance and therefore provide feelings of satisfaction for the child, this is not true for inactive uses of time (Veenhoven, 1984; Emmons, 2003). Another interesting finding is the substantial influence of several activities on the child’s prosocial behaviour, which can be considered a sort of feeling of empathy towards others. This is an attitude expected to grow with the individual, a non-cognitive dimension that measures the passage from ‘childhood’ (when behaviours are motivated by the need for attachment) to ‘adulthood’ (when behaviours are motivated by the feeling of looking after someone else) (Solomon & George, 1996; Nuttall et al., 2015). It is plausible that this ability can be learnt by spending time with parents and other caring adults and observing them. In fact, we find positive effects of time spent with parents, time spent on doing homework (which may also be time shared with parents), taking extra classes (with a tutor) and spending active time within the household (which may also be time shared with other family members) on children’s empathy. We also found that sports have a beneficial effect on prosocial behaviour and peer problems; this effect may be due to another mechanism: the need for collaboration (Lichtenberg et al., 2012). In fact, to succeed, the child needs to interact proficiently with his/her companions.

4.2 Robustness checks

As mentioned above, instead of including lagged inputs and outputs as control variables, as is done in the CUVA model, one could use contemporaneous inputs only, contemporaneous and lagged inputs (cumulative model—CU), or contemporaneous inputs and lagged output (value-added model—VA).

Tables 1620 in the Appendix present the results of these different models: (i) the simple OLS model, which estimates the contemporaneous effect of activities on the outcome (Contemp.); (ii) the VA; (iii) the CU and (iv) the CUVA, the main specification of the study. Most of the results are confirmed in the different models; most importantly, all the results in the CUVA model are also present in the other specifications, with the CUVA specification showing the smallest coefficient (with only three exceptions over 70 estimated parameters) confirming CUVA to be the most restrictive model and providing the most conservative estimates among these models. The results discussed in the previous section, which are robust to the different endogeneity issues, are always confirmed.

5 Conclusion

In this study, we investigated the relationship between time allocation and children’s social, emotional and behavioural skills using UK longitudinal data and testing the robustness of our results to different endogeneity issues. We found that different activities influence the child’s non-cognitive development. Overall, sports, school-related activities, reading and caring/tidying up activities and time spent with parents tend to reduce socio-emotional problems, whereas video-screen time had detrimental effects for older children and extra hours at school were harmful for younger ones. No effects were found for participating in religious activities or going to the library. The largest positive effect of time use was observed in prosocial behaviour in the form of sharing with others and being helpful. Most of the beneficial effects we found on the child’s socio-emotional skills confirm previous findings on her/his cognitive development. To our knowledge, however, this is the first study to find beneficial effects of a child’s time allocation on non-cognitive development.

Considering that children from different family backgrounds do not afford the same opportunities for their time use, from a policy perspective our results call for the provision of free to low-cost extra-curricular activities to be held after school, in particular sports and school-related activities. After-school programmes may also reduce time spent on TV and screen time. Additionally, this could be complemented by providing more information to the parents about the beneficial effects of such activities.

There are three main limitations to this study. First, we do not know how much time the child actually spends on any of the activities. Not only would this be another important source of heterogeneity across children, but it could also reveal the non-linear effects of these activities. Second, to better interpret the results obtained for children’s non-cognitive development, it would be useful to know more details about the activities conducted. For example, to understand the level of passivity involved in activities under the video-screen category, we would need to know whether children are watching a movie/cartoon or an interactive cartoon, playing video-games, watching other people playing those video-games, singing or dancing while watching music videos or searching for commercial videos online (e.g. the unboxing of toys). Finally, we do not have a full description of time use. Therefore, we lack information about other important ways children spend their time, such as ‘pure’ playtime (playing by themselves or with siblings/cousins), time spent at dinner or social events, sleeping routines and the management of boredom and waiting-time. Future studies should investigate such factors to completely unveil the relationship between children’s time use and their non-cognitive development.