Skip to main content
Log in

Improving Mortgage Default Collection Efforts by Employing the Decoy Effect

  • Published:
The Journal of Real Estate Finance and Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We test the ability of the Decoy Effect to enhance debt collection efforts and find that by disclosing the Annual Percentage Rate (APR) in settlement offers, participants are less influenced by the decoy and more apt to select the repayment option that is in their best interest. At the same time, by reporting the APR, borrowers are more willing to make repayments on the modified loan, resulting in a net gain to debt collection efforts. Because disclosing the APR is Consumer Financial Protection Bureau (CFPB) compliant, this simple disclosure has the ability to increase debt collection returns while helping borrowers make better decisions when selecting debt modification repayment plans. Our results suggest an applicability to all types of defaulted debt including mortgages, sub-prime auto loans, credit cards, student loans, and payday loans.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Additional studies examining the decoy effect include, but are not limited to, Ratneshwar et al. (1987); Simonson (1989); Wedell (1991); Mishra et al. (1993); Redelmeier and Shafir (1995); Highhouse (1996); Herne (1997); Herne (1998); Schwartz and Chapman (1999); Slaughter et al. (1999); Slaughter (2007); Lombardi (2009); Clippel and Eliaz (2012); Gerasimou (2013); and Ok et al. (2014).

  2. Pub.L. 111–203; 124 Stat. 1376–2223.

  3. https://www.hmpadmin.com/portal/programs/hamp.jsp. Accessed on March 27, 2018.

  4. Spacing the APRs associated with the three options by a lesser (greater) percentage will reasonably be expected to allow the decoy effect to be more (less) effective. We selected this amount because a 9% increase in yield is certainly enough of a boost in debt collector returns to warrant serious policy adoption consideration.

  5. Anyone can join MTurk by going to their website and registering as a “worker.” While MTurk does not publish the profile of their workers, it is reasonable to assume they cover a broad spectrum of society. Any adult with a computer and access to the Internet is eligible to join. www.mturk.com. Accessed on March 27, 2018.

  6. Funding for this experiment comes from the personal research budget of one of the authors.

  7. Employing a more selective 10 s screen yields qualitatively identical results.

  8. For further information on these metrics, see http://www.usfinancialcapability.org/quiz.php. Accessed on March 27, 2018.

  9. 2013 American Housing Survey, http://www.census.gov/programs-surveys/ahs/data/2013/ahs-2013-summary-tables/national-summary-report-and-tables%2D%2D-ahs-2013.html

  10. Although our instructions indicate that the respondents should not be concerned about the size of the payment, some respondents may include their actual income constraints in their payment decision. As a robustness check, when restricted to higher income respondents (incomes over $60,000), our results remain unchanged (available upon request).

  11. We also investigated whether the results are sensitive to participant and state-specific attributes. Individual characteristics tested include the level of financial literacy, previous default, and net worth. State-specific attributes investigated center around foreclosure rules including whether the states require judicial foreclosure, is a recourse state, whether the state allows statutory redemption, and average foreclosure time. Our results are robust to all these additional checks.

  12. While beyond the scope of the current investigation, these latter results with respect to our control variables may well be due, at least in part, to issues of perceived affordability of the mortgage repayment options. While all experimental respondents were told to assume they could afford all proffered alternatives, the effective cost or implied magnitude of the designated terms may well be perceived differently across wealth, age, and/or geographic groupings.

References

  • Ariely, D., & Wallsten, T. S. (1995). Seeking subjective dominance in multidimensional space: An explanation of the asymmetric dominance effect. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 63(3), 223–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arrow, K.J. (1951). Social choice and individual values. (Cowles commission Mongr. No. 12.).

  • Clippel, G. D., & Eliaz, K. (2012). Reason-based choice: A bargaining rationale for the attraction and compromise effects. Theoretical Economics, 7(1), 125–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cypher, M., Price, S. M., Robinson, S. J., & Seiler, M. J. (2018). Price signals and uncertainty in commercial real estate transactions. Journal of Real Estate Finance and Economics, 57(2), 246–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doyle, J. R., O'Connor, D. J., Reynolds, G. M., & Bottomley, P. A. (1999). The robustness of the asymmetrically dominated effect: Buying frames, phantom alternatives, and in-store purchases. Psychology & Marketing, 16(3), 225–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerasimou, G. (2013). On continuity of incomplete preferences. Social Choice and Welfare, 41(1), 157–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R., & Gough, H. G. (2006). The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(1), 84–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herne, K. (1997). Decoy alternatives in policy choices: Asymmetric domination and compromise effects. European Journal of Political Economy, 13(3), 575–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herne, K. (1998). Testing the reference-dependent model: An experiment on asymmetrically dominated reference points. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11(3), 181–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Highhouse, S. (1996). Context-dependent selection: The effects of decoy and phantom job candidates. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65(1), 68–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huber, J., Payne, J. W., & Puto, C. (1982). Adding asymmetrically dominated alternatives: Violations of regularity and the similarity hypothesis. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(1), 90–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ikromov, N., & Yavas, A. (2012a). Asset characteristics and boom and bust periods: An experimental study. Real Estate Economics, 40(3), 603–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ikromov, N., & Yavas, A. (2012b). Cash flow volatility, prices and price volatility: An experimental study. Journal of Real Estate Finance and Economics, 44(1), 203–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombardi, M. (2009). Reason-based choice correspondences. Mathematical Social Sciences, 57(1), 58–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, S., Umesh, U. N., & Stem, D. E. (1993). Antecedents of the attraction effect: An information-processing approach. Journal of Marketing Research, 30(3), 331–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ok, E. A., Ortoleva, P., & Riella, G. (2014). Revealed (p)reference theory. American Economic Review, 105(1), 299–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratneshwar, S., Shocker, A. D., & Stewart, D. W. (1987). Toward understanding the attraction effect: The implications of product stimulus meaningfulness and familiarity. Journal of Consumer Research, 13(4), 520–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redelmeier, D. A., & Shafir, E. (1995). Medical decision making in situations that offer multiple alternatives. JAMA, 273(4), 302–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sahin, M. A., Sirmans, C., & Yavas, A. (2013). Buyer brokerage: Experimental evidence. Journal of Housing Economics, 22(4), 265–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, J. A., & Chapman, G. B. (1999). Are more options always better?: The attraction effect in physicians' decisions about medications. Medical Decision Making, 19(3), 315–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzkopf, D. L. (2003). The effects of attraction on investment decisions. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 4(2), 96–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sedikides, C., Ariely, D., & Olsen, N. (1999). Contextual and procedural determinants of partner selection: Of asymmetric dominance and prominence. Social Cognition, 17(2), 118–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sefton, M., & Yavas, A. (1995). The welfare effects of a subsidy to multiple listing services. Journal of Real Estate Finance and Economics, 11(1), 85–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seiler, M. (2014). The effect of perceived lender characteristics and market conditions on strategic mortgage defaults. Journal of Real Estate Finance and Economics, 48(2), 256–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seiler, M. (2015a). The role of informational uncertainty in the decision to strategically default. Journal of Housing Economics, 27(March), 49–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seiler, M. (2015b). Do as I say, not as I do: The role of advice versus actions in the decision to strategically default. Journal of Real Estate Research, 37(2), 191–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seiler, M. (2016). The perceived moral reprehensibility of strategic mortgage default. Journal of Housing Economics, 32(June), 18–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seiler, M. (2017). Do liquidated damages clauses affect strategic mortgage default morality? A test of the disjunctive thesis. Real Estate Economics, 45(1), 204–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seiler, M. (2018). Asymmetric dominance and its impact on mortgage default deficiency collection efforts. Real Estate Economics, 46(4), 971–990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seiler, M., Seiler, V., Lane, M., & Harrison, D. (2012). Fear, shame and guilt: Economic and behavioral motivations for strategic default. Real Estate Economics, 40(S1), 199–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonson, I. (1989). Choice based on reasons: The case of attraction and compromise effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(2), 158–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slaughter, J. E. (2007). Effects of two selection batteries on decoy effects in job-finalist choice. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37(1), 76–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slaughter, J. E., Sinar, E. F., & Highhouse, S. (1999). Decoy effects and attribute-level inferences. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(5), 823–828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wedell, D. H. (1991). Distinguishing among models of contextually induced preference reversals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(4), 767–778.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yavas, A., Miceli, T. J., & Sirmans, C. (2001). An experimental analysis of the impact of intermediaries on the outcome of bargaining games. Real Estate Economics, 29(2), 251–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahirovic-Herbert, V., Gibler, K., Chatterjee, S., & Reed, R. (2016). Financial literacy, risky mortgages, and delinquency in the US during the financial crisis. International Journal of Housing Markets and Analysis, 9(2), 164–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank participants at the London School of Economics (LSE), University of Virginia, Consumer Financial Protection Bureau (CFPB), and the FSU-UF-UCF symposium for comments on this paper. We would particularly like to thank McKay Price, Jim Shilling, and John Glascock. All errors remain our own.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael J. Seiler.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Harrison, D.M., Luchtenberg, K.F. & Seiler, M.J. Improving Mortgage Default Collection Efforts by Employing the Decoy Effect. J Real Estate Finan Econ 66, 840–860 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11146-021-09876-8

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11146-021-09876-8

Keywords

Navigation