Skip to main content
Log in

Does the reading acceleration program improve reading fluency and comprehension in emergent bilingual children?

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It has been suggested that the reading acceleration program (RAP) (Breznitz et al. in Nature Communications 4: 1486, 2013), in which participants are forced to read at a rate faster than their normal reading fluency rate, improves reading fluency and comprehension in alphabetic languages. However, its effectiveness has never been examined in emergent bilingual children’s reading. This study explored the impact of the RAP, administered over 12 sessions for 4 consecutive weeks on emergent bilingual children’s reading in Chinese (L1) and English (L2). Participants were 97 Grade 5 students in Mainland China, randomly assigned to one of four groups: (1) a group with accelerated paradigm in English first followed by Chinese; (2) a group with accelerated paradigm in Chinese first followed by English; (3) a group with non-accelerated paradigm in English first followed by Chinese; and (4) a group with non-accelerated paradigm in Chinese first followed by English. The online test battery included reading fluency and comprehension in both Chinese and English. The offline standardized Chinese and English test battery included rapid automatized naming, word reading fluency, text reading fluency, and reading comprehension. Results showed pre-post improvement on both online and offline reading measures in the accelerated and non-accelerated groups. There was evidence of cross-linguistic transfer effect in the RAP intervention. Regardless of condition (accelerated or non-accelerated) children who received training in two languages outperformed those who received training in one language, who in turn outperformed those who did not receive any training targeting reading fluency. Results are discussed in terms of the RAP mechanism that improves reading fluency and reading comprehension in emergent bilingual children.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abu-Rabia, S., Shakkour, W., & Siegel, L. (2013). Cognitive retroactive transfer (CRT) of language skills among bilingual Arabic–English readers. Bilingual Research Journal, 36, 61–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altmisdort, G. (2016). The effects of L2 reading skills on L1 reading skills through transfer. English Language Teaching, 9, 28–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar‐Kochva, I., & Hasselhorn, M. (2015). In search of methods enhancing fluency in reading: An examination of the relations between time constraints and processes of reading in readers of German. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 140–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Lee, Y. L., Abbott, R. D., & Breznitz, Z. (2013). Teaching children with dyslexia to spell in a reading-writers’ workshop. Annals of Dyslexia, 63, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (1987). Increasing first graders’ reading accuracy and comprehension by accelerating their reading rates. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 236–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (1997). The effect of accelerated reading rate on memory for text among dyslexic readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 287–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (2006). Fluency in reading: Synchronization of processes. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., & Berman, L. (2003). The underlying factors of word reading rate. Educational Psychology Review, 15, 247–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., Shaul, S., Horowitz-Kraus, T., Sela, I., Nevat, M., & Karni, A. (2013). Enhanced reading by training with imposed time constraint in typical and dyslexic adults. Nature Communications, 4, 1486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chard, D. J., Vaughn, S., & Tyler, B. (2002). A synthesis of research on effective interventions for building reading fluency with elementary students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 386–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, X., Xu, F., Nguyen, T.-K., Hong, G., & Wang, Y. (2010). Effects of cross-language transfer on first-language phonological awareness and literacy skills in Chinese children receiving English instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 712–728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, S. C., Chen, X., & Geva, E. (2019). Deconstructing and reconstructing cross-language transfer. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 50, 149–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Review of Educational Research, 49, 222–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1991). Interdependence of first-and second-language proficiency in bilingual children. Language Processing in Bilingual Children, 70, 89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dai, L., Zhang, C., & Liu, X. (2016). A special Chinese reading acceleration training paradigm: To enhance the reading fluency and comprehension of Chinese children with reading disabilities. Frontiers in psychology, 7, 1937.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Ramírez, R. D., & Shapiro, E. S. (2007). Cross-language relationship between Spanish and English oral reading fluency among Spanish-speaking English language learners in bilingual education classrooms. Psychology in the Schools, 44, 795–806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donovan, J. J., & Radosevich, D. J. (1999). A meta-analytic review of the distribution of practice effect: Now you see it, now you don’t. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 795–805.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franceschini, S., Bertoni, S., Gianesini, T., Gori, S., & Facoetti, A. (2017). A different vision of dyslexia: Local precedence on global perception. Scientific Reports, 7, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fukkink, R., Hulstijn, J., & Simis, A. (2005). Does training in second-language word recognition skills affect reading comprehension? An experimental study. The Modern Language Journal, 89, 54–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2016). The anatomy of the RAN-reading relationship. Reading and Writing, 29, 1793–1815.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Farnia, F. (2012). Developmental changes in the nature of language proficiency and reading fluency paint a more complex view of reading comprehension in ELL and EL1. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 1819–1845.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Wiener, J. (2015). Psychological assessment of culturally and linguistically diverse children and adolescents. Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gorsuch, G., & Taguchi, E. (2008). Repeated reading for developing reading fluency and reading comprehension: The case of EFL learners in Vietnam. System, 36, 253–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hiebert, E. H., Samuels, S. J., & Rasinski, T. (2012). Comprehension-based silent reading rates: What do we know? What do we need to know? Literacy Research and Instruction, 51, 110–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz-Kraus, T. (2015). Improvement in non-linguistic executive functions following reading acceleration training in children with reading difficulties: An ERP study. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 4, 77–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz-Kraus, T., Cicchino, N., Amiel, M., Holland, S. K., & Breznitz, Z. (2014a). Reading improvement in English-and Hebrew-speaking children with reading difficulties after reading acceleration training. Annals of Dyslexia, 64, 183–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz-Kraus, T., & Holland, S. K. (2015). Greater functional connectivity between reading and error-detection regions following training with the reading acceleration program in children with reading difficulties. Annals of Dyslexia, 65, 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz-Kraus, T., Vannest, J. J., Kadis, D., Cicchino, N., Wang, Y. Y., & Holland, S. K. (2014b). Reading acceleration training changes brain circuitry in children with reading difficulties. Brain and Behavior, 4, 886–902.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, G. (2007). The juggernaut of Chinese–English bilingual education. In A. Feng (Ed.), Bilingual education in China: Practices, policies, and concepts (pp. 94–126). Multilingual Matters.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Institute of Language Teaching and Research (1986). Modern Chinese Frequency Dictionary. Beijing: Beijing Language Institute Press.

  • Jeon, E. H., & Yamashita, J. (2014). L2 reading comprehension and its correlates: A meta-analysis. Language Learning, 64, 160–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y. G. (2015). Developmental, component-based model of reading fluency: An investigation of predictors of word-reading fluency, text-reading fluency, and reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 50, 459–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korinth, S. P., Dimigen, O., Sommer, W., & Breznitz, Z. (2016). Reading training by means of disappearing text: Effects on reading performance and eye movements. Reading and Writing, 29, 1245–1268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korinth, S. P., & Nagler, T. (2021). Improving reading rates and comprehension? Benefits and limitations of the reading acceleration approach. Language and Linguistics Compass, 15, e12408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ku, Y. M., & Anderson, R. C. (2003). Development of morphological awareness in Chinese and English. Reading and Writing, 16, 399–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, M., & Stahl, S. (2003). Fluency: A review of developmental and remedial practices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive psychology, 6, 293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • López-Escribano, C. (2016). Training reading fluency and comprehension of Spanish children with dyslexia. In A. Khateb & I. Bar-Kochva (Eds.), Reading fluency: Current insights from neurocognitive research and intervention studies (pp. 141–161). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B. J. F., Talbot, A. P., & Florencio, D. (1999). Reading rate and prose retrieval. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3(4), 303–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagler, T., Korinth, S. P., Linkersdörfer, J., Lonnemann, J., Rump, B., Hasselhorn, M., & Lindberg, S. (2015). Text-fading based training leads to transfer effects on children’s sentence reading fluency. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagler, T., Linkersdörfer, J., Lonnemann, J., Hasselhorn, M., & Lindberg, S. (2016). The impact of text fading on reading in children with reading difficulties. Journal for Educational Research Online, 8, 26–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagler, T., Lonnemann, J., Linkersdörfer, J., Hasselhorn, M., & Lindberg, S. (2014). The impact of reading material’s lexical accessibility on text fading effects in children’s reading performance. Reading and Writing, 27, 841–853.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagler, T., Zarić, J., Kachisi, F., Lindberg, S., & Ehm, J. H. (2021). Reading-impaired children improve through text-fading training: Analyses of comprehension, orthographic knowledge, and RAN. Annals of Dyslexia, 71, 458–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read. National Institutes of Health.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nevo, E., Brande, S., & Shaul, S. (2016). The effects of two different reading acceleration training programs on improving reading skills of second graders. Reading Psychology, 37, 533–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nevo, E., & Breznitz, Z. (2014). Effects of working memory and reading acceleration training on improving working memory abilities and reading skills among third graders. Child Neuropsychology, 20, 752–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niedo, J., Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (2014). Predicting levels of reading and writing achievement in typically developing, English-speaking 2nd and 5th graders. Learning and Individual Differences, 32, 54–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paige, D. D. (2011). Testing the acceleration hypothesis: Fluency outcomes utilizing still-versus accelerated-text in sixth-grade students with reading disabilities. Literacy Research and Instruction, 50, 294–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pasquarella, A., Chen, X., Gottardo, A., & Geva, E. (2015). Cross-language transfer of word reading accuracy and word reading fluency in Spanish–English and Chinese–English bilinguals: Script-universal and script-specific processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 96–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snellings, P., van der Leij, A., de Jong, P. F., & Blok, H. (2009). Enhancing the reading fluency and comprehension of children with reading disabilities in an orthographically transparent language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 291–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & O’Connor, R. (2009). The role of working memory and fluency practice on the reading comprehension of students who are dysfluent readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 548–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taguchi, E., Takayasu-Maass, M., & Gorsuch, G. (2004). Developing reading fluency in EFL: How assisted repeated reading and extensive reading affect fluency development. Reading in a Foreign Language, 16, 70–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Therrien, W., Wickstrom, K., & Jones, K. (2006). Effects of a combined repeated reading and question generation intervention on reading achievement. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 21, 89–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. (1999). Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Pearson, N. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing: CTOPP. Austin, TX: Pro-ed.

  • Wechsler, D. (2009). Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Pearson.

  • Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.

  • Zhang, J., Anderson, R. C., Li, H., Dong, Q., Wu, X., & Zhang, Y. (2010). Cross-language transfer of insight into the structure of compound words. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 311–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Miao Li.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

There are no financial interests to report.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Li, M., Xi, Y., Geva, E. et al. Does the reading acceleration program improve reading fluency and comprehension in emergent bilingual children?. Read Writ (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10511-6

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10511-6

Keywords

Navigation