Skip to main content
Log in

Executive functions and pragmatics in children with high-functioning autism

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Children with deficits in executive functions (EFs) and impairments in pragmatic language have a range of cognitive and language difficulties that affect their literacy and educational achievements. As deficits in EFs and pragmatic impairments are characteristics of autism spectrum disorders, this study examined the associations between EFs and pragmatic skills in children with high-functioning autism (HFA). Fifteen children with HFA (5–9 years; M = 7.44, SD 1.21), matched to 15 typically developing peers on age, gender, and non-verbal intelligence participated in the study. Children completed a pragmatic language assessment protocol, and a rating scale of EFs was administered to parents. Our results point to two main findings: children with HFA presented pragmatic difficulties and EFs impairments when compared with typically developing peers; and, as shown by a significant indirect effect of group on pragmatics via EFs, the poor pragmatics skills of HFA children were associated with their EFs difficulties. These findings may be of clinical relevance for children with pragmatic impairments, such as autism spectrum disorders. Future studies are crucial to further investigate the relationship between impaired pragmatics and EFs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akbar, M., Loomis, R., & Paul, R. (2013). The interplay of language on executive functions in children with ASD. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,7(3), 494–501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.09.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ambridge, B., & Rowland, C. F. (2013). Experimental methods in studying child language acquisition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science,4(2), 149–168. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.744053.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2009). Studies of adults can inform accounts of theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology,45(1), 190–201. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014098.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,4(10), 829–839. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barbosa, A., Teles, S., & Vicente, S. G. (2011). Behavior rating of executive function (BRIEF): European Portuguese - Parental Version [working research version constructed based on the BRIEF of Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000]. Unpublished Material, Centre of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal.

  • Barnard, L., Muldoon, K., Hasan, R., O’Brien, G., & Stewart, M. (2008). Profiling executive dysfunction in adults with Autism and comorbid learning disability. Autism,12(2), 125–141. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361307088486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Is Asperger Syndrome/High-Functioning Autism necessarily a disability? Development and Psychopathology,12(3), 489–500. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400003126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., Nation, K., & Patterson, K. (2014). When words fail us: Insights into language processing from developmental and acquired disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,369, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2005). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology. Autism,9(1), 7–27. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361305049027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blain-Brière, B., Bouchard, C., & Bigras, N. (2014). The role of executive functions in the pragmatic skills of children age 4–5. Frontiers in Psychology,5, 20. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonifacio, S., Girolametto, L., Bulligan, M., Callegari, M., Vignola, S., & Zocconi, E. (2007). Assertive and responsive conversational skills of Italian-speaking late talkers. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,42(5), 607–623. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820601084386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruce, B., Thernlund, G., & Nettelbladt, U. (2006). ADHD and language impairment: A study of the parent questionnaire FTF (Five to Fifteen). European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,15(1), 52–60. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-006-0508-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Button, S., & Millward, P. (2005). Talking into literacy in the early years. Forum,47(1), 34–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, L. A., Soorya, L., & Halpern, D. (2009). Asperger’s Syndrome and high-functioning Autism. Pediatric Annals,38(1), 30–35. https://doi.org/10.3928/00904481-20090101-01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P., & Smith, P. K. (Eds.). (1996). Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,45(6), 1142–1157. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/093).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cekaite, A. (2013). Child pragmatic development. In C. A. Chapelle (Ed.), The encyclopedia of applied linguistics (pp. 602–609). West Sussex: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coplan, R. J., & Weeks, M. (2009). Shy and soft-spoken: Shyness, pragmatic language, and socioemotional adjustment in early childhood. Infant and Child Development,18(3), 238–254. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dardier, V., Bernicot, J., Delanoë, A., Vanberten, M., Fayada, C., Chevignard, M., et al. (2011). Severe traumatic brain injury, frontal lesions, and social aspects of language use: A study of French-speaking adults. Journal of Communication Disorders,44(3), 359–378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.02.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology,64(1), 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, J. M. (2010). Relations of executive functioning to pragmatic outcome following severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research,53(2), 365–382. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0205).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, D. M., & Dunn, L. M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test (4th ed.). Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durrleman, S., & Franck, J. (2015). Exploring links between language and cognition in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Complement sentences, false belief, and executive functioning. Journal of Communication Disorders,54, 15–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.12.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emerich, D. M., Creaghead, N. A., Grether, S. M., Murray, D., & Grasha, C. (2003). The comprehension of humorous materials by adolescents with high-functioning Autism and Asperger’s Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,33, 253–257. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024498232284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. E. (1995). Understanding minds and metaphors: Insight from the study of figurative language in Autism. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity,10(4), 275–295. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327868ms1004_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression based approach (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. L. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in Autism. Developmental Review,24(2), 189–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2004.01.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning adults with Autism with and without early language delays: Implications for the differentiation between Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,33(1), 3–13. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022270118899.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, Y. (2017). Introduction: What is pragmatics? In Y. Huang (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of pragmatics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphries, T., Koltun, H., Malone, M., & Roberts, W. (1994). Teacher-identified oral language difficulties among boys with attention problems. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,15(2), 92–98. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004703-199404000-00005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development,77(6), 1822–1841. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00976.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, R. M., McGrath, L. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005). Executive dysfunction and its relation to language ability in verbal school-age children with Autism. Developmental Neuropsychology,27(3), 361–378. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2703_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, L. J., Willoughby, M. T., Wilbourn, M. P., Vernon-Feagans, L., & Blair, C. B. (2014). Early communicative gestures prospectively predict language development and executive function in early childhood. Child Development,85(5), 1898–1914. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuusikko, S., Haapsamo, H., Jansson-Verkasalo, E., Hurtig, T., Mattila, M. L., Ebeling, H., et al. (2009). Emotion recognition in children and adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,39, 938–945. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0700-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. B., Song, S. H., Ham, J. H., Song, D. H., & Cheon, K. A. (2015). Idiom comprehension deficits in high-functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder using a Korean Autism social language task. Yonsei Medical Journal,56(6), 1613–1618. https://doi.org/10.3349/ymj.2015.56.6.1613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. B., Ellis Weismer, S., Miller, C. A., Francis, D. J., Tomblin, J. B., & Kail, R. V. (2007). Speed of processing, working Memory, and language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,50(2), 408–428. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/029).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., et al. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,19(2), 185–212. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02211841.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,24(5), 659–685. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loukusa, S., Mäkinen, L., Kuusikko-Gauffin, S., Ebeling, H., & Moilanen, I. (2014). Theory of mind and emotion recognition skills in children with specific language impairment, Autism Spectrum Disorder and typical development: Group differences and connection to knowledge of grammatical morphology, word-finding abilities and verbal working memory. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,49(4), 498–507. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12091.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, D., Biney, H., & Abbot-Smith, K. (2018). Individual differences in children’s pragmatic ability: A review of associations with formal language, social cognition, and executive functions. Language Learning and Development,14(3), 186–223. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2018.1455584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDonald, S. (2000). Exploring the cognitive basis of right-hemisphere pragmatic language disorders. Brain and Language,75(1), 82–107. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2000.2342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKown, C. (2007). Concurrent validity and clinical usefulness of several individually administered tests of children’s social–emotional cognition. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology,36(1), 29–41. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410709336566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McTear, M., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (1991). Pragmatic disorders in children: Assessment and intervention. London: Whurr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development,78(2), 622–646. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Muenz, L. R., Goldstein, G., & Payton, J. B. (1992). Neuropsychological functioning in nonmentally retarded autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,14(5), 749–761. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688639208402860.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nilsen, E., & Graham, S. (2009). The relations between children’s communicative perspective-taking and executive functioning. Cognitive Psychology,58(2), 220–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.07.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C. F., Nash, M., Baird, G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004). Using a parental checklist to identify diagnostic groups in children with communication impairment: A validation of the Children’s Communication Checklist—2. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,39(3), 345–364. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820410001654883.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S. (2001). Advances in the cognitive neuroscience of Autism. In C. A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 537–548). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Cook, I., Coon, H., Dawson, G., Joseph, R. M., Klin, A., et al. (2004). Performance on Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery subtests sensitive to frontal lobe function in people with Autistic Disorder: Evidence from the collaborative programs of excellence in Autism network. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,34(2), 139–150. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:jadd.0000022605.81989.cc.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E. (2010). The development of core cognitive skills in Autism: A 3-year prospective study. Child Development,81(5), 1400–1416. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01481.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, I. T., Bates, J. E., & Staples, A. D. (2015). The role of language ability and self-regulation in the development of inattentive–hyperactive behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology,27(1), 221–237. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prior, M., & Hoffmann, W. (1990). Brief report: Neuropsychological testing of autistic children through an exploration with frontal lobe tests. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,20(4), 581–590. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02216063.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prutting, C. A., & Kirchner, D. M. (1987). A clinical appraisal of the pragmatic aspects of language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,52(2), 105–119. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5202.105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rajendran, G., & Mitchell, P. (2007). Cognitive theories of Autism. Developmental Review,27(2), 224–260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.02.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C. (1995). Manual for the Coloured Progressive Matrices (Revised). Windsor: NFRE-Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reisinger, L. M. (2011). Exploring the behavioral and cognitive profile of children with Autism and children with Pragmatic Language Impairment. (Doctoral Dissertation), McGill University.

  • Ren, X., Wang, T., & Jarrold, C. (2016). Individual differences in frequency of inner speech: Differential relations with cognitive and non-cognitive factors. Frontiers in Psychology,7, 1765. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01675.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rundblad, G., & Annaz, D. (2010). The atypical development of metaphor and metonymy comprehension in children with Autism. Autism,14(1), 29–46. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361309340667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryskin, R. A., Benjamin, A. S., Tullis, J., & Brown-Schmidt, S. (2015). Perspective-taking in comprehension, production, and memory: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,144(5), 898–915. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000093.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schalock, R. L. (1996). Reconsidering the conceptualization and measurement of quality of life. In R. L. Schalock (Ed.), Quality of Life: Conceptualization and measurement (Vol. 1, pp. 123–129). Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Retardation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmid, H. J. (2012). Generalizing the apparently ungeneralizable. Basic ingredients of a cognitive-pragmatic approach to the construal of meaning-in-context. In H. J. Schmid (Ed.), Cognitive pragmatics (pp. 3–22). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schuh, J. M. (2012). Pragmatic language abilities: Working memory influences on mutual information. (Doctoral Dissertation), University of Connecticut.

  • Simões, M. (2000). Investigações no âmbito da aferição nacional do Teste das Matrizes Progressivas Coloridas de Raven (M.P.C.R). Lisboa: Fundação Calouste Gulbenkian & Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia.

  • Singer, M., & Lea, R. B. (2012). Inference and reasoning in discourse comprehension. In H. J. Schmid (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive pragmatics (pp. 85–119). Paris: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology,38(6), 934–947. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Troia, G. A. (2011). How might pragmatic language skills affect the written expression of students with language learning disabilities? Topics in Language Disorders,31(1), 40–53. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0b013e31820a0b71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • United Nations Educational, Scientific, and Cultural Organization (UNESCO). (2004). The plurality of literacy and its implications for policies and programmes. UNESCO Education Sector Position Paper. Paris: UNESCO.

  • Vicente, S. G., Sousa, A. S., & Silva, M. A. (2011). European Portuguese Material for Receptive Vocabulary Assessment [working research version constructed based on the PPVT-4 (Form B) of Dunn, & Dunn, 2007]. Unpublished Material, Centre of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal.

  • Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with Autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain,129(4), 932–943. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ying Sng, C., Carter, M., & Stephenson, J. (2018). A systematic review of the comparative pragmatic differences in conversational skills of individuals with Autism. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments,3, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941518803806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D. (1998). Cognitive complexity and control: II. The development of executive function in childhood. Psychological Science,7(4), 121–126. https://doi.org/10.2307/20182520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., Marcovitch, S., Argitis, G., Boseovski, J., et al. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,68(3), 151.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (SFRH/BD/64166/2009, BPD/100696/2014, PEst-OE/LIN/UI0214/2013).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marisa G. Filipe.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors report no declarations of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Filipe, M.G., Veloso, A., Frota, S. et al. Executive functions and pragmatics in children with high-functioning autism. Read Writ 33, 859–875 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09975-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09975-2

Keywords

Navigation