Skip to main content
Log in

Radical sensitivity is the key to understanding Chinese character acquisition in children

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated Chinese children’s development of sensitivity to positional (orthographic), phonological, and semantic cues of radicals in encoding novel Chinese characters. A newly designed picture-novel character mapping task, along with nonverbal reasoning ability, vocabulary, and Chinese character recognition were administered to 198 kindergartners, 172 second graders and 165 fifth graders. Children’s strategies in using positional, phonological, and semantic cues of radicals varied across grades. The higher the children’s grade level, the more commonly children used semantic and positional cues of radicals. Regression analyses showed that the contribution of semantic radical awareness for explaining Chinese character reading increased as children’s grade increased, whereas the contribution of positional regularity awareness decreased. These findings suggest that learning Chinese characters involves a transition from a sound- and position-based approach to a meaning-based approach.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Semantic-base and phonetic-base are mutually exclusively. If children choose to use a semantic radical, we can predict that they are not using the phonetic radical. Thus, we can use either the semantic-base or phonetic-base as dependent variables here, but not both.

References

  • Cheng, C. M., & Huang, H. M. (1995). The acquisition of general lexical knowledge of Chinese characters in school children. In 7th international conference on the cognitive processing of Chinese and other Asian languages, Hong Kong.

  • Cheung, H., Chan, M., & Chong, K. (2007). Use of orthographic knowledge in reading Chinese-English bi-scriptal children. Language Learning, 57(3), 469–505. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2007.00423.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, H., & Ng, K.-H. (2003). Chinese reading development in some major Chinese societies: An introduction. In C. McBride-Chang & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 3–17). London: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, F. H. K., & Leung, M. T. (2008). Data analysis of Chinese characters in primary school corpora of Hong Kong and Mainland China: Preliminary theoretical interpretations. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 22(4–5), 379–389. doi:10.1080/02699200701776757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100(4), 589–608. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.100.4.589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1989). The development of spelling knowledge and its role in reading acquisition and reading disability. Journal of learning disabilities, 22(6), 356–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (1999). Phases of development in learning to read words. In J. Oakhill, & R. Beard (Eds.), Reading development and the teaching of reading: A psychological perspective (pp. 79–108). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(2), 167–188. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, K. Y., Gao, J. Y., & Ao, X. P. (1984). Pronunciation principles of the Chinese character and alphabetic writing scripts. Chinese character reform, 3, 23–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S. H., Chan, D. W.-O., Tsang, S. M., & Lee, S. H. (2000). The Hong Kong Test of Specific Learning Difficulties in Reading and Writing. Hong Kong: Hong Kong Specific Learning Difficulties Research Team.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S. H. H., Ho, T. T., & Ng, W. K. (2003a). A “radical” approach to reading development in Chinese: The role of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals. Journal of Literacy Research, 35(3), 849–878. doi:10.1207/s15548430jlr3503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Yau, P. W.-Y., & Au, A. (2003b). Development of orthographic knowledge and its relationship with reading and spelling among Chinese kindergarten and primary school children. In C. McBride-Chang & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children. Westport, CT: Greenwood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hong Kong Census and Statistics Department. (2006). 2006 population by-census. Hong Kong: Hong Kong Special Administrative Region Government.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ko, H., & Wu, C. F. (2003). The role of character components in reading Chinese. In C. McBride-Chang & H. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 3–17). London: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lui, H. M., Leung, M. T., Law, S. P., & Fung, R. S. Y. (2010). A database for investigating the logographeme as a basic unit of writing Chinese. International journal of speech-language pathology, 12(1), 8–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J. R., Liu, H., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., Zhou, A., et al. (2005). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92(2), 140–160. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.03.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. S. H. (2000). Developmental issues in Chinese children’s character acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(1), 50–55. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.92.1.50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Manis, F. R. (1996). Structural invariance in the associations of naming speed, phonological awareness, and verbal reasoning in good and poor readers: A test of the double deficit hypothesis. Reading and Writing, 8(4), 323–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C., Cao, F., & Booth, J. (2013). Specialization and universals in the development of reading skill: How Chinese research informs a universal science of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(1), 5–21. doi:10.1080/10888438.2012.689786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1995). Raven’s colored progressive matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96(4), 523–568. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.96.4.523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2008). Orthographic learning, phonological recoding, and self-teaching. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 36, 31–82. doi:10.1016/s0065-2407(08)00002-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shen, H. H., & Bear, D. R. (2000). Development of orthographic skills in Chinese children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13(3–4), 197–236. doi:10.1023/a:1026484207650.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., & Anderson, R. C. (1997). Role of radical awareness in the character and word acquisition of Chinese children. Reading Research Quarterly, 32(1), 78–89. doi:10.2307/748144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., & Anderson, R. C. (1999). Learning to read Chinese: The development of metalinguistic awareness. In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 1–18). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implications for learning to read. Child development, 74(1), 27–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., & Zhu, X. (1997). Using masked priming to examine lexical storage of Chinese compound words. In H.-C. Chen (Ed.), The cognitive processing of Chinese and related Asian languages (pp. 233–241). Hong Kong: Chinese Univ. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tong, X., & McBride-Chang, C. (2014). Chinese children’s statistical learning of orthographic regularities: Positional constraints and character structure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(4), 291–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Barker, T. A. (1994). The development of orthographic processing ability. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge. I: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 243–276). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Yin, B., & Rohsenow, J. S. (1994). Modern Chinese characters. Beijing: Sinolingua.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1999). Phonology, orthography, and semantic activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Memory and Language, 41(4), 579–606. doi:10.1006/jmla.1999.2663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was presented at the Fifteenth Annual Meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading and it was awarded by the Scientific Studies of Reading Society as L. Sandak Young Investigator Award. We thank the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading. It was also supported in part by Collaborative Research Fund of the Hong Kong Special Administrative Region Research Grants Council (CUHK8/CRF/13G) to Catherine McBride. We are also very grateful to schools, parents, and children for their participation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Xiuhong Tong or Xiuli Tong.

Appendix

Appendix

Items in the picture-novel character mapping task and the radical properties of the characters

 

Items

Phonetic radical

Semantic radical

1.

/kei4/

其 /kei4/

2.

/faan6/

反 /faan2/

3.

/ho4/

可 /ho2/

4.

/ngaan5/

艮 /gan3/, /ngan4/

5.

/pun4/

分 /fan1/

6.

/biu1/

表 /biu2/

7.

/coeng1/

囱 /cung1/

8.

/kei4/

其 /kei4/

9.

/mou6/

冒 /mou6/

10.

/fu2/

几 /gei1/, /gei2/

11.

/lou6/

各 /gok3/

12.

/aap3/

甲 /gaap3/

13.

/wan4/

云 /wan4/

14.

/dang1/

登 /dang1/

15.

/syun4/

Unpronounceable

16.

/cong4/

木 /muk6/

17.

/wun2/

宛 /wun2/

18.

/lim5/

僉 /cim1/

19.

/zi2/

氏 /si6/, /zi1/

20.

/haa4/

Unpronounceable

21.

/coeng4/

嗇 /sik1/

22.

/zeoi2/

觜 /zeoi2/,/zi1/

23.

/tau4/

豆 /dau6/

24.

/gaa1/

豕 /ci2/

25.

/fu3/

庫 /fu3/

26.

/zin3/

前 /cin4/

27.

/tong4/

唐 /tong4/

28.

/paau3/

勺 /zoek3/

29.

/kam4/

今 /gam1/

30.

/haai4/

圭 /gwai1/

31.

/taan2/

炎 /jim4/

32.

/kau4/

求 /kau4/

33.

/min6/

面 /min6/

34.

/go1/

哥 /go1/

35.

/gong1/

工 /gung1/

36.

/king4/

京 /ging1/

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tong, X., Tong, X. & McBride, C. Radical sensitivity is the key to understanding Chinese character acquisition in children. Read Writ 30, 1251–1265 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9722-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9722-8

Keywords

Navigation