Skip to main content
Log in

Why do deaf participants have a lower performance than hearing participants in a visual rhyming task: a phonological hypothesis

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

During a visual rhyming task, deaf participants traditionally perform more poorly than hearing participants in making rhyme judgements for written words in which the rhyme and the spelling pattern are incongruent (e.g. hair/bear). It has been suggested that deaf participants’ low accuracy results from their tendency to rely on orthographic similarity. To test this interpretation more directly, we compared profoundly and prelingually deaf, orally educated participants and hearing participants’ accuracy during a visual rhyming judgement task in which the two words of a pair share the orthographic rime, in order to discourage usage of a purely orthographic strategy. Accuracy was lower in deaf than in hearing participants. The gradient of difficulty between items, together with the finding of a significant correlation between accuracy and the consistency of the grapheme to rhyme, suggest that difference in accuracy between groups might be explained by an over regularization in deaf people, which is probably linked to less diversified phonological representations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alegria, J., Charlier, B. L., & Mattys, S. (1999). The role of lip-reading and cued speech in the processing of phonological information in French-educated deaf children. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11(4), 451–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alegria, J., & Mousty, P. (1996). The development of spelling procedures in French-Speaking, normal and reading-disabled children: Effects of frequency and lexicality. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 312–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, T. E., Clark, M. D., Del Giudice, A., Koo, D., Lieberman, A., Mayberry, R., et al. (2009). Phonology and reading: A response to Wang, Trezek, Luckner and Paul. American Annals of the Deaf, 154(4), 338–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aparicio, M., Gounot, D., Demont, E., & Metz-Lutz, M. N. (2007). Phonological processing in relation to reading: An fMRI study in deaf readers. Neuroimage, 35(3), 1303–1316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2013). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.Rpackageversion0.999999-0.

  • Bonnotte, I., & Casalis, S. (2010). Semantic priming in French children with varying comprehension skills. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(3), 309–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read—a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M. (2007). The language-as-fixed-effect-fallacy: Some simple SPSS solutions to a complex problem. London: Royal Holloway, University of London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bus, A. G., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental training studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R., & Wright, H. (1988). Deafness, spelling and rhyme: how spelling supports written word and picture rhyming skills in deaf subjects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 40(4), 771–788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charlier, B. L., & Leybaert, J. (2000). The rhyming skills of deaf children educated with phonetically augmented speech reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 53(2), 349–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior, 12(4), 335–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97(3), 332–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colin, S., Magnan, A., Ecalle, J., & Leybaert, J. (2007). Relation between deaf children’s phonological skills in kindergarten and word recognition performance in first grade. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(2), 139–146. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01700.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, R. (1972). Short-term memory in the deaf: A test for speech coding. British Journal of Psychology, 63(2), 173–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, R. (1979). The deaf schoolchild: Language and cognitive function. London: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodd, B., & Hermelin, B. (1977). Phonological coding by the prelinguistically deaf. Perception and Psychophysics, 21(5), 413–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domínguez, A.-B., & Alegria, J. (2010). Reading mechanisms in orally educated deaf adults. Journal of deaf studies and deaf education, 15(2), 136–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ecalle, J. (2010). L’évaluation de la lecture et des compétences associées (pp. 105–120). XV: Revue Française de linguistique appliquée.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1995). Phases of development in learning to read words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 18(2), 116–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C. (1996). Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A review and a hypothesis. Reading and Writing, 8(6), 453–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grubb, J. D., Bush, A. M., & Geist, C. R. (1998). Effects of second language study of phonemic discrimination and auditory event-related potentials in adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87(2), 447–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, V. L., & Fowler, C. A. (1987). Phonological coding in word reading: Evidence from hearing and deaf readers. Memory and Cognition, 15(3), 199–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M., & Beech, J. (1998). Implicit phonological awareness and early reading development in prelingually deaf children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 3(3), 205–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izzo, A. (2002). Phonemic awareness and reading ability: An investigation with young readers who are deaf. American Annals of the Deaf, 147(4), 18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kourovsky, F., & Rennes, P. (2000). Test D2000. Paris: Les Éditions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lete, B., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Cole, P. (2004). MANULEX: A grade-level lexical database from French elementary school readers. Behaviour Research Methods, 36(1), 156–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leybaert, J. (2000). Phonology acquired through the eyes and spelling in deaf children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75(4), 291–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leybaert, J., & Alegria, J. (1993). Is word processing involuntary in deaf children? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(1), 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leybaert, J., & Alegria, J. (1995). Spelling development in deaf and hearing children: Evidence for use of morpho-phonological regularities in French. Reading and Writing, 7(1), 89–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leybaert, J., & D’Hondt, M. (2003). Neurolinguistic development in deaf children: The effect of early language experience. International Journal of Audiology, 42(Suppl 1), S34–S40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lobrot, M. (1973). Lire. Paris: ESF.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locker, L., Hoffman, L., & Bovaird, J. A. (2007). On the use of multilevel modeling as an alternative to items analysis in psycholinguistic research. Behavior Research Methods, 39(4), 723–730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacSweeney, M., Brammer, M. J., Waters, D., & Goswami, U. (2009). Enhanced activation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in deaf and dyslexic adults during rhyming. Brain, 132(7), 1928–1940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marschark, M., & Harris, M. (1996). Success and failure in learning to read: The special case (?) of deaf children. In C. Cornoldi & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties: Processes and intervention (pp. 279–300). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayberry, R. I., del Giudice, A. A., & Lieberman, A. M. (2011). Reading achievement in relation to phonological coding and awareness in deaf readers: A meta-analysis. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16(2), 164–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, C. (2007). What really matters in the early literacy development of deaf children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12(4), 411–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. (2007). The role of phonology in the word decoding skills of poor readers: Evidence from individuals with prelingual deafness or diagnosed dyslexia. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 19(4), 385–408. doi:10.1007/s10882-007-9057-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morais, J., Alegria, J., & Content, A. (1987). Segmental awareness: Respectable, useful, and almost always necessary. Current Psychology of Cognition/Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 7(5), 530–556.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mousty, P., & Leybaert, J. (1999). Etude longitudinale du développement de la lecture et de l’orthographe grâce à la batterie BELEC. Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée, 49(4), 325–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Musselman, C. (2000). How do children who can’t hear learn to read an alphabetic script? A review of the literature on reading and deafness. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5(1), 9–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the national reading panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health.

  • Paul, P. V., Wang, Y., Trezek, B. J., & Luckner, J. L. (2009). Phonology is necessary, but not sufficient: A rejoinder. American Annals of the Deaf, 154(4), 346–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C., & Sandak, R. (2000). Reading optimally builds on spoken language: Implications for deaf readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5(1), 32–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polka, L. (1991). Cross-language speech perception in adults: Phonemic, phonetic, and acoustic contributions. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89(6), 2961–2977.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Development Core Team. (2008). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0. R Foundation Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org.

  • Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2003). A further look at the” language-as-fixed-effect fallacy”. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seigneuric, A., & Ehrlich, M. F. (2005). Contribution of working memory capacity to children’s reading comprehension: A longitudinal investigation. Reading and Writing, 18(7), 617–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72(2), 95–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterne, A., & Goswami, U. (2000). Phonological awareness of syllables, rhymes, and phonemes in deaf children. The Journal of Child and Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(5), 609–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Reybroeck, M., & Hupet, M. (2009). Effects of various processing demands on the acquisition of number agreement in written French. Journal of the Writing Research, 1, 153–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Y., Trezek, B. J., Luckner, J. L., & Paul, P. V. (2008). The role of phonology and phonologically related skills in reading instruction for students who are deaf or hard of hearing. American Annals of the Deaf, 153(4), 396–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all participants and particularly to deaf participants in this process. We wish also to thank the two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on the previous version of the manuscript. We also thank Carol LaSasso for her assistance with our English writing. We thank Alain Content for his assistance with the statistical procedure.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mario Aparicio.

Appendix

Appendix

List of word-pairs of the rhyming judgment task

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Aparicio, M., Demont, E., Metz-Lutz, MN. et al. Why do deaf participants have a lower performance than hearing participants in a visual rhyming task: a phonological hypothesis. Read Writ 27, 31–54 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9432-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9432-9

Keywords

Navigation