Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The role of invented spelling on learning to read in low-phoneme awareness kindergartners: a randomized-control-trial study

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The goal of the present intervention research was to test whether guided invented spelling would facilitate entry into reading for at-risk kindergarten children. The 56 participating children had poor phoneme awareness, and as such, were at risk of having difficulty acquiring reading skills. Children were randomly assigned to one of three training conditions: invented spelling, phoneme segmentation, or storybook reading. All children participated in 16 small group sessions over 8 weeks. In addition, children in the three training conditions received letter-knowledge training and worked on the same 40 stimulus words that were created from an array of 14 letters. The findings were clear: on pretest, there were no differences between the three conditions on measures of early literacy and vocabulary, but, after training, invented spelling children learned to read more words than did the other children. As expected, the phoneme-segmentation and invented-spelling children were better on phoneme awareness than were the storybook-reading children. Most interesting, however, both the invented spelling and the phoneme-segmentation children performed similarly on phoneme awareness suggesting that the differential effect on learning to read was not due to phoneme awareness per se. As such, the findings support the view that invented spelling is an exploratory process that involves the integration of phoneme and orthographic representations. With guidance and developmentally appropriate feedback, invented spelling provides a milieu for children to explore the relation between oral language and written symbols that can facilitate their entry in reading.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ball, E. W., & Bachman, B. A. (1991). Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and developmental spelling. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 49–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bus, A. G., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental training studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 403–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). The foundations of spelling ability: Evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 751–774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., Volín, J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children. Journal Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 107–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, L. K. (1988). Invented versus traditional spelling in first graders’ writings: Effects on learning to spell and read. Research in the Teaching of English, 22, 281–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). The peabody picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (1989). Development of spelling knowledge and its role in reading acquisition and reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 356–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phoneme awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1987). Does learning to spell help beginners learn to read words? Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 47–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elkonin, D. B. (1973). U.S.S.R. In I. Downing (Ed.), Comparative reading: Cross national studies of behavior and processes in reading and writing (pp. 551–580). New York, NY: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferreiro, E., & Teberosky, A. (1982). Literacy before schooling. Exter, NH: Heineman education Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. Patterson, M. Coltheart, & J. Marshall (Eds.), Surface dyslexia, neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301–330). London, GB: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentry, J. R., & Gillet, J. W. (1993). Teaching kids to spell. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, E. H. (1981). Learning to read and spell: A child’s knowledge of words. DeKalb, IL: N. Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hohn, W., & Ehri, L. (1983). Do alphabet letters help prereaders acquire phoneme segmentation skill? Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 752–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 443–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lever, R., & Sénéchal, M. (2011). Discussing stories: How a dialogic reading intervention improves kindergarteners’ oral narrative construction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martins, M. A., & Silva, C. (2006). The impact of invented spelling on phoneme awareness. Learning-and-Instruction, 16, 41–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1998). The development of invented spelling. Early Education and Development, 9, 147–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, D., & Perney, J. (1984). Developmental spelling as a predictor of first grade reading achievement. The Elementary School Journal, 84, 440–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouellette, G., & Sénéchal, M. (2008a). Pathways to literacy: A study of invented spelling and its role in learning to read. Child Development, 79, 899–913.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouellette, G., & Sénéchal, M. (2008b). A window into early literacy: Exploring the cognitive and linguistic underpinnings of invented spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189–213). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Read, C. (1971). Pre-school children’s knowledge of English phonology. Harvard Educational Review, 41, 1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richgels, D. J. (1995). Invented spelling ability and printed word learning in kindergarten. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 96–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieben, L., Ntamakiliro, L., Gonthier, B., & Fayol, M. (2005). Effects of various early writing practices on reading and spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 145–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some promising predictors. In B. K. Shapiro, P. J. Pasquale, & A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75–119). Timonium, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schulz, K. F., Altman, D. G., & Moher, D. (2010). CONSORT 2010 Statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. Annals of Internal Medicine, 152, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M. (2006). Testing the home literacy model: Parent involvement in kindergarten is differentially related to grade 4 reading comprehension, fluency, spelling, and reading for pleasure. Journal for the Scientific Study of Reading, 10, 59–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological coding and self-teaching: Sine-qua-non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shatil, E., Share, D. L., & Levin, I. (2000). On the contribution of kindergarten writing to grade 1 literacy: A longitudinal study in Hebrew. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silva, C., & Martins, M. A. (2003). Relations between children’s invented spelling and the development of phonological awareness. Educational Psychology, 23, 3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sirois, P., Boisclair, A., & Giasson, J. (2008). Understanding of the alphabetic principle through invented spelling among hearing-impaired children learning to read and write: Experimentation with a pedagogical approach. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 339–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tangel, D. M., & Blachman, B. A. (1992). Effect of phoneme awareness instruction on kindergarten children’s invented spelling. Journal of Reading Behavior, 24, 233–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weschler, D. (1989). Weschler preschool and primary scale of intelligence—revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1998). Woodcock reading mastery tests—revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Monique Sénéchal.

Appendix: Stimuli

Appendix: Stimuli

Letters taught in the interventions

A, B, D, E, I, L, N, O, P, R, S, T, U, Y.

Words used in the interventions

Sessions 1 and 2

ape

eel

on

in

no

Sessions 3 and 4

day

see

pie

add

toe

Sessions 5 and 6

rod

lap

pen

bib

sun

Sessions 7 and 8

sad

rip

nut

rat

net

Sessions 9 and 10

soap

nail

line

pole

seal

Sessions 11 and 12

rain

toad

date

rope

bite

Sessions 13 and 14

lady

ladder

apple

puppy

panda

Sessions 15 and 16

nest

spider

star

bird

snail

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sénéchal, M., Ouellette, G., Pagan, S. et al. The role of invented spelling on learning to read in low-phoneme awareness kindergartners: a randomized-control-trial study. Read Writ 25, 917–934 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9310-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9310-2

Keywords

Navigation