Skip to main content
Log in

Gender ratio and cognitive profiles in dyslexia: a cross-national study

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to analyze possible gender-related differences in the prevalence of dyslexia. A cross-national comparison of Spain and Guatemala was conducted. Both countries speak the same language but have a different standard of living and educational level. A second purpose of this study was to analyze the cognitive profile of Guatemalan and Spanish males and females children with dyslexia. The log-linear analysis indicated that the number of dyslexics detected was different across the countries but there were no differences as a function of gender. Similarly, there were no significant or meaningful differences between dyslexic males and females in the cognitive processes involved in reading. Therefore, gender differences do not appear to be characteristic of developmental dyslexia.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Artiles, A. J., & Pianta, R. C. (1993). Winds of change in Guatemala’s education system. International Journal of Special Education, 8, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bednarek, D., Saldaña, D., & García, I. (2009). Visual versus phonological abilities in Spanish dyslexic boys and girls. Brain and Cognition, 70, 273–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beier, J. I., Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M., Castillo, E. M., Zhang, W., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2003). Abnormal activation of temporoparietal language areas during phonetic analysis in children with dyslexia. Neuropsychology, 17, 610–621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Nielsen, K. H., Abbott, R. D., Wijsman, E., & Raskind, W. (2008). Gender differences in severity of writing and reading disabilities. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 151–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bravo-Valdivieso, L. (2001). Learning disabilities studies in South America. In D. P. Hallahan & B. K. Keogh (Eds.), Research and global perspectives in learning disabilities (pp. 311–328). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B., & Cattell, A. K. S. (1989). Test de Factor g. Escala 1 y 2. (Cordero, De la Cruz, & Seisdedos, Trans.). Madrid: T. E. A. Ediciones. (Original work published in 1950).

  • Chan, D. W., Ho, C. S. H., Tsang, S. M., Lee, S. H., & Chung, K. K. H. (2007). Prevalence, gender ratio and gender differences in reading-related cognitive abilities among Chinese children with dyslexia in Hong Kong. Educational Studies, 33, 249–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiappe, P., Siegel, L. S., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2002). Linguistic diversity and the development of reading skills: A longitudinal study. Scientific Study of Reading, 6, 369–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coope, C. M., & Theobald, S. (2006). Children at risk of neglect: Challenges faced by child protection practitioners in Guatemala City. Child Abuse and Neglect, 30, 523–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dancer, L. S., Anderson, A. J., & Derlin, R. L. (1994). Use of log-linear models for assessing differential item functioning in a measure of psychological functioning. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 710–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeFries, J. C. (1989). Gender ratios in children with reading disability and their affected relatives: A commentary. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 544–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters, and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Federal Register (1977, August 23). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

  • Flannery, K. A., Liederman, J., Daly, L., & Schultz, J. (2000). Male prevalence for reading disability is found in a large sample of black and white children free from ascertainment bias. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 433–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N. (1981). A reaction to the conference on sex differences in dislexia. In A. Ansara, N. Geschwind, A. Galaburda, M. Albert, M. Albert, & N. Gartrell (Eds.), Sex differences in dislexia (pp. 13–18). Towson, MD: Orton Dyslexia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Instituto Nacional de Estadística de Guatemala. (2003). Censo poblacional. SEN: Guatemala.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez, J. E., Antón, L., Diaz, A., Estevez, A., García, A. I., Garcia, E., et al. (2007). Sicole-R: Un sistema de evaluación de los procesos cognitivos en la dislexia mediante ayuda asistida a través del ordenador [Software informático]. Universidad de La Laguna: Authors.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez, J. E., Guzmán, R., Rodríguez, C., & Artiles, C. (2009). Prevalencia de las dificultades específicas de aprendizaje: la dislexia en español. Anales de Psicología, 25, 78–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez, J. E., & Hernández-Valle, I. (1999). A Spanish perspective on learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 267–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez, J. E., & Rodrigo, M. (1994). Is it true that the differences in reading performance between students with and without LD cannot be explained by IQ? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27(3), 155–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez, J. E., Siegel, L. S., O’Shanahan, I., & Ford, L. (2008). The relatives role of IQ and cognitive processes in reading disability. Educational Psychology, 29, 27–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katusic, S. K., Colligan, R. C., Barbaresi, W. J., Schaid, D. J., & Jacobsen, S. J. (2001). Incidence of reading disability in a population-based birth cohort, 1976–1982, Rochester, Minn. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 76, 1075–1077.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liederman, J., Kantrowitz, L., & Flannery, K. (2005). Male vulnerability to reading disabilitiy is not likely to be a myth: A call for new data. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 109–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lubs, H., Rabin, M., Feldman, E., Jallad, B., Kushch, A., & Gross-Glenn, K. (1993). Familial dyslexia: Genetic and medical findings in eleven three-generation families. Annals of Dyslexia, 43, 45–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, M. J., Dyson, A., Nagle, K., Thurlow, M., Rouse, M., Hardman, M., et al. (2006). Cross-cultural perspectives on the classification of children with disabilities. Journal of Special Education, 40, 46–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, T. R., Haslum, M. N., & Wheeler, T. J. (1998). Gender ratio in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 27–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ministerio de Educación (MINEDUC). (2001). Política y normativa de acceso a la educación para la población con necesidades educativas especiales. Guatemala: Guatemalan Government.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Joint Committee on Learning Disabilities. (1994). Collective perspectives on issues affecting learning disabilities. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Caspi, A., Ferguson, D., Horwood, L. J., Goofman, R., Maughan, B., et al. (2004). Sex differences in developmental reading disability. JAMA, 291, 2007–2012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seisdedos, N., De la Cruz, M. V., Cordero, A., & González, M. (1991). Test de aptitudes escolares (TEA). Madrid: T. E. A. Ediciones.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Silva, P. A. (2003). Gender bias in IQ-discrepancy and post-discrepancy definitions of reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 4–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E. (1998). Dyslexia. The New England Journal of Medicine, 338, 307–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Escobar, M. D. (1990). Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls. Results of the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 264, 998–1002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. (1992). An evaluation of the discrepancy definition of dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 618–629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. (2003). Basic cognitive processes and reading disabilities. In H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 158–181). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S., & Himel, N. (1998). Socioeconomic status, age and the classification of dyslexics and poor readers: The dangers of using IQ scores in the definition of reading disability. Dyslexia, 4, 90–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L., & Ryan, E. B. (1989). The development of working memory in normally achieving and subtypes of learning disabled children. Child Development, 60, 973–980.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S., & Smythe, I. S. (2005). Reflections on research on reading disability with special attention to gender issues. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 473–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia. Oxford, U.K.: Blackwell Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences in individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., LeDoux, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2002). Validity of IQ-discrepancy classifications of reading disabilities: A meta-analysis. American Educational Research Journal, 39, 469–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tracey, T. J., Sherry, P., Bauer, G. P., Robins, T. H., Todaro, L., & Briggs, S. (1984). Helpseeking as a function of student characteristics and program description: A logit-loglinear analysis. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 31, 54–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • UNICEF. (2001a). Country note-Guatemala (Executive Borrad, first regular session). Guatemala: United Nations Children’s Fund.

    Google Scholar 

  • UNICEF (2001b). End decade review-Guatemala. United Nations Children’s Fund, unpublished data.

  • United Nations Development Programme. (2007). Human development report 2007/2008: Fighting climate change: Human solidarity in a divided world. New York: Hampshire.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, S. (1990). Gender differences in intelligence, language, visual-motor abilities, and academic achievement in students with learning disabilities. A review of the literature. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 44–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wadsworth, S. J., DeFries, J. C., Stevenson, J., Gilger, J. W., & Pennington, B. F. (1992). Gender ratios among reading-disabled children and their siblings as a function of parental impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 1229–1239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R., & Torgesen, J. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study has been funded by Plan Nacional I+D+i (Feder y Ministerio de Ciencia y Tecnología) SEJ2006-09156, Agencia Española de Cooperación con Iberoamérica (AECI, ref. A/3877/05), and Programa Ramón y Cajal (RyC-2009-04227). Some parts of the article were written while Juan E. Jiménez and Isabel O’Shanahan Juan were visiting the Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology and Special Education at the University of British Columbia of Vancouver, Canada, with the support from the Minister of Education in Spain through research grant PR2007-0395 and PR2007-0405, respectively.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Juan E. Jiménez.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Jiménez, J.E., de la Cadena, C.G., Siegel, L.S. et al. Gender ratio and cognitive profiles in dyslexia: a cross-national study. Read Writ 24, 729–747 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9222-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9222-6

Keywords

Navigation