Skip to main content
Log in

Importing set-theoretic tools into quantitative research: the case of necessary and sufficient conditions

  • Published:
Quality & Quantity Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this article, we import set-theoretic methods from the qualitative research tradition into the quantitative research tradition. We focus specifically on set-theoretic methods designed to analyze the extent to which a condition is necessary and is sufficient for an outcome of interest. We use these methods to reanalyze four major studies from the quantitative tradition. We find that set-theoretic methods call attention to asymmetrical patterns in the data that otherwise go unnoticed and unanalyzed. We develop a general set-theoretic framework for the study of necessity and sufficiency. We conclude that the use of this framework can enrich existing and future quantitative research in the social sciences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For basic statistics on smoking and lung cancer, see Godtfredsen et al. (2005) and Islami et al. (2015). For a short summary of the multimethod research program that established smoking to be a cause of lung cancer, see Freedman (1999).

  2. See Rubinson (2019) for discussion of different types of diagrams that can be used with QCA.

  3. Non-QCA approaches for analyzing necessary conditions and sufficient conditions do exist (e.g., Clark, Gilligan, and Golder 2006; Eliason and Stryker 2009; Goertz 2006; Seawright 2015), but they lack important aspects of the QCA apparatus, including its set-theoretic foundations.

  4. In the QCA literature, this consideration is addressed via the study of “unique coverage” (Ragin 2008).

  5. More generally, the negation of a condition is not equivalent to the opposite of a condition. For example, consider the condition of development and its opposite, underdevelopment. The condition of not-development is not equivalent to the condition of underdevelopment. Not all cases of not-development qualify as cases of underdevelopment.

  6. However, more than half of the subjects were willing to administer at least a “slight shock” on the basis of written instructions and in the absence of an authority figure (Milgram 1973: Table 2 on pp. 60–61).

  7. Conformity theory proposes that individuals in a group relinquish decision-making authority to experts, especially in crises. Agentic state theory proposes that subjects view themselves as agents carrying out the wishes of a legitimate authority figure and thus believe that they are not accountable for their actions.

References

  • Ahmed, A., Sil, R.: When multi-method research subverts methodological pluralism – or why we still need single-method research. Perspect. Polit. 10, 935–953 (2012)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berg-Schlosser, D., De Meur, G., Rihoux, B., Ragin, C.C. (eds.): Configuration Comparative Methods: Qualitative Comparative Analysis (QCA) and Related Techniques. Sage, London (2009)

    Google Scholar 

  • Blass, T. (ed.): Obedience to Authority: Current Perspective on the Milgram Experiments. Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers, London (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  • Blass, T.: From new haven to santa clara: a historical perspective on the milgram obedience experiments. Am. Psychol. 64, 37–45 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brady, H.E. and Collier, D., eds. 2010. Rethinking Social Inquiry: Diverse Tools, Shared Standards, second ed. Landham: Rowman and Littlefield.

  • Braumoeller, B., Goertz, G.: The Methodology of necessary conditions. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 44, 844–858 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, W.R., Gilligan, M.J., Golder, M.: A simple multivariate test for asymmetric hypotheses. Polit. Anal. 14(3), 311–331 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawid, A.P.: Causal inference without counterfactuals. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 95, 407–448 (2000)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deaton, A., Cartwright, N.: Understanding and misunderstanding randomized controlled trials. Soc. Sci. Med. 210, 2–21 (2018)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, T.: Natural Experiments in the Social Sciences. Cambridge University Press, New York (2012)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Eliason, S.R., Stryker, R.: Goodness-of-fit tests and descriptive measures in fuzzy-set analysis. Sociol. Methods Res. 38, 102–146 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiss, P.C., Sharapov, D., Cronqvist, L.: Opposites attract? Opportunities and challenges for integrating large-N QCA and econometric analysis. Polit. Res. q. 66, 191–198 (2013)

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D.A.: Statistical models and shoe leather. Sociol. Methodol. 21, 291–313 (1991)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D.A.: From association to causation: some remarks on the history of statistics. J. De La Société Fanҫaise De Stat. 140, 5–32 (1999)

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A.S., Green, D.P., and Larimer, C.W. : Social pressure and voter turnout: evidence from a large-scale field experiment. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 102: 33–48 (2008)

  • Godtfredsen, N.S., Prescott, E., Osler, M.: Effect of smoking reduction on lung cancer risk. JAMA 294, 1505–1510 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goertz, G.: Assessing the trivialness, relevance, and relative importance of necessary or sufficient conditions in social science. Stud. Comp. Int. Dev. 41, 88–109 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goertz, G., Mahoney, J.: A Tale of Two Cultures: Contrasting Qualitative and Quantitative Research. Princeton University Press, Princeton (2012)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Goertz, G.: Multimethod Research, Causal Mechanisms, and Case Studies: An Integrated Approach. Princeton University Press, Princeton (2017)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Haesebrouck, T. Thomann, E.: Introduction: Causation, inferences, and solution types in configurational comparative methods. Quality and Quantity (2021)

  • Hall, Ned: Two Concepts of Causation. Pp. 181–203 in Causation and Counterfactuals, edited by John Collins, Ned Hall, and L. A. Paul. Cambridge: MIT Press (2004)

  • Heckman, J.J.: Sample selection bias as a specification error. Econometrica 47, 153–161 (1979)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckman, J.J.: The scientific model of causality. Sociol. Methodol. 35, 1–97 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hempel, S.: The Strange Case of the Broad Street Pump: John Snow and the Mystery of Cholera. University of California Press, Berkeley (2007)

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, P.W.: Statistics and causal inference. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 81, 945–960 (1986)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, Macartan, and Alan Jacobs. Forthcoming. Integrated Inferences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Islami, F., Torre, L.A., Jemal, A.: Global trends of lung cancer mortality and smoking prevalance. Trans. Lung Cancer Res. 4, 327–338 (2015)

    Google Scholar 

  • King, G., Keohane, R.O., Verba, S.: Designing Social Inquiry: Scientific Inference in Qualitative Research. Princeton University Press, Princeton (1994)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsella, D.: No rest for the democratic peace. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 99, 453–457 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahoney, J., Acosta, L.: A Regularity Theory of Causality for the Social Sciences. Quality and Quantity (2021)

  • Mello, P.: Qualitative Comparative Analysis: An Introduction to Research Design and Application. Georgetown University Press, Georgetown (2021)

    Google Scholar 

  • Mikkelsen, K.S.: Fuzzy-set case studies. Sociol. Methods Res. 46, 422–455 (2017)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, S.: Obedience to Authority: An Experimental View. Harper and Row, New York (1973)

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, S.L., Winship, C.: Counterfactuals and Causal Inference: Methods and Principles for Social Research, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2015)

    Google Scholar 

  • Oana, I.-E., Schneider, C.Q., Thomann, E.: Qualitative Comparative Analysis Using R: A Beginner’s Guide. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2021)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pearl, J.: The Book of Why: The New Science of Cause and Effect. Basic Books, New York (2018)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ragin, C.C.: The Comparative Method: Moving Beyond Qualitative and Quantitative Strategies. University of California Press, Berkeley (1987)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ragin, C.C.: Fuzzy-Set Social Science. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ragin, C.C.: Redesigning Social Inquiry: Fuzzy Sets and Beyond. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (2008)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rihoux, B., Ragin, C.C. (eds.): Configurational Comparative Methods: Qualitative Comparative Analysis (QCA) and Related Techniques. Sage, Thousand Oaks (2008)

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohlfing, Ingo: Case studies and causal inference: an integrative approach. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillian (2012)

  • Rubin, D.B.: Estimating causal effects of treatments in randomized and nonrandomized studies. J. Educ. Psychol. 66, 688–701 (1974)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubinson, Claude: Presenting qualitative comparative analysis: notation, tabular layout, and visualization. Methodological Innovations May-August: 1–22 (2019)

  • Russett, B.: The democratic peace: ‘And Yet It Moves.’ Int. Secur. 19, 164–175 (1995)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, C.Q.: Idealists and realists in QCA. Polit. Anal. 26, 246–254 (2018)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, C.Q., Wagemann, C.: Set-Theoretic Methods for the Social Sciences: A Guide to Qualitative Comparative Analysis. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2012)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, C.Q., Rohlfing, I.: A Unifying framework for causal research in set-theoretic multi-method research. Sociol. Methods Res. 47, 37–63 (2018)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seawright, J.: Multi-Method Social Science. Cambridge University Press, New York (2015)

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, John. 1855/1965. On the mode of communication of cholera, 2nd Ed. London: John Churchill

  • Talmy, L.: Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cogn. Sci. 12, 49–100 (1988)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiem, A., Baumgartner, M., Bol, D.: Still lost in translation! misunderstandings between configurational comparativists and regression analysts. Comp. Pol. Stud. 49, 742–774 (2016)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vis, B.: The comparative advantages of fsQCA and regression analysis for moderately large-N analyses. Sociol. Methods Res. 41, 168–198 (2012)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weart, S.R.: Never at War: Why Democracies Will Not Fight Each Other. Yale University Press, New Haven (1998)

    Google Scholar 

  • Zadeh, L.A.: Fuzzy sets. Inf. Control 12, 338–353 (1965)

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Earlier versions of the this paper were presented at the 2020 Annual Meetings of the American Political Science Association and the 2020 Annual Meetings of the American Sociological Association. For helpful comments, we thank Gary Goertz, Ingo Rohlfing, and the anonymous reviewers at Quality and Quantity.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to James Mahoney.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

1.1 Calculating necessity and sufficiency with graded conditions

Procedures for assessing degrees of necessity and degrees of sufficiency with continuously coded graded conditions have been around since Zadeh (1965). Recent formulations for set-theoretic analysis include Ragin (2008: chap. 3) and Schneider and Wagemann (2012: chap. 3). With graded conditions, some cases have partial degrees of membership. For example, if the condition is developed country, some countries are only partly in the condition. With continuous-set measurement, analysts code cases continuously from 1.0 to 0.0 based on their degree of membership in the condition. Cases with a 1.0 membership value are full members; cases with a 0.0 membership value are full non-members. Cases with 0.5 membership are true crossover cases, half in and half out of the condition. Cases with membership values > 0.5 < 1.0 are more in a condition than out; cases with membership values > 0.0 < 0.5 are more out of a condition than in.

When cases are coded continuously in this way, the formulas for calculating the degree of necessity and degree of sufficiency are simply:

$$\% {\text{ }}\;{\text{Necessary}}\;{\text{ }}\left( {X \to Y} \right){\text{ }} = \;\Sigma \left[ {{\text{min}}X_{i} ,Y_{i} } \right]/{\text{ }}\Sigma \left( {X_{i} } \right);{\text{ and}}$$
$$\% {\text{ }}\;{\text{Sufficient }}\;\left( {X \to Y} \right){\text{ }} = \;\Sigma \left[ {{\text{min}}X_{i} ,Y_{i} } \right]/{\text{ }}\Sigma \left( {Y_{i} } \right).$$

It also possible to assess degrees of necessity and sufficiency with interval variables. The relevant techniques are developed in Goertz (2006) and Eliason and Stryker (2009).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mahoney, J., Owen, A. Importing set-theoretic tools into quantitative research: the case of necessary and sufficient conditions. Qual Quant 56, 2001–2022 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-021-01188-6

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-021-01188-6

Keywords

Navigation