Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The biological side of social determinants: Neural costs of childhood poverty

  • Open File
  • Published:
PROSPECTS Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Interdisciplinary efforts to foster the development and education of children living in poverty require a comprehensive concept of multiple dimensions, within a systemic approach involving ecological and transactional perspectives. Constructing a common interdisciplinary language dealing with child development in ecological terms is a necessary first step toward building networks that can guide us in designing and implementing comprehensive, coherent actions. In this context, studies of how social determinants influence brain development include critical and sensitive growth periods for different neural systems, modulation of brain development by epigenetics mechanisms, influences of environmental toxins, lack of adequate nutrition, and stress and self-regulatory mechanisms. This neuroscientific agenda pioneers these explorations concerning the elemental components that bear on different levels of organization. Ecological considerations about how poverty shapes child neurocognitive development and its biological and social determinants should identify different protective and risk factors—as well as mediation mechanisms—that could help us better understand poverty’s effects and should guide us in designing actions to optimize children’s emotional, cognitive, and learning development.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Figure 1

Note: “Mental capital” refers to the totality of an individual’s cognitive and emotional resources, including their cognitive capability, flexibility and efficiency of learning, emotional intelligence (e.g., empathy and social cognition), and resilience in the face of stress. The extent of an individual's resources reflects his/her basic endowment (genes and early biological programming), and their experiences and education, which take place throughout the lifecourse.

Figure 2

Source: Figure and captions are reproduced by permission from Foresight Mental Capital and Wellbeing Project (2008).

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Avants, B. B., Hackman, D. A., Betancourt, L. M., Lawson, G. M., Hurt, H., & Farah, M. J. (2015). Relation of childhood home environment to cortical thickness in late adolescence: Specificity of experience and timing. PLoS One, 10, e0138217. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0138217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to rearing environment depending on dopamine-related genes: New evidence and a meta-analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 39–52. doi:10.1017/S0954579410000635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bavelier, D., Levi, D. M., Li, R., Dan, Y., & Hensch, M. P. (2010). Removing brakes on adult brain plasticity: From molecular to behavioral interventions. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 14964–14971. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4812-10.2010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaver, K. M., Wright, J. P., DeLisi, M., & Vaughn, M. G. (2012). Dopaminergic polymorphisms and educational attainment: Results from a longitudinal sample of Americans. Developmental Psychology, 48, 932–938. doi:10.1037/a0026313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beddington, J., Cooper, C. L., Field, J., Goswami, U., Huppert, F. A., Jenkins, R., et al. (2008). The mental wealth of nations. Nature, International Weekly Journal of Science, 455, 1057–1060. doi:10.1038/4551057a.

    Google Scholar 

  • Betancourt, L. M., Avants, B., Farah, M. J., Brodsky, N. L., Wu, J., Ashtari, M., et al. (2015). Effect of socioeconomic status (SES) disparity on neural development in female African-American infants at age 1 month. Developmental Science. doi:10.1111/desc.12344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C. (2010). Stress and the development of self-regulation in context. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 181–188. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00145.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). Child development in the context of adversity: Experiential canalization of brain and behavior. American Psychologist, 67, 309–318. doi:10.1037/a0027493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2016). Poverty, stress, and brain development: New directions for prevention and intervention. Academic Pediatrics, 16, S30–S36. doi:10.1016/j.acap.2016.01.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., Granger, D. A., Willoughby, M., Mills-Koonce, R., Cox, M., Greenberg, M. T., et al. (2011). Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Development, 82, 1970–1984. doi:10.1111/j1467-8624.2011.01643.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Lansford, J. E., Deater-Deckard, K., & Bradley, R. H. (2015). A developmental analysis of caregiving modalities across infancy in 38 low- and middle-income countries. Child Development, 86, 1571–1587. doi:10.1111/cdev.12402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 371–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BDCG [Brain Development Cooperative Group] (2012). Total and regional brain volumes in a population-based sample from 4 to 18 years: The NIH MRI study of normal brain development. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 1–12. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brito, H. H., & Noble, K. G. (2014). Socioeconomic status and structural brain development. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 276. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brody, G. H., Dorsey, S., Forehand, R., & Armistead, L. (2002). Unique and protective contributions of parenting and classroom processes to the adjustment of African American children living in single-parent families. Child Development, 73, 274–286. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn, J., & Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on children. Future of Children, 7, 55–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Angiulli, A., Van Roon, P. M., Weinberg, J., Oberlander, T. F., Grunau, R. E., Hertzman, C., et al. (2012). Frontal EEG/ERP correlates of attentional processes, cortisol, and motivational states in adolescents from lower and higher socioeconomic status. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 306. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00306.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Angiulli, A., Lipina, S. J., & Olesinska, A. (2012). Explicit and implicit issues in the developmental cognitive neuroscience of social inequality. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 254. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demir, Ö. E., & Küntay, A. C. (2014). Cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying socioeconomic gradients in language development: New answers to old questions. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 113–118. doi:10.1111/cdep.12069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donald, K. A., Eastman, E., Howells, F. M., Adnams, C., Riley, E. P., Woods, R. P., et al. (2015). Neuroimaging effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on the developing human brain: A magnetic resonance imaging review. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 27, 251–269. doi:10.1017/neu.2015.12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dornan, P., & Woodhead, M. (2015). How inequalities develop through childhood: Life course evidence from the Young Lives cohort study. Discussion Paper 2015-01. Florence: UNICEF Office of Research.

  • Duncan, G. J., & Magnusson, K. (2012). Socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning: Moving from correlation to causation. Willey Interdisciplinary Reviews. Cognitive Science, 3, 377–386. doi:10.1002/wcs.1176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eamon Keegan, M. (2002). Effects of poverty on mathematics and reading achievement of young adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 22, 49–74. doi:10.1177/0272431602022001003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Essex, M. J., Boyce, W. T., Hertzman, C., Lam, L. L., Armstrong, J. M., Neumann, S. M., et al. (2013). Epigenetic vestiges of early developmental adversity: Childhood stress exposure and DNA methylation in adolescence. Child Development, 84, 58–75. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01641.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. W., Dongping, L., & Whipple, S. S. (2013). Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1342–1396. doi:10.1037/a0031808.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. J., Shera, D. M., Savage, J. H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., et al. (2006). Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Research, 1110, 166–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, L. C. H., & Gunnar, M. R. (2009). Poverty-alleviation program participation and salivary cortisol in very low-income children. Social Science & Medicine, 68, 2180–2189. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.03.032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foresight Mental Capital and Wellbeing Project (2008). Systems maps. London: The Government Office for Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganzel, B., Morris, P., & Wethington, E. (2010). Allostasis and the human brain: Integrating models of stress from the social and life sciences. Psychological Review, 117, 134–174. doi:10.1037/a0017773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgieff, M. K., Brunette, K. E., & Tran, P. V. (2015). Early life nutrition and neural plasticity. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 411–423. doi:10.1017/S0954579415000061.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gianaros, P. J., & Hackman, D. (2013). Contribution of neuroscience to the study of socioeconomic health disparities. Psychosomatic Medicine, 75, 1–6. doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182a5f9c1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grandjean, P., & Landrigan, P. J. (2014). Neurobehavioural effects of developmental toxicity. Lancet Neurology, 13, 330–338. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70278-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 65–73. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., & Meany, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 651–659. doi:10.1038/nrn2897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hackman, D. A., Gallop, R., Evans, G. W., & Farah, M. J. (2015). Socioeconomic status and executive function: Developmental trajectories and mediation. Developmental Science, 18, 686–702. doi:10.1111/desc.12246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hair, N. L., Hanson, J. L., Wolfe, B. L., & Pollak, S. D. (2015). Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatrics, 169, 822–829. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.1475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Tresch Owen, M., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., et al. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychological Science, 26, 1071–1083. doi:10.1177/0956797615581493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 55–88. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. B., Riis, J. L., & Noble, K. G. (2016). State of the art review: Poverty and the developing brain. Pediatrics, 137, pjj: e20153075. doi:10.1542/peds.2015-3075.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karatoreos, I. N., & McEwen, B. S. (2013). Annual research review: The neurobiology and physiology of resilience and adaptation across the life course. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 337–347. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, P., Evans, G. W., Angstadt, M., Ho, S. S., Sripada, C. S., Swain, J. E., et al. (2013). Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 18442–18447. doi:10.1073/pnas.1308240110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipina, S. J. (2016). Critical considerations about the use of poverty measures in the study of cognitive development. International Journal of Psychology. doi:10.1002/ijop.12282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipina, S. J. (2014). Biological and sociocultural determinants of neurocognitive development: Central aspects of the current scientific agenda. In A. Battro & I. Potrykus (Eds.), Bread and brain, education and poverty (pp. 1–30). Vatican City: Pontifical Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipina, S. J., & Colombo, J. A. (2009). Poverty and brain development during childhood: An approach from cognitive psychology and neuroscience. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lipina, S. J., & Evers, K. (2017). Neuroscience of childhood poverty: Evidence of impacts and mechanisms as vehicles of dialog with ethics. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(61). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00061.

  • Lipina, S. J., & Segretin, M. S. (2015). 6000 días más: Evidencia neurocientífica acerca del impacto de la pobreza infantil1 [6000 more days: Neuroscientific evidence about the impact of childhood poverty]. Psicología Educativa, 21(107), 116. doi:10.1016/j.pse.2015.08.003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipina, S. J., Segretin, M. S., Hermida, M. J., Prats, L., Fracchia, C., & Colombo, J. A. (2013). Linking childhood poverty and cognition: Individual and environmental predictors of non-verbal executive control in an Argentine sample. Developmental Science, 16, 697–707. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipina, S. J., Simonds, J., & Segretin, M. S. (2011). Recognizing the child in child poverty. Vulnerable Children and Youth Studies, 6, 8–17. doi:10.1080/17450128.2010.521598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 434–445. doi:10.1038/nrn2639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mackey, A. P., Finn, A. S., Leonard, J. A., Jacoby-Senghor, D. S., West, M. R., Gabrieli, C. F. O., et al. (2015). Neuroanatomical correlates of the income-achievement gap. Psychological Science, 26, 925–933. doi:10.1177/0956797615572233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maholmes, V., & King, R. B. (2012). The Oxford Handbook of poverty and child development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McEwen, B. S., & Gianaros, P. J. (2010). Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: Links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1186, 190–222. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05331.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, K. A., & Sheridan, M. A. (2016). Beyond cumulative risk: A dimensional approach to childhood adversity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 239–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H. L., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 2693–2698. doi:10.1073/pnas.1010076108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NICHD [National Institute of Child & Human Development Early Child Care Research Network] (2005). Duration and developmental timing of poverty and children´s cognitive and social development from birth through third grade. Child Development, 76, 795–810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Brito, N. H., Bartsch, H., Kane, E., Kuperman, J. M., et al. (2015). Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nature, International Weekly Journal of Science, 18, 773–778. doi:10.1038/nn.3983.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noble, K. G., Norman, M. F., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8, 74–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odgers, C. L. (2015). Income inequality and the developing child: Is it all relative? American Psychologist, 70, 722–731. doi:10.1037/a0039836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlakis, A. E., Noble, K., Pavlakis, S. G., Ali, N., & Frank, Y. (2015). Brain imaging and electrophysiology biomarkers: Is there a role in poverty and education outcome research? Pediatric Neurology, 52, 383–388. doi:10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2014.11.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peretz, I., & Zatorre, R. J. (2005). Brain organization for music processing. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 89–114. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, S. C., Finegood, E. D., & Swain, J. E. (2013). Poverty and language development: Roles of parenting and stress. Innovation in Clinical Neuroscience, 10, 10–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piccolo, L. R., Merz, E. C., He, X., Sowell, E. R., & Noble, K. G. (2016). Age-related differences in cortical thickness vary by socioeconomic status. PLoS One, 11, e0162511. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0162511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind. New York: Scientific American Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raizada, R. D. S., & Kishiyama, M. M. (2010). Effects of socioeconomic status on brain development, and how cognitive neuroscience may contribute to leveling the playing field. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 3. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.003.2010.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reardon, S. (2015). Poverty shrinks brains from birth. Nature, International Weekly Journal of Science. doi:10.1038/nature.2015.17227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, T. L., & Sweatt, J. D. (2011). Epigenetic mechanisms and environmental shaping of the brain during sensitive periods of development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 398–408. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02282.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, M. M. (2005). A powerful theory and a paradox: Ecological psychologists after Barker. Environment and Behavior, 37, 295–329. doi:10.1177/0013916504270696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheridan, M. A., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2014). Dimensions of early experience and neural development: Deprivation and threat. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 580–585. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheridan, M. A., Sarsour, K., Jutte, D., D’Esposito, M., & Boyce, W. T. (2012). The impact of social disparity on prefrontal function in childhood. PLoS ONE, 7, e35744. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0035744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shonkoff, J. P., Boyce, W. T., & McEwen, B. S. (2009). Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: Building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention. JAMA, 301, 2252–2259. doi:10.1001/jama.2009.754.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shonkoff, J. P., Garner, A. S., & Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, and Dependent Care, & Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics (2012). The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics, 129, e232–e246. doi:10.1542/peds.2011-2663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sirois, S., Spratling, M., Thomas, M. S., Westermann, G., Mareschal, D., & Johnson, M. J. (2008). Précis of neuroconstructivism: How the brain constructs cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 321–331. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0800407X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., & Hirschfeld, L. A. (2004). The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 40–46. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephens, N. M., Markus, H. R., & Phillips, L. T. (2014). Social class culture cycles: How three gateway contexts shape selves and fuel inequality. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 16.1–16.24. doi:10.1177/0022022114534768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, B. L., Levitt, P., & Stanwood, G. D. (2009). Prenatal exposure to drugs: Effects on brain development and implications for policy and education. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 303–312. doi:10.1038/nrn2598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ursache, A., & Noble, K. G. (2016). Neurocognitive development in socioeconomic context: Multiple mechanisms and implications for measuring socioeconomic status. Psychophysiology, 53, 71–82. doi:10.1111/psyp.12547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagmiller, R. L. (2015). The temporal dynamics of childhood economic deprivation and children’s achievement. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 158–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiebe, S. A., Clark, C. A. C., De Jong, D. M., Chevalier, N., Espy, K. A., & Wakschlag, L. (2015). Prenatal tobacco exposure and self-regulation in early childhood: Implications for developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 397–409. doi:10.1017/S095457941500005X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoshikawa, H., Aber, J. L., & Beardslee, W. R. (2012). The effects of poverty on the mental, emotional, and behavioral health of children and youth. American Psychologist, 67, 272–284. doi:10.1037/a0028015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, T. Y., & Meaney, M. J. (2010). Epigenetics and the environmental regulation of the genome and its function. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 439–466. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sebastián J. Lipina.

Additional information

The author is grateful for the support of CONICET, FONCYT, CEMIC, UNSAM, Fundación Conectar, and his colleagues at the Unidad de Neurobiología Aplicada and Universidad de San Martín.

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lipina, S.J. The biological side of social determinants: Neural costs of childhood poverty. Prospects 46, 265–280 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11125-017-9390-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11125-017-9390-0

Keywords

Navigation