Design of system-optimal information strategies
Four system-optimal information strategies have been developed; these strategies aim to influence individuals’ route choice behavior without restricting their freedom of choice, each with the objective to improve network efficiency. Strategies range from low information content (i.e., providing almost no contextual information) to high information content (i.e., providing detailed information on context as well as the importance and consequences of certain choices). Moreover, some strategies capitalize on travelers’ bounded rationality, whereas others focus on influencing or reinforcing their attitude towards the social route alternative. Table 1 provides an overview of this classification. Each strategy combines several principles that are, according to literature, potentially successful in changing (travel) behavior. This is done in such a manner that the strategies are distinct from each other, while remaining realistic, credible and practical (this is the reason why some cells in Table 1 are empty).
Table 1 Overview of strategy classification Now, a short description of each strategy will be provided. The fully detailed operationalization of each strategy will follow in “Operationalization of information strategies” section.
-
strategy 1 ‘Recommendation’: This strategy provides plain advice on the route alternative that the specific individual should choose to the desire of the traffic manager or road authority without any contextual information. Thereby, this strategy capitalizes on individuals’ potential perception errors (Carrion 2013) or disinterest in choosing the shortest travel time alternative. Moreover, it reduces individuals’ cognitive effort as it allows them to simply follow the provided recommendation (inspired by the effort-accuracy trade-off framework by (Johnson and Payne 1985).
-
strategy 2 ‘Nudge’: This strategy presents information about the choice situation, and each choice option, in such a manner that individuals are somewhat directed towards the desired choice option without realizing it. The social route alternative is both presented first and emphasized as such, creating awareness of its existence. This is in line with the theory of default settings (e.g. Pichert and Katsikopolous 2007; Sunstein and Thaler 2003). Additionally, positive aspects of the social alternative, as well as negative aspects of the non-social alternatives are highlighted to make these salient (Avineri 2009a) and anticipate on potential loss aversion among travelers (Kahneman and Tversky 1979). Positive aspects are also emphasized in the label of the social route.
-
strategy 3 ‘Social Reinforcement’: This strategy provides objective information about the choice situation and each choice option. Additionally, it provides information on the choices of others by showing the (in our experiment hypothetical) percentage of travelers that choose the social routing option (inspired by Araghi, Kroesen, Molin, and Van Wee (2014) who applied this principle in the context of carbon footprint offsetting). As a result, this strategy creates awareness about existing social norms regarding certain travel behavior and it reinforces trust or belief in a successful collective outcome or achievement (i.e., system optimum). Moreover, it puts their sacrifices (e.g., travel time, cost and effort) into perspective as they will know they are not the only ones making these sacrifices. Consequently, both an individual’s normative belief and his or her attitude towards the desired social choice are potentially influenced.
-
strategy 4 ‘Educate’: This strategy explains the context and importance of the individual’s choice behavior for the desired collective outcome of achieving a system optimum. Moreover, it informs the traveler about each route alternative providing objective information on their characteristics. Finally, it instructs the traveler about the desired behavior by recommending the social alternative. As a result, this strategy creates consciousness about the context in which route choices take place and raises awareness of the consequences of individual’s own behavior regarding the desired collective outcome.
Stated choice experiment (SP): questionnaire
Participants
211 respondents were recruited based on voluntary participation and self-assessment of eligibility; they were asked to only complete the questionnaire if they use a car for their commute at least every now and then. Links to the questionnaire were published on the employee portal of the University of Twente (Enschede, The Netherlands) and through social media (Facebook and Twitter). The survey was online from May to August, 2016. Respondents who completed the questionnaire could participate in a prize draw in which they could win a voucher—three vouchers were available—at the worth of €50,—which could be used at a well-known online shopping platform.
Materials
The online questionnaire consisted of four parts; the first part collected stated choices through hypothetical situations, the second part assessed respondents’ decision-making styles in line with dual-process theories developed within the field of cognitive psychology (e.g., Stanovich and West 2000), the third part assessed respondents’ social value orientation and the final part of the questionnaire collected information about respondents’ actual mobility pattern, both in general and related to their commute trip. Moreover, demographic information (i.e., age, gender, and education) was obtained. Validated questionnaires from the field of social psychology are used to assess respondents’ decision-making styles and social value orientation.
A decision style which encompasses processes that are ‘automatic, largely unconscious, and relatively undemanding of computation capacity’ (Stanovich and West 2000, p. 658) is often associated with habitual behavior (e.g., Jakobssen 2003; Verplanken et al. 1997). We measure a habit-driven decision style by means of the decisional involvement scale that was developed by Verplanken et al. (1994) to examine habit in mode choice for shopping trips. This measure consists of eight statements using 5-point Likert-scales (Cronbach’s α = .82). A high score on the aggregate decisional involvement scale suggests that travelers made their route choices deliberately; no habit is involved. As decisional involvement is known to be highly dependent on the context, statements were presented with the stem ‘When I commute by car…’ and words related to mode choice were replaced with words related to route choice.
A decision style which ‘encompasses the processes of analytic intelligence’ (Stanovich and West 2000, p. 658) is often associated with maximizing behavior, as opposed to satisficing behavior. We measure maximizing versus satisficing decision-making by the context-free maximizing tendency scale that was developed by Lai (2010). This measure consists of five statements using a 5-point Likert-scale (Cronbach’s α = .69). A high score on this scale implies that the respondent has an intrinsic tendency to maximize, while a low score is associated with the intrinsic tendency to satisfice and choose the option that is ‘good’ enough.
We measure social value orientation using the canonical SVO-slider measure developed by Murphy et al. (2011). This measure provides respondents with six carefully designed choice situations in which they have to allocate a hypothetical money-budget to themselves and someone else. Importantly, the ‘other person’ is hypothetical and anonymous, and there is no follow-up in terms of the other person accepting or rejecting the offer (as in the ultimatum game). This SVO-measure has been found to be more reliable than previously used SVO constructs, such as the Ring Measure (Liebrand 1984) and the Triple-Dominance Measure (Van Lange et al. 1997), and is compatible with a route choice context, in the sense that road users are anonymous and in the sense that reciprocity does not play a role.
Procedure
In collecting stated choices, respondents were asked to picture a hypothetical commute trip. They were provided with travel information with respect to this commute trip according to one of the information strategies from “Methodology” section (i.e., there were four versions of the questionnaire—each containing only one of four information strategies—and each respondent received only one version of the questionnaire, which was randomly assigned). Respondents were told that the information message was received on their smartphone and was sent by their local road authority. They were asked to choose between two route alternatives: their (hypothetical) usual route that takes 28 min and some ‘similar route’ with a slightly higher travel time that contributes to a certain societal goal. The assumed timing of the information message is at trip departure.
Design
For the stated choice, a 2 × 3 full factorial design was used that considers travel time sacrifices imposed on the respondent (i.e., small versus large) and societal goals (i.e., congestion alleviation, traffic safety and environmental sustainability) that are aimed for by the information message; hence, respondents generally made six choices. Note that only three choices were made when the recommend-strategy was applied; this strategy did not provide travel time information and as such the travel time sacrifice did not vary. Travel time sacrifices of 3 min and 7 min are applied. These sacrifices originate from findings of a field study by Zhu and Levinson (2010) who found that many travelers who did not use their shortest travel time alternative, used alternatives that were less than 5 min longer than this shortest travel time alternative. The reason for distinguishing between different societal goals is that it allows us to study whether travelers’ inclination to comply with the social routing advice depends on the goal which the government aims to achieve with the advice.
Revealed choice experiment (RP): field experiment
Participants
28 participants were voluntarily recruited among employees of companies located at the Business and Science Park Twente and the neighboring University of Twente in Enschede, The Netherlands. To that end, an advertisement was published on the employee portal of the University of Twente and an email was sent to the secretaries of companies and faculties. Employees were only eligible for participation if they made at least 3 commute trips per week by car during morning peak hour. It was ensured that their usual route to work passed certain predefined locations covering the main inbound routes to the destination area, guaranteeing the existence of an acceptable and realistic detour for their commute trip. The experiment took place during 5 consecutive weeks (January 16th until 17th February 2017).
Participants who completed the experiment could participate in a prize draw in which they could win an Ipad or a voucher—three vouchers were available—at the worth of €50,- and €20,-, which could be used at a well-known online shopping platform.
Materials
Data was collected using the smartphone application ‘SMART Mobility’ (SMART in Twente 2016). This application automatically collects trip-data, i.e., origin, destination, departure time, arrival time, route and mode (-chain) for each trip. Further, the app can send messages to specific users and it can send specific questions to certain users which are triggered by the occurrence of a certain event or activity, according to the principle of Experience-based Sampling (Hektner et al. 2007). Moreover, participants filled out a short questionnaire in order to assess to what extent certain personality traits influenced their actual behavior. The exact same questionnaires and scales applied in the stated choice experiment were included in order to assess participants’ decision styles and attitudes towards social behavior as well as the extent to which their route choices are deliberate or habit-driven.
Procedure
Participants installed the application ‘SMART Mobility’ on their personal smartphone. On working days, this application sends tailor-made information messages containing route advice for the morning commute to its users. The timing of the information message is set to 15 min before the user’s average commute departure time to ensure that users receive the message before they depart. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two information strategies; i.e., the ‘Recommend’-strategy or the ‘Educate’-strategy. For two days every working week the social route was advised to each participant, while on the remaining days of the working week their usual route was advised. This is to avoid that participants feel that they themselves have to sacrifice all the time, while never reaping the benefits, and therefore might stop complying with the advice or do not comply at all. Note that the behavior of drivers who do not participate in the experiment does not change. Hence, actual benefits will not be experienced by participants. However, as demonstrated by Ҫolak et al. (2016), travel time benefits will be marginal (ranging from 1 to 3 min) and might be imperceptible for the majority of travellers due to the natural variability in travel time caused by events, weather conditions and traffic lights. After a commute trip was made, app-users automatically received two questions about the main reason for choosing a particular route and the role that the information message played in that decision using experience-based sampling. Finally, actual travel times on both the usual and social routes were collected from Google Maps at a 5-min interval in order to monitor the road network and quantify the actual travel time sacrifices that have been made by the participants.
Design
Due to small sample size, only two out of four information strategies were applied; i.e., the ‘Recommend’-strategy and the ‘Educate’-strategy. These strategies were most distinctly classified regarding information content and focus. Moreover, only the commonly used goal to alleviate congestion was implemented. In order to enable within-subject comparison between stated choice and revealed choice, participants answered a question on their intended behavior at least 1 month before the start of the experiment; i.e., a choice situation, tailored to the participants’ choice context as would be experienced during the field experiment, was provided. This 1 month time period was applied in order to limit the influence of the stated intention on participants’ subsequent revealed behavior during the experiment.
Datasets
From this field experiment, we obtained two datasets; Stated Intention (SI) and Revealed Preference (RP). The SI-dataset contains the intended behavior before the start of the experiment provided by 22 participants. Note that these stated intentions are actually stated preferences; we use the term intentions in order to make a clear distinction between this dataset and the SP-dataset obtained from the questionnaire in “Stated choice experiment (SP)—questionnaire” section. The RP-dataset contains the actual behavior of 28 participants (including the 22 participants from the SI-dataset). We obtained 269 route choice observations in which the information message was read before reaching the passage point where the decision whether or not to comply should be made. In 116 of these 269 observations (43%) the social route was advised. Since 3 participants either did not read the information message or did not undertake a commute trip on days that the social route was advised, the number of participants reduces to 25 for analyses that only consider these specific days.
Operationalization of information strategies
Table 2 provides an overview of our operationalization of each information strategy. In the SP-experiment, each message was embedded in a picture of a smartphone screen in order to increase realism.
Table 2 Overview of operationalization of information strategies Outline of empirical analysis
Our analysis consists of four parts. First, we provide compliance rates for each dataset to gain some initial insights and understanding. Note that compliance is measured as the event where the respondent or participant chooses the social route in line with received advice. As such, compliance might occur for some other reason than the received advice as well (we elaborate further on this in section “Analysis of motivations”). Subsequently, we compare findings between the SP, SI and RP datasets. Wardman (1988) identified two approaches to test the external validity of stated preferences that are mainly used in transportation contexts. The first approach is to compare stated intentions regarding a certain event with the actual behavior after the event has occurred; these are so-called ‘before and after’ studies. A second approach is the comparison of travel behavior models based on stated preferences and revealed preferences. We apply both approaches. For the first approach, SI and RP choices are compared in a descriptive way. For the second approach, choices made in SP and RP are analyzed by means of estimating discrete choice models on information compliance. Finally, we conduct qualitative analyses of motivations to comply with the received route advice to obtain insights into the reasons behind observed behavior. We use a 5% significance level for all statistical tests.
Model specification
We estimated Mixed Logit models accounting for panel effects, taking into account that respondents and participants (in both experiments) made multiple choices and hence might carry some of their preferences across choice tasks.
Three models were estimated; one model based on the SP-dataset, one model based on the RP-dataset and one joint SP/RP-model based on both datasets. In the joint SP/RP-model both datasets are combined by scaling the utilities in one dataset in order to allow for differences in error term variance across datasets. We included three attributes in each model, i.e., the information strategy that was provided (4 levels in SP: Recommendation, Nudge, Social reinforcement, Education—2 levels in RP: Recommendation, Education), the societal goal that was pursued by the information content (3 levels in SP: alleviate congestion, increase safety, reduce emissions—1 level in RP: alleviate congestion) and the travel time sacrifice that was needed in order to comply with the information (continuous). Moreover, we included three two-level attributes related to personality traits (i.e., Social Value Orientation (individualist versus cooperator), Maximizing Tendency (maximizer versus satisficer), Decisional Involvement (habit executioner versus non-habit executioner)), and three attributes related to socio-demographics (i.e., Age ≤ 34 year, 35–54 year, ≥ 55 year), Education (professional education versus lower or vocational education) and Gender (male versus female). Note that these are all dummy coded, except for the continuous travel time sacrifice. As not all strategies included information on travel times—in such cases, no explicit sacrifice was provided—travel time sacrifice was interacted with a dummy for the presence of travel time information.
The utility function of compliance for the SP-model and the RP-model (note that the utility of the non-compliance option is fixed at zero for normalization purposes):
$$U_{nt} = ASC_{ } + \mathop \sum \limits_{m} \beta_{m} x_{m} + \nu_{n} + \varepsilon_{nt}$$
(1)
In which \(U_{nt}\) denotes the total utility associated with compliance by individual \(n\) in context \(t\). \(\beta_{m}\) denotes the parameter to be estimated that is associated with the \(m\)th attribute \(x_{m}\). \(ASC\) denotes an intrinsic willingness to comply with the advice. Finally, \(\nu_{n}\) and \(\varepsilon_{nt}\) are random errors. The former is Normally distributed with a mean of zero and an estimated standard deviation. This error only varies across individuals; it is constant within individuals and across tasks, reflecting a stable inclination of the individual (not) to comply with the information. The latter error is distributed i.i.d. Extreme Value type 1 across both individuals and choice tasks, reflecting additional variation in unobserved utility (‘white noise’).
Note that the societal goal attribute is not included in the RP-utility function, because only one goal was applied in the RP-experiment.
The applied utility function of compliance for the joint SP/RP-model:
$$U_{nt} = e^{{\left( {\mu \times SP} \right)}} \times \left( {{\text{ASC}} + \mathop \sum \limits_{m} \beta_{m} x_{m} + \nu_{n} + \varepsilon_{nt} } \right)_{ }$$
(2)
In which \(\mu\) represents the scale factor that is applied to the SP-dataset in order to obtain the same variance in both datasets. SP indicates to which dataset the observation belongs (i.e., it equals 1 for SP and 0 for RP). Note that \(\mu\) equals zero when there is no difference in variance of unobserved utility between the two datasets. The models are estimated using the Biogeme software package (Bierlaire 2003) with the ‘donlp2’-algorithm (Spellucci 1993) using at least 1000 Halton draws. Experiments with less draws indicated that 1000 draws were sufficient to obtain stable parameter estimates.