Electability is like the end of The Sopranos. Everybody talks about it, but nobody agrees on what it means.
–Ronald Brownstein, The Atlantic, 2019
Abstract
Previous work has shown candidate electability is an important consideration to voters in deciding who to support. However, we do not know what candidate qualities voters consider more electable, especially in the absence of polling information. While scholarship has documented general election penalties for candidates with certain demographic and ideological characteristics, we do not know whether voters actually use these factors when judging electability. Using a conjoint experimental design, we examine how candidate characteristics influence perceptions of candidate electability. We find voters perceive women and minorities as less electable and ideologically extreme candidates as more electable. However, perceptions of electability vary with voter characteristics. Our results indicate that arguments about electability, for many individuals, are based on their own ideological preferences (and to a lesser extent, their identity) rather than systematically viewing candidates with attributes that provide general election advantages as more electable.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
Why are some candidates perceived as more electable? While primary voters value candidate electability (Abramowitz, 1989; Rickershauser & Aldrich, 2007; Simas, 2017), how voters evaluate candidate traits when considering candidate electability is unstudied. Although scholars have examined the relationship between candidate traits and actual general election success (e.g., Hall, 2015; Terkildsen, 1993; Utych, 2020), whether voters perceive electability in the same way is unclear.
Despite extensive research on how various factors—most notably race (e.g., Sigelman et al., 1995; Terkildsen, 1993; Visalvanich, 2017b), gender (e.g., Dolan, 2004; Hershey, 1980; Sigelman & Sigelman, 1982), and ideology (e.g., Hall, 2015; Utych, 2020)—affect candidate vote shares in a general election (i.e. actual electability), we know less about what candidate characteristics voters believe make a candidate more electable and how those perceptions might vary across individuals.Footnote 1
To answer this question, we use observational and experimental evidence. Using polling data from 2019 and 2020 about perceptions about the electability of Democratic presidential candidates to motivate our work, we find evidence respondents view candidates whose ideology aligns with their own as more electable. However, observational data from the 2020 Democratic presidential primary field limits our ability to isolate the effect of certain candidate characteristics (e.g., ideology, race, and gender) on electability from other characteristics (e.g., fundraising or media attention).Footnote 2 This data also limits our ability to examine Republicans behaviors, which may also be different (Grossman & Hopkins, 2016).
As such, we use a conjoint survey experiment from a nationally representative sample to evaluate the importance of candidate characteristics on perceptions of electability. We ask respondents presented with a hypothetical congressional primary election match-up to indicate which candidate is more electable.Footnote 3 By randomizing candidate characteristics of interest (specifically, candidate race, gender, and ideology), we isolate their effects on voters’ perceptions of electability.
We find voters view minority and female candidates as less electable in the general election. The effect of candidate demographics, however, is much smaller than other factors such as candidate experience and candidate ideology. We find candidates who are ideologically extreme are perceived by voters to be more electable, a finding which runs counter to conventional wisdom (Masket & Noel, 2012) and actual general election success (Hall, 2015; Utych, 2020). However, these effects are driven by Republicans. While Republicans perceive candidate electability to increase with ideological extremism, Democrats perceive a slight general election electability penalty for ideologically extreme candidates.
However, across both parties, individuals view candidates who have ideological preferences aligned with their own to be more electable. Ideologically extreme respondents in both parties are more likely to view more ideologically extreme candidates as electable and partisan differences in perceptions of the electability of moderate candidates are driven by the presence of more ideologically extreme respondents among Republicans.
Similarly, although voters view minority candidates as slightly less electable, these effects are driven by white respondents’ perceptions. Minority respondents do not perceive differences in electability by candidate race. However, women are just as likely as men to perceive electoral penalties for female candidates.
Finally, in our survey experiment we find a strong relationship between voters’ perceptions of electability and vote choice. There are, however, some exceptions. While Democratic respondents viewed non-white candidates and female candidates as less electable than white candidates and male candidates, Democrats preferred non-white and female candidates. This result, however, is again driven by respondent race and gender, with women and minorities and more likely to support candidates of their own subgroup.
Actual Electability
While partisans want to nominate candidates who will advance their preferences in office, policy outcomes depend on their candidate winning office. As such, individuals may prefer to nominate candidates who are merely acceptable but who can win the general election. When given information about candidates’ general election chances, voters use that information (Abramowitz, 1989; Masket, 2020; Rickershauser & Aldrich, 2007; Simas, 2017; Stone & Abramowitz, 1983). What is less clear, however, is what types of candidates voters perceive as electable absent polling.Footnote 4
Research on actual (rather than perceived) electability has largely focused on candidate ideology. The median voter theorem predicts more ideologically centrist candidates should have greater appeal to the electorate (Downs, 1957). Observational research indicates ideologically extreme candidates suffer an electoral penalty in the general election (Hall, 2015; Utych, 2020), although the advantage appears muted in high-profile election campaigns (Cohen et al., 2016) and has declined in recent years in a more polarized environment (Canes-Wrone & Kistner, 2022; Utych, 2020).
Candidate traits also affect actual electability, with work indicating minority candidates (e.g., Sigelman et al., 1995; Terkildsen, 1993; Visalvanich, 2017b) and women (e.g., Dolan, 2004; Hershey, 1980; Karpowitz et al., Forthcoming; Sigelman & Sigelman, 1982, but see Schwarz and Coppock (2022)) suffer general election penalties.Footnote 5
Voters’ Perceptions of Electability
It is unclear, however, whether these factors also affect voters’ perceptions of candidate electability. Research on voters perceptions of electability has largely focused on single candidate traits (Sigelman et al. (1986); Williams (1990), but for an exception see Green et al. (2022).Footnote 6
Furthermore, scholarship has ignored the possibility perceptions of electability vary by voter characteristics.Footnote 7 Focusing on vote choice, voters’ evaluations of candidates, including evaluations of same party candidates, are linked to their own predispositions and grow over the campaign (Bartels, 1988) and party leaders view their own ideological preferences as more electable (Broockman et al., 2021).
In addition, perceptions of electability might vary by party. Republicans care more about ideological purity (Grossman & Hopkins, 2016). As such, we might expect penalties to perceptions of electability for ideological extremism to be smaller among Republicans. Similarly, work on political elites indicates Democrats are more supportive of minorities and women (Crowder-Meyer & Cooperman, 2018; Doherty et al., 2019; Hassell & Visalvanich, 2019) suggesting race and gender penalties on perceptions of electability could be smaller for Democrats.
Lastly, voter assessments of candidate electability, while influencing vote choice, are distinct from vote choice. Voters may compromise if it will increase the likelihood of success in the general election (Mirhosseini, 2015; Simas, 2017) but, some individuals with strong preferences (either ideological or descriptive) may view compromise as undesirable even if it reduces the likelihood of winning slightly (Bawn et al., 2019; Blum, 2020; Crowder-Meyer & Cooperman, 2018). As such, we also examine whether voters support candidates with attributes perceived as less electable.
Real World Evidence of Perceptions of Electability
We begin by looking for evidence of relationships between voter characteristics and perceptions of candidate electability in real campaign situations. The large and diverse 2020 Democratic presidential field provides an opportunity to see whether perceptions of electability vary systematically with individual respondent characteristics. In 2019 and in the early months of 2020, ABC and the Washington Post sponsored four nationally representative surveys where they asked Democrats and Democratic leaners (n = 1751 for all four surveys) to indicate which of the Democratic candidates for president “has the best chance to defeat Donald Trump in the general election?”Footnote 8 We use those surveys to examine how respondent characteristics affect the likelihood respondents chose candidates with similar characteristics as the most electable.Footnote 9
Figure 1 shows the marginal effect of sharing a trait on the likelihood of identifying a candidate with that trait as the candidate with “the best chance to defeat Donald Trump in the general election” using a linear probability model. In addition to variables for characteristics respondents shared with candidates (race, gender, and ideology), we also control for respondent income, education, age, and the specific survey. Full models are available in online appendix Table A1.
Panel (a) in Fig. 1 shows the relationship between sharing a political ideology and identifying a candidate with a similar ideology as the most likely to defeat Donald Trump. The first estimate in Panel (a) shows the effect of being moderate (relative to identifying as very liberal) on the likelihood of choosing a moderate candidate as the most likely to defeat Donald Trump. The second (right hand) point estimate in Panel (a) shows the effect of identifying as very liberal (relative to being moderate) on the likelihood of selecting a very liberal candidate as the most likely to defeat Donald Trump. Respondents were coded as having selected a liberal candidate if they identified Bernie Sanders or Elizabeth Warren as the candidate most likely to beat Donald Trump and coded as having selected a moderate candidate if they chose Joe Biden, Steve Bullock, Pete Buttigieg, or Amy Klobuchar.
As shown in Panel (a) in Fig. 1, very liberal (moderate) voters were 17.6% (14.5%) points more likely to view one of the liberal (moderate) candidates as the most likely candidate to beat Donald Trump in the 2020 presidential election. Results for individual candidates are available in the online appendix in Table A2. In separating the results by candidate, we find the effect of support for a moderate candidate is driven by moderate voters’ perceptions of Joe Biden.Footnote 10 However, both Warren and Sanders were viewed as more electable by very liberal respondents.
Panel (b) shows the relationship between respondent race and selecting a candidate of a similar race as most likely to defeat Trump. Respondents were coded as having selected a Black candidate if they selected Cory Booker or Kamala Harris. Respondents selecting Julian Castro were classified as having chosen a Latino candidate. Respondents selecting any of those three candidates as the most likely to defeat Trump in the general election were identified as having selected a minority candidate.Footnote 11 Because all minority candidates had withdrawn from the race by the third poll, this analysis only includes the first two polls taken in 2019 (n = 897).
Latino respondents were 1.3 percentage points more likely to indicate the Latino candidate, Julian Castro, was the candidate with the best chance of defeating Donald Trump in the general election compared to non-Latino respondents (p < 0.1, two-tailed test).Footnote 12 We do not, however, find any evidence Black respondents were more likely to indicate one of the African-American candidates was the most electable, or minority respondents in general were more likely to select a minority candidate as the most likely to defeat the incumbent president. Moreover, as shown in the appendix, Black respondents were much more likely to view Joe Biden as the most electable, likely because of his role as the Vice President to Barack Obama. This emphasizes the difficulty in identifying causal relationships between individual candidate characteristics in a dynamic campaign environment and when candidate characteristics are not randomized.
Panel (c) shows the relationship between respondent gender and selecting a female candidate (Tulsi Gabbard, Kamala Harris, Amy Klobuchar, Elizabeth Warren, Kirsten Gillibrand, or Marianne Williamson) as the most electable candidate. We find no evidence female candidates were more likely to be viewed as the most electable candidate in the 2020 Democratic primary field by female respondents.Footnote 13
We note, however, that these effects do not capture how the dynamics of the campaign affect perceptions of electability. While we focus here on the association between individual characteristics and the propensity to identify similar candidates as the most electable candidate, these propensities are shaped by the nature of the campaign (Bartels, 1988). Indeed, we should note the overall perceptions of electability for candidates of different types change over the course of the campaign. For example, the perception of a liberal (moderate) candidate as most electable ranges from 21 to 27% (29–47%) and perceptions of minority or female candidates as the most electable range from 2 to 12% and 8 to 24% respectively depending on the survey.Footnote 14
In short, it is impossible to draw definitive conclusions about how shared characteristics affect perceptions of electability using this data because candidate characteristics are strongly related to other characteristics that may also influence perceptions of electability (e.g., candidate characteristics affect support from party leaders (Doherty et al., 2019; Butler & Preece, 2016), fundraising dynamics (Crowder-Meyer & Cooperman, 2018), and media coverage (Kahn, 1994)). Moreover, candidates recognize their strengths and weaknesses and design campaigns to mitigate weaknesses and emphasize strengths. While these results suggest the possibility perceptions of electability may vary systematically by respondent characteristics, it lacks the internal validity necessary to permit clear identification of relationships between candidate characteristics and voters’ perceptions of electability.
Experimental Data
Given our primary interest in identifying these relationships and the limitations of our real world observational data outlined above, we turn to a survey experiment implemented as part of a nationally representative survey (n = 2051) collected through Lucid Marketplace in August of 2020.Footnote 15Footnote 16 After completing demographic questions, including questions related to a respondent’s racial and ethnic identity, age, household income, partisan identification, and political ideology (see Table 1),Footnote 17 respondents completed a series of forced choice experiments asking them to evaluate two candidates in a hypothetical primary.Footnote 18 Respondents were shown a primary matchup between two candidates corresponding to their partisan identification (leaners included) and individuals identifying as pure independents were randomly assigned to partisan treatments.Footnote 19 Upon viewing the profiles, respondents were asked which candidate would be more likely to win in a general election and which candidate they would vote for in the primary. Respondents evaluated eight candidate pairings resulting in 16,408 choice tasks.Footnote 20
Table 2 shows the universe of candidate attributes with the baseline category in parentheses.Footnote 21 Our primary interest is candidate race, gender, and ideology.Footnote 22 For ideology, we vary treatments by party. For Democratic candidates, ideological values include ‘Moderate,’ ‘Liberal,’ and ‘Extremely Liberal.’ For Republican candidates, ideological values include ‘Moderate,’ ‘Conservative,’ and ‘Extremely Conservative.’Footnote 23
Finally, we also include a number of other candidate characteristics to provide a more rounded perspective of the potential candidates. This additional information “should reduce social desirability biases by providing alternative justifications for sensitive choices” (Hainmueller et al., 2014, p. 19). We provided information about two aspects of candidate charisma—candidate speaking ability and the personableness of the candidate—and information about age and previous office-holding experience.Footnote 24
Conjoint Experiment Results
We estimate average marginal component effects (AMCEs) using a linear model (Hainmueller et al., 2014), clustering standard errors by respondent. The AMCE estimates the average extent to which a particular set of characteristics affects the probability a respondent will select a particular candidate. We rely on advice from Bansak et al. (2022) and treat AMCEs as the effect of changing an attribute from its baseline level on the probability of viewing that candidate as more electable.Footnote 25
Overall Perceptions of Electability
Before looking at whether perceptions of electability vary systematically by respondent characteristics, we first examine overall trends. Figure 2 presents the effects of candidate characteristics on perceptions of candidate electability for all respondents and for Democratic and Republican respondents separately. We focus on race, gender, and ideology but we also include the effects of candidate experience because it is an easily and clearly quantifiable characteristic previous work has highlighted as affecting actual general election success (Jacobson & Kernell, 1981, but see Porter & Treul, 2018), thus providing a reasonable gauge of the effects. We do note, however, the effects of speaking ability and personableness are larger than the effects we discuss here. Full models with all variables and models for only individuals who voted in the primary are in the online appendix.
Because preferences can diverge in the baseline level across groups, we exercise some caution in interpreting differences-in-AMCEs across party as differences in preferences because AMCE differences could be affected by each party’s underlying preferences (Leeper et al., 2020). As such, differences in AMCEs do not necessarily represent overall preference differences across Republicans and Democrats.
Race and Gender
As shown in Fig. 2, compared to white candidates, we find Black and Hispanic candidates were 6% less likely to be viewed as electable and we find similar effect sizes for both Democrats and Republicans. While effect sizes would be classified as small using Cohen’s (1969) rule of thumb and recent standards outlined by Hartman and Hidalgo (2018) using equivalence testing, they are, at the same time, similar in substance to the effect of candidate experience (Jacobson & Kernell, 1981).
Similarly, the results in Fig. 2 show voters view female candidates as less electable. Women are about 3% less likely to be viewed as electable compared to men and these effects are similar for both Democrats and Republicans. On the whole, these findings confirm previously conjectures perceptions of electability are at least influenced by candidate race and gender (Green et al., 2022; Masket & Noel, 2019).
Ideology
We do not, however, find a positive relationship between candidate ideological moderation and perceptions of electability among the full sample. Candidates described as moderate were 3% less likely to be viewed as electable relative to ideological candidates (conservative for Republicans and liberal for Democrats). There is also no perceived electability penalty for more ideologically extreme candidates. Candidates described as very liberal (very conservative) were just as likely be viewed as electable relative to candidates who were described as liberal (conservative) and roughly 3% more likely to be viewed as electable relative to moderate candidates. While not large effects, they are in the opposite direction of effects of ideological moderation on actual vote share (Hall, 2015; Utych, 2020).
This is one area where there are distinct differences by party. For Republicans, as shown in Fig. 2, moderate candidates are viewed as less electable. There is, however, no similar penalty for moderate candidates (relative to more ideologically extreme candidates) among Democrats, and candidates classified as very liberal were viewed by Democratic respondents as slightly less electable (p < 0.10).
Individual Attributes Affect Electability Perceptions
We move now to the question of whether perceptions of electability vary systematically with respondent characteristics. We test for this possibility by re-estimating the AMCEs while interacting respondent characteristics (specifically race, gender, and ideology) with candidate characteristics. We present the full set of coefficients from the models in the online appendix in Fig. A3 and Table A4. Below we focus specifically on how respondent race, gender, and ideology affect evaluations of the electability of candidates with similar and dissimilar characteristics. Before continuing, we first want to make a note about statistical power. Because each respondent completed eight tasks (evaluating 16 candidates), we have almost 8,000 observations from minority respondents who make up roughly a quarter of the sample, providing us sufficient power to detect effects as small as 3%.Footnote 26
Race and Gender
While overall Black and Hispanic candidates are less likely to be considered electable, Fig. 3 indicates these effects are driven by white voters. Figure 3 plots the differences in the likelihood of choosing a minority candidate as more electable by the minority status of the respondent. While white respondents consistently view minority candidates as less electable than white candidates, non-white voters do not, an effect consistent across parties (see online appendix Fig. A3).Footnote 27
We do not, however, see any differences in how male and female respondents evaluate female candidates (consistent with the observational results presented previously). Instead, both men and women view women as less electable (see online appendix Fig. A5).
Ideology
Figure 4 shows even larger differences in evaluations of electability of moderate and ideologically extreme candidates by the ideology of the respondent. Ideologically extreme (moderate) respondents are roughly 11 (7) percentage points more likely to believe ideologically extreme (moderate) candidates are more electable than moderates (extremists), effects comparable to or larger than those of candidate experience.
However, we do not find, as shown in Fig. 5, any substantive differences in the patterns across parties. Both Republican and Democratic respondents view candidates with similar ideologies to their own as more electable (although the penalty for moderates among extremist respondents for Republicans is marginally larger). Overall partisan differences in perceptions of electability of ideologues (see Fig. 2) seem to be driven by the ideological makeup of the parties (with there being more (fewer) ideologically extreme Republicans (Democrats) and fewer (more) moderate Republicans (Democrats)).Footnote 28
Differences in Vote Choice and Electability
Lastly, we examine differences between perceptions of electability and vote choice to understand where individual preferences and perceptions of electability deviate sufficiently to where voters are willing to support less electable candidates. However, this test of the differences between who voters perceive as electable and who they support is a hard test. Respondent vote choice was asked immediately after the question about candidate electability. Thus, electability concerns would be foremost in respondents’ minds. We would expect differences between perceptions about electability and vote choice to be stronger if respondents were not primed about electability first.
Figure 6 shows effects of candidate characteristics on respondents’ vote preferences. As before, we present the results for race, gender, and ideology along with candidate experience (full results are available in online appendix Fig. A6 and Table A6).Footnote 29 Comparing Fig. 6 to Fig. 2, in most cases respondents’ vote choices are similar to perceptions of electability. There are, however, some differences which we explore below.
Race
One difference between perceptions of electability and vote choice is the area of race. As noted in Fig. 2, minority candidates are perceived as less electable by about 6 percentage points for both Democrats and Republicans. In contrast, as shown in Fig. 6, among all voters, changing the racial identity of the candidate from white to Black had no effect on the likelihood a respondent would indicate they would vote for that candidate. We do find the penalty for changing the candidate’s race from white to Hispanic was still negative and significant (3%).
However, these effects again mask differences in two important ways. First, there are significant heterogeneities by respondent characteristics. Although minority respondents view white and minority candidates roughly as equally electable (as shown previously in Fig. 3), Fig. 7 shows minority respondents are more likely to indicate they would vote for a minority candidate than for a white candidate.
These racial differences partially help explain the second heterogeneity: differences by party, as noted Fig. 6. Minorities make up almost 36% of Democrats in the sample compared to only 15% of Republicans in the sample. Although white Democrats are also slightly more likely to support minority candidates, minority respondents are much more likely to indicate they would vote for a minority candidate.Footnote 30 While both Democrats and Republicans view minority candidates as less electable, their willingness to vote for these candidates is fundamentally different. Whereas Democrats perceive Blacks and Hispanics as slightly less electable, they are, at the same time, actually more likely to vote for Black and Hispanic candidates. In contrast, while Republicans also perceive Black and Hispanic candidates as slightly less electable they are less likely to vote for those candidates prefer black and Hispanic candidates.
Previous work has shown perceptions of electability affect vote choice (Simas, 2017). Using our data we can examine the mediating effect of electability on vote choice and specifically whether voters would exhibit even stronger first-order preferences for racial minorities absent concerns about their electability following the procedures used by Green et al. (2022). In the online appendix, we use the Mediation package in Stata (Hicks & Tingley, 2011) which implements the procedures described by Imai et al. (2010) and Imai et al. (2011). In this case, we look at both the direct relationship between candidate race and vote choice and the indirect relationship between those two variable through perceptions of electability. Previous work has looked at this relationship but only for Democrats (Green et al., 2022).Footnote 31
Online appendix Table A11 shows the direct and indirect effects of candidate race on vote choice mediated by electability. The average direct effects of the candidate being Black (or Hispanic) is 2.4 (1.2) percentage points, indicating that, absent electability concerns, voters would prefer minority candidates by that margin. However, the average causal mediation effect is − 2.9 (− 3.2) percentage points, meaning electability concerns reduced overall support for Black candidates and were a net liability for Hispanic candidates.
Moreover, Tables A12 and A13 show these effects are not uniform by party. Whereas the average direct effect for Democrats is positive and significant (7.3 percentage points and 5.0 percentage points for Blacks and Hispanics, respectively), it is negative and significant for Republicans (− 2.6 and − 3.0 percentage points for Black and Hispanic candidates, respectively). The average causal mediation effect, however is negative for both.
Gender
Similarly, whereas women were perceived as less electable overall, we see respondents are slightly more likely to indicate they would vote for a female candidates overall. While not a large effect, changing the gender of a candidate from a man to a woman increases the likelihood of that candidate being chosen as the respondent’s vote choice preference by about 2%. As with race, however, these overall effects mask important heterogeneities by gender and party.
In Fig. 6, we find the effect of changing a candidate’s gender from male to female on the likelihood of being selected as a respondents’ primary vote choice to be positive for Democrats (5% increase), but negative (but not significant) for Republicans (1% decrease). Whereas both Democrats and Republicans view female candidates as slightly less electable, Democrats are slightly more likely to vote for a female candidate in the primary while for Republicans there is not statistically discernable differences in vote choice by candidate gender.Footnote 32
The differences by party in the choice to support female candidates despite concerns about electabilty is driven both by the demographics of the parties and by different patterns of preferences across parties. As shown in Fig. 8, women are slightly more likely to indicate they will vote for a female candidate (and women are slightly more likely to be Democrats). However, in online appendix Fig. A9, we show the relative preference for female candidates is stronger among female Democrats than for female Republicans.
Lastly, we can also look at the direct and indirect effects of candidate gender using the mediation analysis outlined above to look at how gender shaped concerns about electability affect candidate support. Online appendix Table A9 shows the overall direct and indirect relationships between candidate gender and vote choice through electability and Table A10 breaks out the result by party. Consistent with what we have presented previously (and with previous work (Green et al., 2022) who examine this relationship only among Democrats), we find both a direct (and positive) effect of candidate gender on vote choice and also an indirect (and negative) effect of those candidate characteristics on on vote choice mediated by electability. Combining these positive and negative effects, results in a small positive effect overall on vote choice. These effects, however, are not uniform across parties. As shown in online appendix Table A10, Democrats exhibit patterns consistent with what we describe above, albeit with larger direct effects. Republicans, however, show no significant positive direct effects, and small negative mediating effects for an overall effect that is negative (but not statistically significant).
Ideology
Lastly, previously we found moderate candidates were perceived as less electable, but that these differences were driven by more ideological voters regardless of party (see Figs. 4 and 5). In contrast to our findings for race and gender, we do not find any differences between the relationship between ideology and electability and ideology and vote choice, nor do we find any differences in the patterns across Democrats and Republicans (see Fig. A8 in the online appendix). While overall Democrats perceive very ideological candidates as less electable and are marginally less likely to vote for them and Republicans perceive very ideological candidates more electable and are more likely to vote for them, these effects are driven entirely by the percentage of Democrats and Republicans who identify as extreme ideologues. The underlying relationships between respondent ideological characteristics and their support for candidates of a particular ideological typology are not fundamentally different across parties (as shown in online appendix Fig. A8). Differences across parties are entirely due to differences in the numbers of very ideological voters within each party.
Online appendix Table A14 shows the direct and indirect effects of candidate ideology on vote choice mediated by electability and Tables A16 and A15 break those results out by party. Table A14 shows overall there is neither direct effects of candidate ideology (the average direct effect is − 0.7 percentage points) nor mediation effects (the average causal mediation effect is − 0.4 percentage points). However, as might be expected, these effects mask strong heterogeneities by respondent ideology with moderate respondents imposing both direct and indirect penalties and extreme respondents boosting their support of ideologically extreme candidates both directly and through electability.
Discussion and Conclusion
Previous work has provided an incomplete picture of how voters use candidate traits in evaluating electability and how those perceptions vary across individuals. Using data from real world campaigns and from a survey experiment, we find what voters perceive to be electable characteristics depends on those voters’ political preferences and characteristics.
Overall, we find individuals’ identity and preferences influence how they evaluate candidate electability. Using data from the 2020 Democratic presidential primary, we find evidence that moderate (liberal) Democratic respondents are more likely to identify a moderate (liberal) candidates as the most electable. However, identifying the nature of these effects is difficult because of our inability to decouple individual candidate characteristics from other characteristics (e.g., fundraising) that may also influence perceptions of electability or to control for campaign actions.
However, our primary purpose in this work is to identify clear causal relationships between candidate characteristics, something the messy nature of real campaigns makes difficult to do. As Rose McDerrmott notes, scholars knowing “the conclusions result from the manipulations imposed [can next try] to extrapolate those findings to other contexts” (McDermott, 2011, p. 43). To provide for better internal validity we rely on a conjoint survey experiment.
First, the conjoint experiment results provide no evidence voters systematically associate ideological moderation with electability. Instead, as with the real world data, we find evidence voters’ perceptions of the relationship between ideology and electability align closely with their own ideological preference.
We do find significant partisan differences in the relationship between candidate ideology and perceptions of electability, with Republican respondents seemingly finding more ideologically extreme candidates more electable and Democratic respondents finding more moderate candidates as more electable. While differences could be a function of the 2020 electoral environment where Republicans were re-nominating a president viewed as ideologically extreme (Hopkins & Noel, 2022), our results suggest these findings are a function of differences in the ideological composition of the parties (Grossman & Hopkins, 2016). Both Republicans and Democrats view candidates with ideological identities aligning with their own stated ideological preferences as more electable. Because there are more moderate Democrats in the Democratic Party than there are moderate Republicans in the Republican Party, Democrats are more disposed to view moderates as more electable while Republicans are more likely to perceive more extreme candidates as more electable.
Second, respondents, on the whole, perceived women and minority candidates to be less electable when compared to male and white candidates. We also do not find differences by party on the effect of race and gender on perceptions of electability. However, there are differences by race in perceptions of the electability of minorities, with minority respondents perceiving minority candidates as just as electable as white candidates while white respondents view minority candidates as less electable.
Finally, while the traits respondents found electable closely matched traits they favored when choosing to vote for a candidate, there were two notable exceptions. Democratic respondents saw non-white candidates and women candidates as less electable than white male candidates, but indicated they would support these candidates anyways. This finding aligns with recent research that has found a liberalization in racial attitudes among Democratic partisans who have come to value descriptive representation of minorities and women, a contributing to a trend of increased representation for these groups in the party in elected office (Crowder-Meyer & Cooperman, 2018; Hassell & Visalvanich, 2019) and align with discussions within the Democratic party that were ongoing during the 2020 election (Masket, 2020). However, these patterns are also a product of minorities and women (who are more likely to identify as Democrats) being more willing to vote for candidates like themselves despite electability concerns.
Despite the importance of our results which show the impact that candidate race, gender, and ideology have on perceptions of electability, it is important to note that our study does not exhaust the number of potential factors that might contribute to those perceptions. There are an endless multitude of candidate traits and campaign tactics (or interactions between traits and tactics) that likely have an influence on perceptions of candidate electability. While this study has focused on candidate qualities and their effect on electability, it is also true that how candidates appeal to different parts of their political coalitions (Grossman & Hopkins, 2016; Masket, 2020) and how those appeals interact with candidate characteristics (Karpowitz et al., 2020) could also factor into their broader appeal. Thus, while our efforts here have greatly expanded our understanding of electability, there remains much to be studied about primary elections and voters’ perceptions of electability.
In addition, while the results from our survey experiment that follow provide clear internal validity of the relationship between individual characteristics and voters’ perceptions of electability, the real world results also highlight the need for future work to better understand how campaign actions (and the actions of other political actors) extenuate or mitigate these relationships.
On the whole, however, our findings present a clearer picture of perceptions of how candidate electability is shaped by candidate race, gender, and ideology and the interaction of those characteristics with the characteristics of the individuals making those evaluations. Although years of scholarly research has provided clear evidence of the relationship between many candidate characteristics and actual electability, our findings show voters’ perceptions of how those components affect a candidate’s electability are connected to an individual’s own ideological preferences and identity.
Data availability
Replication data for this project can be found at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/HUMCVU.
Notes
We are interested most in identifying causal relationships because “without first establishing internal validity, it remains unclear what process should be explored in the real world” (McDermott, 2011, p. 43).
Although some observational work finds no effects of gender and race on vote shares (e.g., ,Black & Erickson, 2003; Highton, 2004), women and minorities run in different districts (Branton, 2009) and face a more difficult pipeline to the general election (especially for higher offices) result in different comparison groups (Anzia and Berry, 2011; Dynes et al., 2021; Shah, 2014). Likewise, while recent a recent meta analysis of experimental evidence shows no electoral penalty for women (Schwarz & Coppock, 2022), this work focuses on voter responses to candidate sex rather than candidate gender. Work focusing on gender finds Republican voters penalize candidates with “feminine” self-presentations regardless of the candidate’s sex (Karpowitz et al., Forthcoming).
An exception is Green et al. (2022) who look at how support for female and minority candidates varies by racial resentment and hostile sexism, but do not consider variation by respondent ideology, race, or gender.
The surveys were conducted by Langer Research Associates. The first survey was conducted June 28–July 2, 2019, the second September 2–5, 2019, the third January 20–23, 2020, and the fourth February 14–17, 2020.
YouGov asked respondents to identify how likely an individual candidate was to defeat President Trump in the general election. This alternative measure might produce more nuance in how candidate characteristics (and campaign dynamics) affect perceptions of electability. Unfortunately, detailed respondent level data for these surveys is not publicly available (see Sides et al. (2022) for topline analysis).
Only 39 individuals, 43 individuals, and one individual in the four surveys choose Amy Klobuchar, Pete Buttigieg, or Steve Bullock, respectively, as the candidate most likely to defeat Donald Trump.
We do not include Andrew Yang as a minority candidate because of the small number of Asian respon- dents (n = 43 when his name is included on the survey) and because perceptions of Asian candidates among non-Asians is fundamentally different than perceptions of other minority candidates (Visalvanich, 2017a).
We are also likely underpowered given the small number of Latino respondents in the data (n = 109).
We have small sample problems when disaggregating by candidate as Gillibrand (0), Gabbard (5), Klobuchar (39), and Williamson (1) all had under 40 respondents indicate they were the most likely candidate to defeat Donald Trump. The effects for Harris (57 respondents) and Warren (143 respondents), while both not significant, go in opposite directions with women more likely (less likely) to select Warren (Harris) as the most electable candidate suggesting the importance of the intersectionality of race and gender. We are hesitant to read too much into this because of the small sample of individuals who viewed Harris as most likely to defeat Trump.
The wide range in perceptions of moderate candidate electability is primarily due to widespread agreement of Joe Biden being the most electable in the first three surveys, but that number dropping in February after losing the Iowa Caucus and the New Hampshire Primary (but before finishing second in the Nevada Caucus and winning the South Carolina Primary), showing the impact of campaign events on primary electability.
For more information on Lucid and checks for data quality see the Online Appendix.
This experiment was deemed exempt by the IRB at Florida State University (00001397).
Due to survey error, there was no option for respondents to indicate their race and ethnicity as Hispanic or Latino. While unfortunate, it likely does not affect our conclusions. The inclusion of these Latino respondents within the White/Caucasian demographic likely minimizes differences between minorities and whites seen here, meaning the sizes we find here are likely larger.
We use primary elections because this is the electoral context where concerns about electability play into vote choice. We use congressional primaries because it is more realistic voters would have no general election polling information to cue electability and also because we were interested in how differences in district demographics and competitiveness affect perceptions of electability (addressed elsewhere).
Excluding partisan leaners or independents does not change the results.
Researchers can assign dozens (many more than eight) of choice tasks without declines in response quality (Bansak et al., 2018).
We also varied district demographics (either 55 or 95% white) and competitiveness (the retiring same-party incumbent winning 51 or 68% of the vote the previous election). Consistent with other work suggesting perceptions of electability do not change as district characteristics change (Broockman et al., 2021; Hassell and Visalvanich, 2019), we do not find any differences in perceptions of electability across these variables. As such, we do not separate out the (lack of) heterogeneous effects along these dimensions.
In our treatment, we signal race and gender explicitly rather than through names because of concerns with the nature of names also signaling other attributes (Crabtree et al., 2022).
We intentionally chose to represent candidate ideology through labels rather than through issue positions because we are interested in the relationship between respondent and candidate characteristics. Using ideological labels allows respondents to place themselves and candidates on that same scale by allowing respondents to label their own ideological identity and to project that onto candidates. We assume, however, most respondents know moderate is closer to the ideological center (regardless of what that center actually is).
We include these valence attributes to reduce desirability biases by providing plausible alternative justifications for choices and voters value candidate traits beyond party, ideology, or policy positions (Heixweg, 1979).
See also work from Abramson et al. (2022) for an ongoing discussion of how to interpret AMCEs.
We are more limited if we focus only on minority Republicans (almost 2000 observations) or if we focus only on Black respondents regardless of party (just over 2000 respondents) but this still provides power to detect effects as small as 4.5%.
We pool non-white respondents together because the literature on multi-ethnic voting has generally found minority voters to be more favorable to minority candidates (Baretto, 2010). These effects are also similar if we look only at black respondents (see Table A5). Because of our survey error, we are not able to look at Hispanic respondents’ evaluations of Hispanic (or Black) candidates.
The pattern for ideologues (liberals/conservatives rather than moderates or very liberal/very conservative) is consistent with the trend line of moderates, but not as steep.
We also ran models (available in the online appendix) only including respondents who had voted the primary election. They are similar to results presented here.
Running the models on only white Democrats shows candidate race has a positive effect on vote choice, although its effect size is reduced (3 percentage points instead of 6) and only approaches statistical significance (p < 0.06 level for Blacks and p < 0.14 for Hispanics rather than the p < 0.01 level shown in the text).
We note, however, these causal direct and indirect relationships should be evaluated with great caution because the assumptions needed to identify causal mechanisms through this procedure are strong and often implausible in situations where both the treatment and the mediator are not randomized (Bullock et al., 2010) (which is the case in this design, and in the design of Green et al. (2022) referenced above, as perceptions of electability are not randomized). Alternatively Acharya et al. (2018) detail a design better identifying mediating factors in conjoint experiments by examining the effects of randomly including or excluding attributes on the causal effects of the other attributes (in our case, randomly providing or not providing explicit information about electability), however, our experiment was not designed with this intent in mind and as such does not randomize explicit information about electability.
References
Abramowitz, A. I. (1989). Viability, electability, and candidate choice in a presidential primary election: A test of competing models. Journal of Politics, 51(4), 977–992.
Abramson, S. F., Koçak, K., & Magazinnik, A. (2022). What do we learn about voter preferences from conjoint experiments? American Journal of Political Science, 66(4), 1008–1020.
Acharya, A., Blackwell, M., & Sen, M. (2018). Analyzing causal mechanisms in survey experiments. Political Analysis, 26(4), 357–378.
Anzia, S. F., & Berry, C. R. (2011). The Jackie (and Jill) Robinson effect: Why do congresswomen outperform congressmen? American Journal of Political Science, 55(3), 478–493.
Bansak, K., Hainmueller, J., Hopkins, D. J., & Yamamoto, T. (2018). The number of choice tasks and survey satisficing in conjoint experiments. Political Analysis, 26(1), 112–119.
Bansak, K., Hainmueller, J., Hopkins, D. J., & Yamamoto, T. (2022). Using conjoint experiments to analyze elections: the essential role of the average marginal component effect. Political Analysis, 41(4), 500–518.
Baretto, M. (2010). Ethnic cues. University of Michigan Press.
Bartels, L. M. (1988). Presidential primaries and the dynamics of public choice. Princeton University Press.
Bawn, K., Brown, K., Ocampo, A. X., Patterson, S., Ray, J. L., & Zaller, J. (2019). A congress of champions: Principal agent relationships in US house nominations. Retrieved from https://projects.iq.harvard.edu/files/piep/files/principal_agent_approach_to_house_nominations.pdf
Black, J. H., & Erickson, L. (2003). Women candidates and voter bias: Do women politicians need to be better? Electoral Studies, 22(1), 81–100.
Blum, R. M. (2020). How the tea party captured the GOP: Insurgent factions in american politics. University of Chicago Press.
Branton, R. P. (2009). The importance of race and ethnicity in congressional primary elections. Political Research Quarterly, 62(3), 459–473.
Broockman, D. E., Carnes, N., Crowder-Meyer, M., & Skovron, C. (2021). Why local party leaders don’t support nominating centrists. British Journal of Political Science, 51(2), 724–749.
Brownstein, R. (2019, Sept 12). How pundits may be getting electability all wrong. The Atlantic. Retrieved from https://www.theatlantic.com/politics/archive/2019/09/electability-democrats-2020-joe-biden/597904/
Bullock, J. G., Green, D. P., & Shang, E. H. (2010). Yes, but what’s the mechanism? (Don’t expect an easy answer). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(4), 550–558.
Butler, D. M., & Preece, J. R. (2016). Recruitment and perceptions of gender bias in party leader support. Political Research Quarterly, 69(4), 842–851.
Canes-Wrone, B., & Kistner, M. R. (2022). Out of step and still in congress? Electoral consequences of incumbent and challenger positioning across time. Quarterly Journal of Political Science, 17(3), 389–420.
Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
Cohen, M., McGrath, M. C., Aronow, P., & Zaller, J. (2016). Ideologically extreme candidates in U.S. presidential elections, 1948–2012. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 667(1), 126–142.
Crabtree, C., Michael Gaddis, S., Holbein, J. B., & Larsen, E. N. (2022). Racially distinctive names signal both race/ethnicity and social class. Sociological Science, 9, 454–472.
Crowder-Meyer, M., & Cooperman, R. (2018). Can’t buy them love: How party culture among donors contributes to the party gap in women’s representation. Journal of Politics, 80(4), 1211–1224.
Doherty, D., Dowling, C. M., & Miller, M. G. (2019). Do local party chairs think women and minority candidates can win? Evidence from a conjoint experiment. Journal of Politics, 81(4), 1282–1297.
Dolan, K. (2004). Voting for women: How the public evaluates women candidates. Westview Press.
Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. Harper.
Dynes, A. M., Hassell, H. J. G., Preece, J. R., & Miles, M. R. (2021). Personality and gendered selection processes in the political pipeline. Politics & Gender, 17(1), 53–73.
Green, J., Schaffner, B., & Luks, S. (2022). Strategic discrimination in the 2020 democratic primary. Public Opinion Quarterly, 86, 886–898.
Grossman, M., & Hopkins, D. A. (2016). Asymmetric politics: Ideological republicans and group interest democrats. Oxford University Press.
Hainmueller, J., Hopkins, D. J., & Yamamoto, T. (2014). Causal inference in conjoint analysis: understanding multidimensional choices via stated preference experiments. Political Analysis, 22(1), 1–30.
Hall, A. B. (2015). What happens when extremists win primaries? American Political Science Review, 109(1), 1–46.
Hartman, E., & Daniel Hidalgo, F. (2018). An equivalence approach to balance and placebo tests. American Journal of Political Science, 62(4), 1000–1013.
Hassell, H. J. G., & Visalvanich, N. (2019). The party’s primary preferences: Race, gender, and party support of congressional primary candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 63(4), 905–919.
Heixweg, S. (1979). An examination of voter conceptualization of the ideal political candidate. Southern Speech Communication Journal, 44(4), 373–385.
Hershey, M. R. (1980). Support for political woman: The effects of race, sex, and sexual roles. In J. C. Pierce & J. L. Sullivan (Eds.), The electorate reconsidered (pp. 180–197). Sage Publications.
Hicks, R., & Tingley, D. (2011). Causal mediation analysis. The Stata Journal, 11(4), 609–615.
Highton, B. (2004). White voters and African American candidates for congress. Political Behavior, 26(1), 1–25.
Hopkins, D. J., & Noel, H. (2022). Trump and the shifting meaning of ’conservative’: Using activists’ pairwise comparisons to measure politicians’ perceived ideologies. American Political Science Review, 116(3), 1133–1140.
Imai, K., Keele, L., & Tingley, D. (2010). A general approach to causal mediation analysis. Psychological Methods, 15(4), 309–334.
Imai, K., Keele, L., Tingley, D., & Yamamoto, T. (2011). Unpacking the black box of causality: Learning about causal mechanisms from experimental and observational studies. American Political Science Review, 105(4), 765–789.
Jacobson, G. C., & Kernell, S. (1981). Strategy and choice in congressional elections. Yale University Press.
Kahn, K. F. (1994). The distorted mirror: Press coverage of women candidates for statewide office. Journal of Politics, 56(1), 154–173.
Karpowitz, C. F., Monson, J. Q., Preece, J. R., & Aldrige, A. T. G. (Forthcoming). Selecting for masculinity: Women’s under-representation in the Republican Party. American Political Science Review. https://www.dropbox.com/s/3z0ofo1qt96z1qj/Selecting%20for%20Masculinity%205-26-2022.pdf?dl=0
Karpowitz, C. F., King-Meadows, T., Pope, J. C., & Quin Monson, J. (2020). What leads racially resentful voters to choose black candidates? Journal of Politics, 83(1), 103–121.
Leeper, T. J., Hobolt, S. B., & Tilley, J. (2020). Measuring subgroup preferences in conjoint experiments. Political Analysis, 28(2), 207–221.
Masket, S. E., & Noel, H. (2019, May 15). Should we consider ‘electability’ when evaluating candidates? Vox.com. Retrieved from https://www.vox.com/mischiefs-of-faction/2019/5/15/18623442/considering-electability
Masket, S. (2020). Learning from loss: The democrats 2016–2020. Cambridge University Press.
Masket, S. E., & Noel, H. (2012). Serving two masters: Using referenda to assess partisan versus dyadic legislative representation. Political Research Quarterly, 65(1), 104–123.
McDermott, R. (2011). Internal and external validity. In J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski, & A. Lupia (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of experimental political science (pp. 27–40). Cambridege University Press.
Mirhosseini, M. R. (2015). Primaries with strategic votes: Trading off electability and ideology. Social Choice and Welfare, 44(3), 457–471.
Porter, R., & Treul, S. (2018). Inexperienced candidates are becoming more common, but only in one party. VOX: Mischiefs of Faction. Retrieved from https://www.vox.com/mischiefs-of-faction/2018/4/20/17261870/inexperienced-candidates-republicans
Rickershauser, J., & Aldrich, J. H. (2007). “It’s the electability, stupid” or maybe not? Electability, substance, and strategic voting in presidential primaries. Electoral Studies, 26(2), 371–380.
Schwarz, S., & Coppock, A. (2022). What have we learned about gender from candidate choice experiments? A meta-analysis of sixty-seven factorial survey experiments. Journal of Politics, 84(2), 655–668.
Shah, P. (2014). It takes a black candidate: A supply-side theory of minority representation. Political Research Quarterly, 67(2), 266–279.
Sides, J., Tausanovitch, C., & Vavreck, L. (2022). The bitter end: The 2020 presidential campaign and the challenge to american democracy. Princeton University Press.
Sigelman, C. K., Sigelman, L., Thomas, D. B., & Ribich, F. D. (1986). Gender, physical attractiveness, and electability: An experimental investigation of voter biases. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 16(3), 229–248.
Sigelman, C. K., Sigelman, L., & Walkosz, B. J. (1995). Black candidates, white voters: Understanding racial bias in political perceptions. American Journal of Political Science, 38(1), 243–265.
Sigelman, L., & Sigelman, C. K. (1982). Sexism, racism, and ageism in voting behavior: An experimental analysis. Social Psychology Quarterly, 45, 263–269.
Simas, E. N. (2017). The effects of electability on US primary voters. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties, 27(3), 274–290.
Stone, W. J., & Abramowitz, A. I. (1983). Winning may not be everything, but it’s more than we thought: Presidential party activists in 1980. American Political Science Review, 77(4), 945–956.
Terkildsen, N. (1993). When white voters evaluate black candidates: The processing implications of candidate skin color, prejudice, and self-monitoring. American Journal of Political Science, 37(4), 1032–1053.
Thomsen, D. M. (2020). Ideology and gender in U.S. house elections. Political Behavior, 42(2), 415–442.
Utych, S. M. (2020). Man bites blue dog: Are moderates really more electable than ideologues? Journal of Politics, 82(1), 392–396.
Visalvanich, N. (2017a). Asian candidates in America: The surprising effects of positive racial stereotyping. Political Research Quarterly, 70(1), 68–81.
Visalvanich, N. (2017b). When does race matter? Exploring white responses to minority congressional candidates. Politics, Groups, and Identities, 5(4), 618–641.
Williams, L. F. (1990). White/black perceptions of the electability of black political candidates. In L. J. Barker (Ed.), Black electoral politics. New York: Routledge.
Acknowledgements
The authors are grateful to Dave Anderson, Julia Azari, Tessa Ditonto, Seth Masket, the participants at the North-East Research Development Workshop of Durham University and Newcastle University, and the participants at a 2022 MPSA Panel for their feedback and suggestions. We are also thankful to Steve Utych for his collaboration in implementing the survey experiment.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors did not receive support from any organization for the submitted work. The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Hassell, H.J.G., Visalvanich, N. Perceptions of Electability: Candidate (and Voter) Ideology, Race, and Gender. Polit Behav (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-023-09909-3
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-023-09909-3