Aydin, C. (2009). On the significance of ideals. Peirce and the good life. Transactions of the C.S. Peirce Society, 45, 422–443.
Google Scholar
Aydin, C. (2015). The artifactual mind: Overcoming the ‘inside-outside’ dualism in the extended mind thesis and recognizing the technological dimension of cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 14(1), 73–94.
Google Scholar
Aydin, C. (2018a). From camera obscura to fMRI: How brain imaging technologies mediate free will. In J. Aagaard, J. K. Berg Friis, J. Sorenson, O. Tafdrup, & C. Hasse (Eds.), Postphenomenological methodologies: New ways in mediating techno-human relationships (pp. 103–121). Lanham: Rowman Littlefield Press/Lexington Books.
Google Scholar
Aydin, C. (2018b). World oriented self-formation as sublimation: Or why postphenomenology needs Peircean pragmatism. Cognitio, 19(2), 204–219.
Google Scholar
Bagdasaryan, J., & Le Van Quyen, M. (2013). Experiencing your brain: Neurofeedback as a new bridge between neuroscience and phenomenology. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 680.
Google Scholar
Bartholdy, S., Musiat, P., Campbell, I. C., & Schmidt, U. (2013). The potential of neurofeedback in the treatment of eating disorders: A review of the literature. European Eating Disorders Review, 21(6), 456–463.
Google Scholar
Bailey, L. W. (1989). Skull’s darkroom: The camera Obscura and subjectivity. In P. T. Durbin (Ed.), Philosophy of technology: Practical, historical and other dimensions (pp. 63–79). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Masicampo, E. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2011). Do conscious thoughts cause behavior? Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 331–361.
Google Scholar
Bowell, R. G., & Kober, H. (2016). Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: A meta-analytic review. Obesity Reviews, 17(2), 159–177.
Google Scholar
Chaudhary, U., Xia, B., Cohen, L. G., & Birbaumer, M. (2017). Brain-computer interface-based communication in the completely locked-in state. PLoS Biology, 15(1), e1002593.
Google Scholar
Christoff, K., Cosmelli, D., Legrand, D., & Thomspon, E. (2011). Specifying the self for cognitive neuroscience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 104–112.
Google Scholar
Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58(1), 7–19.
Google Scholar
Danziger, K. (1994). Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psychological research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Davey, C. G., Pujol, J., & Harrison, B. J. (2016). Mapping the self in the brain’s default mode network. Neuroimage, 132, 390–397.
Google Scholar
Descartes R. (1965). Discourse on method, optics, geometry, and meteorology. In Paul J. Olscamp (transl.). New York/Kansas City: The Bobbs-Merrill company, Inc.
Devue, C., & Brédart, S. (2011). The neural correlates of visual self-recognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(1), 40–51.
Google Scholar
Doris, J. (2002). Lack of character: Personality and moral behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Dretske, F. (1988). Explaining behavior: Reasons in a world of causes. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Dumit, J. (2004). Picturing personhood: Brain scans and biomedical identity. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Google Scholar
Farrer, C., & Frith, C. D. (2002). Experiencing oneself vs another person as being the cause of an action: The neural correlates of the experience of agency. Neuroimage, 15(3), 596–603.
Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Google Scholar
Frankfurt, H. (1971). Freedom of the will and the concept of a person. The Journal of Philosophy, 68, 5–20.
Google Scholar
Fried, I., Mukamel, R., & Kreiman, G. (2011). Internally generated preactivation of single neurons in human medial frontal cortex predicts volition. Neuron, 69, 548–562.
Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2007). Neurophilosophy and Neurophenomenology. In L. Embree & T. Enon (Eds.), Phenomenology 2005 (Vol. 5, pp. 293–316). Bucharest: Zeta Press.
Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2013). A pattern theory of self. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 443.
Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., & Crisafi, A. (2009). Mental institutions. Topoi, 28(1), 45–51.
Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., & Daly, A. (2018). Dynamical relations in the self-pattern. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 664.
Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., & Varela, F. (2003). Redrawing the map and resetting the time: Phenomenology and the cognitive sciences. Canadian Journal of Philosophy. Supplementary, 29, 93–132.
Google Scholar
Garrisson, K. A., Scheinost, D., Worhunsky, P. D., Elwafi, H. M., Thornhill IV, T. A., et al. (2013). Real-time fMRI links subjective experience with brain activity during focused attention. Neuroimage, 81(November 1), 110–118.
Google Scholar
Gevensleben, H., Holl, B., Albrecht, B., Schlamp, D., Kratz, O., et al. (2009). Distinct EEG effects related to neurofeedback training in children with ADHD: A randomized controlled trial. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 74, 149–157.
Google Scholar
Gillihan, S. J., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Is self special? A critical review of evidence from experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 76–97.
Goldman, R. L., Borckardt, J. J., Frohman, H. A., O’Neil, P. M., Madan, A., et al. (2011). Prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) temporarily reduces food cravings and increases the self-reported ability to resist food in adults with frequent food craving. Appetite, 56(3), 741–746.
Google Scholar
Hacking, I. (1986). Making up people. In T. L. Heller, M. Sosna, & D. E. Wellbery (Eds.), Reconstructing individualism: Autonomy, individuality, and the self in Western thought (pp. 222–236). Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Google Scholar
Haggard, P. (2017). Sense of agency in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18, 196–207.
Google Scholar
Haynes, J.-D., & Rees, G. (2006). Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 523–534.
Google Scholar
Ihde, D. (1990). Technology and the lifeworld: From garden to earth. Indianapolis: Indiana University Press.
Google Scholar
Ihde, D. (2007). Art precedes science. In P. Kockelkoren (Ed.), Mediated vision (pp. 25–37). Arnhem: ArtEZ/Veenman Publishing.
Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Chua, H. F., Tompson, S., & Han, S. (2013). Neural mechanisms of dissonance: An fMRI investigation of choice justification. Neuroimage, 69, 206–212.
Google Scholar
Klein, C. (2010). Images are not the evidence in neuroimaging. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 61(2), 265–278.
Google Scholar
Klemm, R. (2010). Free will debates: Simple experiments are not so simple. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 6, 47–65.
Google Scholar
Kockelkoren, P. (2003). Technology: Art, fairground and theatre. Rotterdam: NAi Publishers.
Google Scholar
Legrand, D., & Ruby, P. (2009). What is self-specific? Theoretical investigation and critical review of neuroimaging results. Psychological Review, 116(1), 252–282.
Google Scholar
Ljubisavljevic, M., Maxood, K., Bjekic, J., Oommen, J., & Nagelkerke, N. (2016). Long-term effects of repeated prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on food craving in normal and overweight young adults. Brain Stimulation, 9(6), 826–833.
Google Scholar
Lutz, A., & Thompson, E. (2003). Neurophenomenology: Integrating subjective experience and brain dynamics in the neuroscience of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10(9–10), 31–52.
Google Scholar
Macdonald, C. (2007). Introspection and authoritative self-knowledge. Erkenntnis, 67(2), 355–372.
Google Scholar
Malabou, C. (2008). What should we do with our brain? (S. Rand, trsl.). New York: Fordham University Press.
Google Scholar
Micoulaud-Franchi, J. A., Quiles, C., Fond, G., Cermolacce, M., & Vion-Dury, J. (2014). The covariation of independent and dependant variables in neurofeedback: A proposal framework to identify cognitive processes and brain activity variables. Consciousness and Cognition, 26, 162–168.
Google Scholar
Monroe, A. E., & Malle, B. F. (2010). From uncaused will to conscious choice: The need to study, not speculate about people’s folk concept of free will. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 1, 211–224.
Google Scholar
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolnu, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain – A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage, 31, 440–457.
Google Scholar
Peirce, C.S. (1931-1935, 1958). Collected papers of C.S. Peirce. In Ch. Hartshorne & P. Weiss; A. Burks (Eds). Cambridge: Harvard University press, CP 8.320 (abbreviation: CP followed by ‘[volume].[page]’).
Peirce, C.S. The essential Peirce. Hauser, N. & Kloesel, N.; The Peirce Edition Project (Eds.). Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. 1992 and 1998 (EP followed by ‘[volume], [page]’).
Poldrack, R. A. (2011). Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data: From reverse inference to large-scale decoding. Neuron, 72(5), 692–697.
Google Scholar
Roskies, A. L. (2007). Are neuroimages like photographs of the brain? Philosophy of Science, 74(5), 860–872.
Google Scholar
Sartre, J. P. (1943). Being and nothingness: An essay on phenomenological ontology (H.E. Barnes, trsl.). London: Routledge.
Google Scholar
Slaby, J. (2010). Steps towards a Critical Neuroscience. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(3), 397–416.
Google Scholar
Slors, M. (2015). Conscious intending as self-programming. Philosophical Psychology, 28(1), 94–113. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2013.803922.
Article
Google Scholar
Soon, C .S., Brass, M., Heinze, H.-J., & Hayned, J.-D. (2008). Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 543–545.
Stillman, T. F., Baumeister, R. F., & Mele, A. R. (2011). Free will in everyday life: Autobiographical accounts of free and unfree actions. Philosophical Psychology, 24, 381–394.
Google Scholar
Sulzer, J., Sitaram, R., Blefari, M. L., Kollias, S., Birbaumer, N., Stephan, K. E., Luft, A., & Gassert, R. (2013). Neurofeedback-mediated self-regulation of the dopaminergic midbrain. Neuroimage, 83, 176–184.
Google Scholar
Taylor, C. (1989). Sources of the self: The making of modern identity. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
Taylor, C. (1992). The ethics of authenticity. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
Thibault, R. T., MacPherson, A., Lifshitz, M., Roth, R. R., & Raz, A. (2018). Neurofeedback with fMRI: A critical systematic review. Neuroimage, 172, 786–807.
Google Scholar
Vansteensel, M. J., Pels, E. G. M., Bleichner, M. G., Branco, M. P., Denison, T., Freudenburg, Z. V., Gosselaar, P., Leinders, S., Ottens, T. H., Van Den Boom, M. A., Van Rijen, P. C., Aarnoutse, E. J., & Famsey, N. F. (2016). Fully implanted brain-computer interface in a locked-in patient with ALS. The New England Journal of Medicine, 375(21), 2060–2066.
Google Scholar
Varela, F. (1996). Neurophenomenology: A methodological remedy for the hard problem. Journal for Consciousness Studies, 3(4), 330–349.
Google Scholar
Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Verbeek, P. P. (2005). What things do: Philosophical reflections on technology, agency, and design. University Park: Pennsylvania State University Press.
Google Scholar
Vidal, F., & Ortega, F. (2017). Being brains: Making the cerebral subject. New York: Fordham University Press.
Google Scholar
Vincent, N. A. (Ed.). (2013). Neuroscience and legal responsibility. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Vogeley, K., & Gallagher, S. (2011). Self in the brain. In S. Gallagher (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of the self (pp. 111–136). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G., & Baler, R. D. (2011). Reward, dopamine and the control of food intake: Implications for obesity. Trends in Cognitive Science, 15(1), 37–46.
Google Scholar
de Vries, J., Byrne, M., & Kehoe, E. (2015). Cognitive dissonance induction in everyday life: An fMRI study. Social Neuroscience, 10(3), 268–281.
Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M., & Wheatley, T. (1999). Apparent mental causation: Sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist, 54, 480–492.
Google Scholar
Weingarten, H. P., & Elston, D. (1990). The phenomenology of food cravings. Appetite, 15(3), 231–246.
Google Scholar
Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322, 606–607.
Google Scholar
Young, K. D., Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Misaki, M., Yuan, H., Drevets, W. C., & Bodurka, J. (2014). Real-time fMRI neurofeedback training of amygdala activity in patients with major depressive disorder. PLoS One, 9(2), e88785.
Google Scholar
Zahavi, D. (2015). Self and other: From pure ego to co-constituted we. Continental Philosophy Review, 48(2), 143–160.
Google Scholar