Skip to main content
Log in

Comparative metaphysics: the development of representing natural and normative regularities in human and non-human primates

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

How do human children come up to carve up and think of the world around them in its most general and abstract structure? And to which degree are these general forms of viewing the world shared by other animals, notably by non-human primates? In response to these questions of what could be called comparative metaphysics, this paper discusses new evidence from developmental and comparative research to argue for the following picture: human children and non-human primates share a basic framework of natural ontology: they think about their natural surroundings in essentialist ways in terms of natural kind objects constituted by their essential properties, and in generic terms as governed by general descriptive regularities. In contrast, there is a great divide when it comes to how human children and non-human primates carve up their social environment: only human children then go on to use their essentialist and generic thinking for developing a distinctively social ontology, to conceive of their surrounding in terms of socially constituted objects governed by general prescriptive norms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Whether the two types of rules really are categorically different, or whether there actually is a meaningful distinction to be made here, are of course controversial philosophical questions (see, e.g. (Hindriks 2009). These need not concern us here, however, since what matters for present purposes is that understanding human social practices with the conventional use of objects requires an understanding of the normative rules of those practices, be they regulative, constitutive or both.

  2. This holds within limits, of course. There are boundary conditions such that only physical objects that we can perceive and in some way deal with, for example, can acquire status functions. The crucial point, though, is that within these limits, the physical properties of the objects are largely irrelevant.

  3. For some potential precursors, see (Rudolf von Rohr et al. 2011)

References

  • Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E. S., & Wasserman, S. (1985). Object permanence in 5-month-old infants. Cognition, 20(3), 191–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blake, P. R., & Harris, P. L. (2009). Children’s understanding of ownership transfers. Cognitive Development, 24(2), 133–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1990). Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 54, 86–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacchione, T., Schaub, S., & Rakoczy, H. (2013). Fourteen-month-old infants infer the continuous identity of objects on the basis of non-visible causal properties. Developmental Psychology, 49(7), 1325–1329. doi:10.1037/a0029746.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1996). Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment under uncertainty. Cognition, 58(1), 1–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denison, S., & Xu, F. (2010). Twelve- to 14-month-old infants can predict single-event probability with large set sizes. Developmental Science, 13(5), 798–803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. (2003). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. (2004). Psychological essentialism in children. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 404–409. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Hoffrage, U. (1995). How to improve Bayesian reasoning without instruction: Frequency formats. Psychological Review, 102(4), 684–704.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gómez, J. C. (2008). The Evolution Of Pretence: From Intentional Availability To Intentional Non-Existence. Mind & Language, 23(5), 586–606. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.2008.00353.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gómez, J. C., & Martin-Andrade, B. (2002). Possible precursors of pretend play in nonpretend actions of captive gorillas (Gorilla gorilla). In R. Mitchel (Ed.), Pretending and imagination in animals and children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (2012). Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory theory. Psychological Bulletin, 138(6), 1085–1108. doi:10.1037/a0028044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hindriks, F. (2009). Constitutive Rules, Language, and Ontology. Erkenntnis, 71(2), 253–275. doi:10.1007/s10670-009-9178-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. (1989). Concepts, kinds and cognitive development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S., & Kalish, C. W. (2009). Children’s ascriptions of property rights with changes of ownership. Cognitive Development, 24(3), 322–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korsgaard, C. M. (1996). The sources of normativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lohse, K., Gräfenhain, M., Behne, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2014). Young Children Understand the Normative Implications of Future-Directed Speech Acts. PLoS ONE, 9(1), e86958. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0086958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mendes, N., Hanus, D., & Call, J. (2008a). Raising the level: Orangutans use water as a tool. Biology Letters, 3, 453–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mendes, N., Rakoczy, H., & Call, J. (2008b). Ape metaphysics: Object individuation without language. Cognition, 106(2), 730–749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mendes, N., Rakoczy, H., & Call, J. (2011). Primates do not spontaneously use shape properties for object individuation: A competence or a performance problem? Animal Cognition, 14, 407–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(2), 109–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence. New York: Basic Books.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1975). The origin of the idea of chance in children. (Trans L. Leake et al.): (1975).

  • Price, E., & Whiten, A. (2012). Social Learning and Culture in Primates: Evidence from Free-Ranging and Captive PopulationsSocial learning in primates. In E. A. Wasserman & T. R. Zentall (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Cognition (pp. 862–880). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1975). The Meaning of ‘Meaning’. In K. Gunderson (Ed.), Language, Mind and Knowledge (pp. 131–193). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quine, W. V. O. (1957). Speaking of objects. Proceedings and Addresses of the American Philosophical Association, 31, 5–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H. (2006). Pretend play and the development of collective intentionality. Cognitive Systems Research, 7, 113–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H. (2008a). Pretence as individual and collective intentionality. Mind and Language, 23(5), 499–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H. (2008b). Taking fiction seriously: Young children understand the normative structure of joint pretend games. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 1195–1201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H., Brosche, N., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Young children’s understanding of the context-relativity of normative rules in conventional games. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 445–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H., & Cacchione, T. (2014). The developmental and evolutionary origins of psychological essentialism lie in sortal object individuation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

  • Rakoczy, H., Clüver, A., Saucke, L., Stoffregen, N., Gräbener, A., Migura, J., & Call, J. (2014b). Apes are intuitive statisticians. Cognition, 131(1), 60–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H., & Schmidt, M. F. H. (2013). The early ontogeny of social norms. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1),17-21.doi:10.1111/cdep.12010.

  • Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Two-year-olds grasp the intentional structure of pretense acts. Developmental Science, 9(6), 557–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2007). The ontogeny of social ontology: Steps to shared intentionality and status functions. In S. L. Tsohatzidis (Ed.), Intentional Acts and Institutional Facts: Essays on John Searle’s Social Ontology (pp. 113–137). Berlin: Springer Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Done wrong or said wrong? Young children understand the normative directions of fit of different speech acts. Cognition, 13(2), 205–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2004). Young children know that trying is not pretending - a test of the “behaving-as-if” contrual of children’s early concept of “pretense”. Developmental Psychology, 40(3), 388–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2005). On tools and toys: How children learn to act on and pretend with ‘virigin’ objects. Developmental Science, 8(1), 57–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The sources of normativity: Young children’s awareness of the normative structure of games. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 875–881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawls, J. (1955). Two concepts of rules. The Philosophical Review, 64, 3–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rossano, F., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young children’s understanding of violations of property rights. Cognition, 121, 219–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudolf von Rohr, C., Burkart, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (2011). Evolutionary precursors of social norms in chimpanzees: a new approach. Biology and Philosophy, 26(1), 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, M., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young children attribute normativity to novel actions without pedagogy or normative language. Developmental Science, 14(3), 530–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, M., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Young children enforce social norms selectively depending on the violator’s group affiliation. Cognition, 124(3), 325–333. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: An essay on the philosophy of language. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1995). The construction of social reality. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (2001). Rationality in Action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (2005). What is an institution. Journal of Institutional Economics, 1(1), 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (2010). Making the social world. The structure of human civilization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Snare, F. (1972). The concept of property. American Philosophical Quarterly, 9(2), 200–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strawson, P. F. (1959). Individuals: An essay in descriptive metaphysics. London: Methuen.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Téglás, E., Girotto, V., Gonzalez, M., & Bonatti, L. L. (2007). Intuitions of probabilities shape expectations about the future at 12 months and beyond. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(48), 19156–19159. doi:10.1073/pnas.0700271104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Rakoczy, H. (2003). What makes human cognition unique? From individual to shared to collective intentionality. Mind and Language, 18(2), 121–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: morality and convention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walton, K. L. (1990). Mimesis as make-believe. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiggins, D. (1997). Sortal concepts: A reply to Xu. Mind and Language, 12(3–4), 413–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wyman, E., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Normativity and context in young children’s pretend play. Cognitive Development, 24(2), 146–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F. (1997). From Lot’s wife to a pillar of sand: Evidence that physical object is a sortal concept. Mind and Language, 12(3/4), 365–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F. (2002). The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts in infancy. Cognition, 85(3), 223–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F. (2007). Sortal concepts, object individuation, and language. [References]. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 400–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F., & Carey, S. (1996). Infants″ metaphysics: The case of numerical identity. Cognitive Psychology, 30(2), 111–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F., Carey, S., & Quint, N. (2004). The emergence of kind-based object individuation in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 49(2), 155–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F., & Garcia, V. (2008). Intuitive statistics by 8-month-old infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(13), 5012–5015. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704450105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hannes Rakoczy.

Additional information

Some of the material presented in this paper was previously used in Rakoczy & Schmidt (2013).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rakoczy, H. Comparative metaphysics: the development of representing natural and normative regularities in human and non-human primates. Phenom Cogn Sci 14, 683–697 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9406-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9406-7

Keywords

Navigation