Skip to main content
Log in

Attention, ritual glitches, and attentional pull: the president and the queen

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article proposes an analysis of a ritual glitch and resulting “misfire” from the standpoint of a phenomenologically informed anthropology of human interaction. Through articulating a synthesis of some of Husserl‘s insights on attention and affection with concepts and methods developed by anthropologists and other students of human interaction, a case is made for the importance of understanding the social organization of attention in ritual encounters. An analysis of a failed toast during President Obama’s 2011 State Visit to the United Kingdom is used to illustrate how attention is directed toward certain participants, actions, and objects – as opposed to others. Affect-loaded empathic reactions are explained by the protracted temporal unfolding of an action whose successful conclusion – or “repair” - is ostensively and publicly delayed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For the purposes of this article we will consider the toast as one single speech act, even though when a toast is as elaborate as the one performed by President Obama the verbal and non-verbal sequence of acts could be seen as a combination of a series of different types of speech acts, which are subsumed under the speech act “toast.” For an analysis of toasts in informal settings, see Solomon (1999).

  2. In the rest of the article, following common usage in the popular press and taking advantage of the fact that there is one and only one “queen” that we write about here, we will refer to Queen Elizabeth II as “the Queen.” As for President Obama, we will alternate among a variety of referential expressions, including “The U.S. President,” “President Obama,” and “the President.” The use of the last name was felt appropriate and in some cases necessary given that Queen Elizabeth has interacted in her life with other eleven U.S. Presidents, including George W. Bush, whose toast to the Queen will be briefly discussed in this article.

  3. As readers familiar with Heidegger’s writings will immediately see, we have incorporated several of Heidegger’s insights into the different ways in which human relate to equipment or, in Heidegger’s language, equipment reveals itself to human users, but we have taken the liberty to remix some of the features of his three-way distinction among conspicuousness, obtrusiveness, and obstinacy (Heidegger 1962, §16, and footnote 1 on p. 104) to fit our own goals, which include a concern for the degree of human agency in a world populated by both people and things and not as much interest in the implications of tool-use in the history of western metaphysics.

  4. One of our reviewers insightfully noted that this form of attentional pull may indeed generatively contribute to debates in the literature over how best to conceptualize the role of emotions in the experience of empathy. As she or he noted, while many scholars (see Decety 2011, D’Vignemont and Jacob 2012, Snow 2000) wish to “restrict empathy to cases in which the target experiences some emotion,” many phenomenologists tend “not to restrict empathy in this way” (see Gallagher 2999, Zahavi 2010). Significantly, the perspective advanced in this article holds that the attentional pull of others’ emotions may importantly modulate our empathic engagement with them as differing aspects of their first-person perspective on the world are, in the process, made increasingly salient to us.

  5. From our perspective, empathy must be understood as a process that is temporally arrayed, intersubjectively constituted, and culturally patterned. As a mode of reciprocal attunement, empathy necessarily arises within a fundamental asymmetry of perspectives. The persistence of intersubjective asymmetry is what in fact distinguishes empathic experiences from other forms of co-existence. As emphasized by Husserl (e.g., 1989: 177; see also Stein 1989: 10), for the empathizer, empathy is a primordial disclosing of non-primordial experience, that is, experiences lived through by another. Along these lines, Throop has argued elsewhere that “[i] t is never the first-person experiences as actually lived through by an empathizee that are ‘seen’ by an empathizer in an act of empathy. Nor is it their expressive acts qua acts that are. Instead, it is the disclosed experiences of the other ‘shining through’ (Husserl 2001b) such expressions that constitute an empathic experience of another” (Throop 2012a: 412; see also Zahavi 2010).

  6. http://www.royal.gov.uk/LatestNewsandDiary/StateVisitprogrammes/2011/Sta/TheStateBanquet.aspx

  7. The video clip of the toast was shown to colleagues on two occasions, at a workshop at UCLA Center on Language, Interaction and Culture in February 2012 and at the Conference on Empathy organized by Dan Zahavi at the University of Copenhagen in October 2012.

  8. http://www.washingtonpost.com/blogs/reliable-source/post/burnt-toast-how-obama-bungled-his-salute-to-the-queen/2011/05/26/AGtv3HCH_blog.html

  9. http://www.mediaite.com/tv/president-obama-awkwardly-flubs-toast-to-the-queen/

  10. http://www.whitehouse.gov/the-press-office/2011/05/24/remarks-president-obama-and-her-majesty-queen-united-kingdom-dinner-toas

  11. Following common practice in the media, hereafter we will simply write “Camilla” to refer to Camilla Parker Bowles, Duchess of Cornwall.

  12. In focusing on empathic assessments in such media reactions, we do not explicitly address the possible influence of race and racialization in the context of our analysis. For excellent linguistic anthropological discussions of media responses to Obama's Presidency that could help better illuminate the potential impact of racial bias in amplifying, and in part shaping, these characterizations of the mishap in the media (especially in the US), see Alim and Smitherman (2012) and Jackson (In Press).

  13. http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=7ZCsfyaOGdw

  14. http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=pNRXGRFJdDY

  15. http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Tla7L-dIAL4

References

  • Alim, S. H.. & Smitherman, G. (2012). Articulate while black: Barack Obama, Language, and race in the U.S. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Austin, J.L. (1975). How to do things with words. Second edition. J.O. Urmson and Marina Sbis, Eds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Bégout, B. (2000). La Généalogie do la Logique. Husserl, l’antéprédicatif et le catégorial. Paris: Vrin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, C. (2009). Ritual theory, ritual practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, B. (2012). Reflections of self from missing things: How people move on from losing possessions. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 642, 228–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a theory of practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (2003). Pointing and placing. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: where language, culture, and cognition meet. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Andrade, R. (1995). The development of cognitive anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • D’Vignemont, F., & Jacob, P. (2012). What is it like to feel another’s pain?”. Philosophy of Science, 79(2), 295–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J. (2011). Neuroscience of empathic responding. In S. Brown, L. Perner, & R. Brown (Eds.), Self interest and beyond: Towards a New understanding of human caregiving. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Depraz, N. (2004). Where is the phenomenology of attention that Husserl intended to perform? A transcendental pragmatic-oriented description of attention. Continental Philosophy Review, 37(1), 5–20.

  • Desjarlais, R., & Throop, J. (2011). Phenomenological approaches in anthropology. Annual Review of Anthropology, 40, 87–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1922). Human nature and conduct: An introduction to social psychology. London: Allen and Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drewal, M. T. (1992). Yoruba ritual: Performers, play, agency. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (1986). The audience as co-author: an introduction. Text, 6–3, 239–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (1994). From grammar to politics: Linguistic anthropology in a western samoan village. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (1997a). Linguistic anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (1997b). Universal and cultural-specific properties of greetings. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 7, 63–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (2003). The voice of the audience in contemporary american political discourse. In D. Tannen & J. Alatis (Eds.), Linguistics, language, and the real world: Discourse and beyond: Georgetown University roundtable in language and linguistics 2001. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (2004). “Agency in Language.” In A. Duranti, ed. A Companion to Linguistic Anthropology. Malden, Mass: Blackwell. Pp. 451–73.

  • Duranti, A. (2009). The relevance of Husserl’s theory to language socialization. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 19(2), 205–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (2010). Husserl, intersubjectivity, and anthropology. Anthropological Theory, 10(1), 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (2015). The anthropology of intentions: Language in a world of others. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, Alessandro and Steven Black. (2012). “Socialization and Improvisation.” In A. Duranti, E. Ochs, and B. Schieffelin, eds. Handbook of Language Socialization. Malden, Mass: Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Duranti, A., Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (Eds.). (2012). Handbook of language socialization. Malden, Mass: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1976). Witchcraft, oracles, and magic among the azande. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (1986) Empathy, simulation, and narrative. Science in Context 25(3): 355–381

  • Gluckman, M. (1954). Rituals of rebellion in South-East Africa. Manchester: Manchester University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1963). Behavior in public places: Notes on the social organization of gathering. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1981). Conversational organization: Interaction between speakers and hearers. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, M. H. (1990). He-said-She-said: Talk as social organization among black children. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1994). Professional vision. American Anthropologist, 96(3), 606–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2003). Pointing as situated practice. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture and cognition meet. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, M. H. (2006). The hidden life of girls: Games of stance, status, and exclusion. Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C., & Duranti, A. (1992). Rethinking context: An introduction. In A. Duranti & C. Goodwin (Eds.), Rethinking context: Language as an interactive phenomenon (pp. 1–42). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1962). Being and Time. John Macquarrie & Edward Robinson (eds). New York: Harper & Row, Publishers.

  • Heritage, J., & Clayman, S. (2010). Talk in action: Interactions, identities, and institutions. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hollan, D., & Throop, J. (2008). Whatever happened to empathy?”. Ethos, 36(4), 385–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollan, D., & Throop, J. (2011). The anthropology of empathy: Introduction. In D. Hollan & J. Throop (Eds.), The anthropology of empathy: Experiencing the lives of others in pacific societies. New York: Berghahn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1962). Ideas: General introduction to pure phenomenology. New York: Collier Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, Edmund. 1964. The Phenomenology of Internal Time-Consciousness. Translated by James S. Churchill. Edited by Martin Heidegger. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

  • Husserl, Edmund. 1989. Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and to Phenomenological Philosophy – Second Book. R. Rojcewicz and A. Schuwer trans. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

  • Husserl, E. (2001a). Analysis concerning the active and passive synthesis. A.J. Steinbock (trans). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (2001b). Logical investigations. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irvine, J. T. (1979). Formality and informality in communicative events. American Anthropologist, 81(4), 773–790.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, J. (In Press) Structural differentiation and the poetics of violence shaping Barack Obama’s presidency: A study in personhood, literacy, and the improvisation of African-American publics.” Language sciences.

  • Keating, E. (1998). Power sharing: Language, rank, gender and social space in pohnpei, micronesia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence, D. & S. M. Low. (1990)The Built Environment and Spatial Form. Annual Review of Anthropology, Vol 19: 453–505.

  • Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K. M., & Throop, C.J. (Eds.). (2010). Toward an anthropology of the will. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

  • Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (1984). Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories and their implications. In R. Shweder & R. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays in mind, self, an emotion (pp. 276–320). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (2012). The theory of language socialization. In A. Duranti, E. Ochs, & B. Schieffelin (Eds.), The handbook of language socialization (pp. 1–21). Wiley-Blackwell: Malden, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortner, S. (2006). Anthropology and social theory: Culture, power, and the acting subject. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, L. (Ed.). (1995). Other intentions: Cultural context and the attribution of inner states. Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H. (1975). Everyone Has to Lie. In M. Sanches & B. G. Blout (Eds.), Sociocultural dimensions of language Use. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H. (1992). Lectures on Conversation, Volumes I & II. G. Jefferson, editor. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H., Schegloff, E., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50(4), 696–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sawyer, R. K. (1996). The semiotics of improvisation: the pragmatics of musical and verbal performance. Semiotica, 108(3/4), 269–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sawyer, R. K. (2001). Creating conversations: Improvisation in everyday discourse. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sawyer, R. K. (2003). Group creativity: Music, theater, collaboration. Mahawah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1992). Repair after next turn: the last structurally provided defense of intersubjectivity in conversation. American Journal of Sociology, 97(5), 1295–1345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1979). “The Relevance of Repair for Syntax-for-Conversation.” Pp. 261–88 in Syntax and Semantics 12: Discourse and Syntax, edited by Talmy Givon. New York: Academic Press.

  • Schegloff, E. A., Jefferson, G., & Sacks, H. (1977). The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language, 53, 361–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, N. E. (2000). Empathy. American Philosophical Quarterly, 37(1), 65–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, O. (1999). Casting the future: discourse and temporality in Russian toast performances. Crossroads of Language, Interaction, and Culture, 1, 3–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, E. (1989). On the problem of empathy. Washington, DC: ICS Publications.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Steinbock, A. J. (1995). Home and beyond: generative phenomenology after Husserl. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J. (2003). Articulating experience. Anthropological Theory, 3(2), 219–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J. (2010a). Suffering and sentiment: Exploring the vicissitudes of pain and experience in yap. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J. (2010b). Latitudes of loss: on the vicissitudes of empathy. American Ethnologist, 37(4), 771–782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J. (2012a). On the varieties of empathic experience: tactility, mental opacity, and pain in Yap. Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 26(3), 408–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J. (2012b). On inaccessibility and vulnerability: some horizons of compatibility between phenomenology and psychoanalysis. Ethos, 40(1), 75–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J. (2012c). Moral sentiments. In D. Fassin (Ed.), A companion to moral anthropology (pp. 150–168). Wiley-Blackwell: Malden, MA.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J. (2014). Moral moods. Ethos, 42(1), 65–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J., & Murphy, K. M. (2002). Bourdieu and phenomenology: a critical assessment. Anthropological Theory, 2(2), 185–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, V. (1969). The ritual process: Structure and anti-structure. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, V. (1974). Dramas, fields and metaphors: Symbolic action in human society. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Gennep, A. (1960). The rites of passage. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waldenfels, B. (2011). Phenomenology of the alien: Basic concepts. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. (2010). Empathy, embodiment and interpersonal understanding: from Lipps to schutz. Inquiry, 53(3), 285–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to C. Jason Throop.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Throop, C.J., Duranti, A. Attention, ritual glitches, and attentional pull: the president and the queen. Phenom Cogn Sci 14, 1055–1082 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9397-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9397-4

Keywords

Navigation