Introduction

Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a chronic, autoimmune disease which affects approximately 36 per 100,000 people worldwide (Walton et al., 2020). The most common disease course, relapsing-remitting MS (RRMS), constitutes 85% of cases and is marked by distinct episodes of symptom relapse and remission (Leray et al., 2016), while the other disease courses involve progressive worsening of symptoms. Although the most prominent symptoms are in sensorimotor function, increasing emphasis has been placed over recent decades on cognitive effects. Cognitive impairment (CIm) may be a particularly disabling consequence of MS, affecting employment status (Clemens & Langdon, 2018), medication adherence (Bruce et al., 2010), management of finances (Goverover et al., 2019), social functioning (Rao et al., 1991a), and ability to perform activities of daily living (Yazgan et al., 2021). Preservation of cognition is frequently endorsed as a priority among people with MS (Day et al., 2018; Heesen et al., 2008), with one qualitative study highlighting that people with MS were supportive of routine cognitive testing as a means to document an under-addressed, ‘invisible’ symptom, and to advance research in this area (Mortensen et al., 2020).

Despite acceptance that CIm is a potential symptom of MS, estimates of impairment prevalence are equivocal and have not been explored meta-analytically or specifically for people with RRMS. Estimates frequently cited in the literature are broad in range, such as the 40–65% range reported in a seminal literature review (k = 11, n = 640) by Amato and colleagues in 2006 (Amato et al., 2006), and do not provide a specific overall prevalence estimate. Additionally, the reviewed studies are now many decades old and do not utilize current best practice methods or tests for assessing CIm prevalence (Parsons et al., 1957). Moreover, the samples within studies which sought to estimate CIm prevalence within the MS population are largely unrepresentative as they are small (n < 100) or recruited from community samples (McIntosh-Michaelis et al., 1991; Rao et al., 1991a) which may vary in CIm prevalence to clinic-based samples (Amato et al., 2006). Furthermore, these studies did not differentiate between those with RRMS and the progressive courses of MS, which typically involve more severe cognitive dysfunction (Johnen et al., 2017). Thus, the current literature lacks a precise, recent, and representative estimate of the overall prevalence of CIm among people with RRMS. This estimate would be of great interest to neurologists and other health professionals involved in the care of people with RRMS, as many newly diagnosed people with RRMS report concerns about their cognition (Day et al., 2018; Heesen et al., 2008). An updated and more rigorous estimate of CIm prevalence may increase confidence in patient communication regarding the risk of developing CIm. This estimate may also guide service provision by informing resource allocation within MS health care settings toward cognitive health.

The aim of this study was to estimate the pooled prevalence of CIm in RRMS through a systematic review and meta-analysis of the literature. The primary outcome of interest was CIm prevalence as determined through standardized neuropsychological testing, where at least two cognitive domains (e.g., attention, memory, executive functioning) must be reduced to meet criteria for CIm. This is not only considered best practice (Hancock et al., 2022) but also allows exclusion of studies which only examine one cognitive domain, given that domains are differentially affected in MS (Prakash et al., 2008) and single-domain studies are thus likely to misrepresent the overall CIm prevalence. This restriction was also adopted to increase the precision of the prevalence estimate, particularly as previous meta-analyses estimating CIm prevalence in other conditions have found the high variability of definitions across studies to be a barrier to interpretation of their findings (Rayes et al., 2018; Sexton et al., 2019; Yohannes et al., 2017). However, other definitional aspects of CIm, such as the standards deemed to indicate impaired test performance, also have the potential to impact estimates of prevalence. Thus, a secondary aim of this study was to review the definitions of CIm used and examine whether this, in addition to clinical, demographic, and methodological variables, moderates reported prevalence rates.

Method

Registration

This study was conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) checklist. The protocol was prospectively published on PROSPERO (CRD42021281815).

Eligibility Criteria

Studies were included if they determined the prevalence of CIm using standardized neuropsychological measures among adults (at least 18 years) diagnosed with RRMS, and reported the proportion of participants who were impaired in two or more cognitive domains. Also eligible were studies which used more than one test to measure a single cognitive domain, and defined CIm as two tests in the impaired range spread across at least two different domains.

Exclusion criteria were studies which (1) only used a cognitive screening measure rather than a neuropsychological test battery, e.g., Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) or Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE), given reports that screens have limited sensitivity for detecting CIm in MS (Portaccio et al., 2009a, b; Sehanovic et al., 2020); (2) used self-report rating scales to determine CIm; (3) only required impairment in one cognitive domain for CIm and did not report proportion of participants impaired in two or more domains; (4) aggregated performances over multiple cognitive domains to a single index, as it was possible for individuals with an impairment in only one domain to meet CIm criteria under these conditions; (5) recruited participants with various disease courses and did not report the CIm prevalence for those with RRMS; (6) included participants with pediatric-onset MS; and (7) gauged participants’ cognitive status to determine their eligibility to participate (e.g., required a minimum score on a cognitive screen, only recruited people with cognitive complaints).

Where the same cohort was examined across multiple timepoints, only the publication associated with data collected from the first timepoint was included. Additional data were requested from corresponding authors where relevant.

Search Strategy and Study Selection

Embase, Scopus, Medline, and PsycINFO were searched from inception to March 2023, with no restriction to sample size or the year or country of publication. The search strategy (Fig. 1 of the Supplement) combined keywords and Medical Subject Heading terms related to (1) multiple sclerosis; (2) CIm; and (3) neuropsychological testing. Duplicates were removed using an automated EndNote function.

One reviewer (W. W.) screened all titles and abstracts using the application Rayyan (Ouzzani et al., 2016) and a random 20% was independently assessed by a second reviewer (T. W.) to verify the accuracy and consistency of the screening process. Full-text articles were independently assessed by reviewers (W. W. and T. W.) using a standardized eligibility spreadsheet, with excellent agreement (κ = .84). Studies published in non-English languages were reviewed following translation with Google Translate, which was effective for translating articles to review information relevant to the current study. Discrepancies were resolved by consensus, including with a third reviewer (H. F.).

Data Extraction

A standardized data extraction spreadsheet was used by two independent reviewers (W. W. and T. W.). The primary outcome extracted was the number of participants with RRMS deemed to have CIm. Where multiple prevalence rates were reported according to different CIm criteria, the rate corresponding with the authors’ primary definition of CIm was extracted. Other extracted data included study characteristics (country, recruitment setting, RRMS sample size), demographic and clinical variables (age, gender, disease duration, Expanded Disability Status Scale [EDSS]), and details about the measurement and definition of CIm (neuropsychological tests, cut-off used to indicate test impairment, number of impaired domains required for CIm, and whether healthy controls, normative samples, or premorbid estimates were used as the comparison group).

Study quality was independently assessed by two reviewers (W. W. and T. W.) according to the modified version of the Quality Assessment of Diagnostic Accuracy Studies (QUADAS) tool (Broen et al., 2012; Broen et al., 2016; Leboeuf-Yde & Lauritsen, 1995), available in Table 1 of the Supplement. The QUADAS has been recommended by the Cochrane Collaboration (Whiting et al., 2011), and was modified by Broen and colleagues (2012) according to criteria developed by Leboeuf-Yde and Lauritsen (1995) and Walker and colleagues (2000) to increase applicability for prevalence studies. It includes 10 criteria regarding participant representativeness, data quality, description of method and results, and definition of prevalence. Points are allocated for each criterion, which are then summed into a model-free count score without estimation of a statistical model (Scherer & Emslander, 2024). A maximum score of 19 points can be earned, with a score of 14 or higher indicating acceptable quality (Broen et al., 2012).

Statistical Analyses

Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (CMA) version 3.0 (Borenstein et al., 2013) was used to conduct analyses and generate plots. Following logit transformations of the prevalence rates, the pooled prevalence of CIm was estimated using a random-effect model with 95% confidence intervals (CIs). The logit-transformed prevalence rates were back-transformed into proportions to aid interpretability and ease of reporting. Between-study heterogeneity was assessed using the I2 statistic, with values above 40% considered to indicate moderate heterogeneity and values above 60% considered substantial (Deeks et al., 2019).

Meta-regression and subgroup analyses were conducted to determine moderators of CIm prevalence. Continuous variables of interest were year of publication, RRMS sample size, mean age, mean disease duration, proportion of female participants, and number of neuropsychological tests administered. Categorical variables were aspects of the definition of CIm used, including the cut-off used to determine impaired test performance (1.5 standard deviations [SDs], 1.67 SDs, or 2 SDs below the comparator’s mean), number of impaired tests required (2 or >2), and comparison group (normative values or healthy controls). As no studies used premorbid estimates, we were unable to include this as a level within subgroup analyses. Many studies reported EDSS using medians rather than means. We extracted whichever was reported and recoded this to a categorical variable. As no studies reported EDSS scores corresponding to severe neurological disability and only four studies reported scores in the moderate range, those which reported EDSS below 2.0 (i.e., no disability) were recoded as lower EDSS and those 2.0 or greater were recoded as higher EDSS (Kurtzke, 1983). Study quality was also recoded to a categorical variable as it was inappropriate for meta-regression given the modified QUADAS yield model-free sum scores (Scherer & Emslander, 2024) and due to the restricted scores obtained. Majority of studies earned the same score of 12/20, with only one study meeting the minimum acceptable score of 14/20. Hence, studies with scores below 12 were recoded as lower quality and scores at or above 12 were recoded as higher quality, and were subject to subgroup analyses to identify whether the studies which earned a score lower than the mode of 12 reported different prevalence rates to the majority.

Risk of publication bias was assessed using a funnel plot and Egger’s test. Funnel plots which appear asymmetric and significant Egger’s tests are suggestive of publication bias. The trim-and-fill procedure was used to determine whether removal of any studies would improve funnel plot symmetry (Duval & Tweedie, 2000).

Results

The search yielded 21,878 articles, of which 13,185 were duplicates. Two additional articles were sourced from correspondence with authors and reference lists. Of the 8695 titles and abstracts screened for eligibility, 183 were sought for full-text review. Fifty studies, which included data from 5859 people with RRMS, met the inclusion criteria and were included in the analyses (reference list in Fig. 2 of the Supplement). Details of the selection process are available in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart of the selection process for studies

Study Characteristics

Characteristics of included studies are available in Table 1. Year of publication ranged from 2004 to 2023. All studies which reported the location where participants were recruited did so from a clinical setting (n = 43; 86%), most commonly a MS center (n = 32; 64%), with one study utilizing both clinical and community sampling. Thirty-one studies (62%) solely recruited participants with RRMS, and the remainder recruited mixed samples with different disease courses. Some mixed sample studies did not report demographic data for the participants with RRMS; these studies were included in the overall CIm prevalence estimate but were unable to be included in the moderator analyses if they did not report the relevant moderator.

Table 1 Characteristics of included studies

Demographic and clinical characteristics were variably reported using means or medians, and some studies which recruited mixed samples did not report summary statistics for the subset of participants with RRMS. Thirty-eight (76%) studies reported sex of RRMS participants, with 69% of these being female overall. Mean age of RRMS participants was reported or calculable in 36 (72%) studies and ranged from 30.5 to 58.1 years. Mean disease duration of RRMS participants was reported or calculable in 33 (66%) studies and ranged from 2.1 to 27.0 years. Of studies which used the Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS) to measure neurological disability, means of RRMS participants were reported or calculable in 26 (52%) studies and ranged from 1.3 to 4.2, and medians were reported in 11 (22%) studies and ranged from 1.5 to 3.5.

Many studies used standardized batteries developed for use in MS populations. The most common was the Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N; Bever et al., 1995) used in 29 (58%) studies, which served as a standalone battery for determining CIm in 12 of these. Seven (14%) studies used the Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Function in MS (MACFIMS; Benedict et al., 2002), three (6%) used the Brief International Cognitive Assessment for MS (BICAMS; Benedict et al., 2012), and two (4%) used the Neuropsychological Screening Battery for MS (NSBMS; Bobholz & Rao, 2003). Forty-eight studies (96%) included a verbal fluency measure, though the type (e.g., phonemic or semantic fluency) differed across studies. Beyond this, the most frequently used test was the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) used in 44 (88%) studies, followed by the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) used in 43 (86%) studies. The number of tests administered ranged from 3 to 16. Most studies administered five (k = 13; 26%), six (k = 16; 32%), or seven (k = 10; 20%) tests in total, with seven studies (14%) administering more than six tests and four studies (8%) administering fewer than five tests.

The definitions of CIm varied. Selected studies used one of three cut-off points to indicate impaired performance on a test: 17 (34%) studies used a cut-off of 1.5 SDs (i.e., 7th percentile) below the comparator group’s mean, 17 (34%) used a cut-off of 1.67 SDs (i.e., 5th percentile), and 16 (32%) used a cut-off of 2 SDs (i.e., 2nd percentile). Studies differed in whether they described their criteria as dependent on the number of ‘tests’ or ‘domains’ which were impaired. In some cases, this terminology was functionally equivalent; for instance, many studies used standardized batteries which allocate the same test(s) to each cognitive domain. Most studies (n = 38; 76%) required impairment in at least two tests or cognitive domains for a participant to be considered impaired, while six (12%) required three tests, and one (2%) required four. Five (10%) studies only required one impaired test for CIm but reported the number of participants with two or more impaired tests; this data was extracted for the analyses. Thirty-five (70%) studies compared RRMS participants’ performances against published normative values and the remaining 15 (30%) recruited healthy controls.

Study Quality

Study quality was generally low when examined according to criteria developed for prevalence studies (Table 2 of the Supplement). Only one study (2%) achieved the minimum score for acceptable quality on the modified QUADAS (i.e., 14/19; Broen et al., 2016). There was little variation in scores, with most studies (k = 37; 74%) earning 12 points generally across the same criteria. Several details regarding the representativeness of the sample were rarely reported: no studies used random sampling, only one study (2%) made a statement to indicate the representativeness of their sample, one study (2%) described recruitment non-responders or reasons for non-responding, and three studies (6%) reported the response rate.

CIm Prevalence

The pooled prevalence of CIm (defined as evidence of relatively reduced performance across at least two cognitive domains) was 32.5% (95% CI 29.3–36.0%) across 5859 people with RRMS. There was substantial heterogeneity between studies (Q49 = 309.62, p < .001, I2 = 84.17, τ2 = 0.23), with reported prevalence rates ranging from 10.2 to 60.0%. The forest plot depicting prevalence and 95% CIs is displayed in Fig. 2. The funnel plot (Fig. 3 of the Supplement) was generally symmetric, Egger’s test was non-significant (p = .104), and the trim-and-fill procedure did not suggest trimming any studies; thus, there was no evidence of publication bias.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Forest plot of prevalence rates and weights of selected studies

Moderators of CIm Prevalence

Results of moderator analyses are summarized in Table 2. Meta-regression analyses indicated that older age (k = 36, b = 0.034, Q1 = 5.66, p = .017) and greater disease duration (k = 33, b = 0.042, Q1 = 7.96, p = .005) were significantly associated with higher prevalence of CIm. Studies which administered more tests also reported significantly greater CIm prevalence (k = 50, b = 0.073, Q1 = 5.10, p = .024). There were no significant effects of year of publication (k = 50, b = −0.013, Q1 = 0.62, p = .431), RRMS sample size (k = 50, b = 0.000, Q1 = 0.02, p = .875), or the proportion of female participants (k = 38, b = −0.018, Q1 = 1.15, p = .283).

Table 2 Results of random effect meta-analysis and moderator analyses

Though there appeared to be a trend toward greater CIm prevalence in studies with a mean or median EDSS of 2.0 or above (36.7%; 95% CI 33.0–40.7%) compared with those with EDSS below 2.0 (29.6%; 95% CI 23.7–36.2%), subgroup analyses indicated that this was non-significant (k = 38, Q1 = 3.39, p = .066). There were no significant effects of the cut-off used to indicate impaired test performance (k = 50, Q2 = 2.14, p = .343), number of impaired tests to indicate CIm (k = 50, Q1 = 0.04, p = .845), the comparison group used (k = 50, Q1 = 0.64, p = .425), or study quality (k = 50, Q1 = 2.87, p = .090).

Discussion

The present meta-analysis estimated that approximately one third (32.5%) of adults with RRMS experience CIm. While previous meta-analytic findings have provided indication of the magnitude of CIm in RRMS (Prakash et al., 2008), our findings extend the literature by providing a quantitative estimate of the proportion of people with RRMS who are affected. Though lower than the 40–65% estimate reported by Amato and colleagues in 2006 (Amato et al., 2006), our estimate is an empirical analysis focused on RRMS which incorporated comprehensive data across 50 studies (n = 5859) and only included studies which adopted a stringent definition of CIm (Hancock et al., 2022).

There was substantial between-study heterogeneity, which was partially accounted for by clinical and demographic factors. Disease duration and age were associated with greater CIm prevalence, such that samples with longer disease durations and higher ages had greater CIm prevalence. These findings are consistent with a past meta-analysis (k = 57; n = 3891) by Prakash et al. (2008), which found that the magnitude of cognitive deficits (defined as the difference in cognitive performance relative to healthy controls) among people with RRMS was moderated by higher age, while greater disease duration moderated the magnitude of deficits in the domain of memory and learning only. The impact on disease duration and age on CIm prevalence is also consistent with expectations given that overall disability in RRMS tends to worsen over time even with treatment (Cree et al., 2016).

One challenge in collating CIm prevalence data across the literature are the disparate definitions of CIm used across studies. Although it would have been ideal to adopt one unified definition of CIm for the current study, this was not possible as prevalence rates for different definitions of CIm were rarely extractable from studies. We attempted to address this variability by including aspects of CIm definitions as moderators in our analyses. However, we found that differences in the definitions of CIm used across studies did not moderate prevalence rates. Specifically, prevalence rates did not vary significantly between studies which utilized cut-offs of 1.5, 1.67, or 2 SDs to indicate impaired test performance, or between those which required at least two impaired tests or at least three impaired tests for a participant to meet criteria for CIm. This may be due to our stringent and best-practice CIm definition (i.e., evidence of reduction in at least two cognitive domains) and exclusion of studies that only required impairment in one domain. Additionally, the prevalence rates reported in studies which compared RRMS participants against healthy controls (34.8%; 95% CI 28.2–41.9%) were not significantly different from those which used published normative values (31.6%; 95% CI 27.8–35.6%), supporting the use of published normative values as a valid comparison group for research purposes.

We also found that studies that used more extensive test batteries reported significantly higher CIm prevalence rates. This finding is somewhat unsurprising considering that the administration of more tests provides greater opportunity for a participant to be detected as having CIm. This is especially the case due to the definitions of CIm adopted in some research, for instance, if multiple tests are used to measure a single cognitive domain and reduction on any of these tests is considered sufficient to indicate impairment in that domain. We note that the majority of included studies administered tests relevant to cognitive changes in MS, such as the SDMT or PASAT, and many studies administered brief batteries developed specifically for people with MS (e.g., the BRB-N used in 58% of studies). While these tests and batteries have been validated to be sensitive to cognitive impairment in MS (Drake et al., 2010; Sumowski et al., 2018; Strober et al., 2009), the finding that a more comprehensive test battery is more likely detect cognitive deficits supports this as standard practice for neuropsychological assessment in clinical settings.

It was not possible to formally estimate domain-specific impairment rates across all participants as many studies did not report how many participants were impaired on each test or domain. Of the studies which did report the most frequently impaired domain, attention and information processing speed, typically measured by the SDMT and/or PASAT, was by far the most common (Altieri et al., 2021; Amato et al., 2008; Bisecco et al., 2015; Conti et al., 2021; d’Ambrosia et al., 2020; Deloire et al., 2005; Gois et al., 2021; Goretti et al, 2014; Jakimovski et al., 2019; Lozano-Soto, 2021; Mashayekhi et al., 2022; Portaccio et al., 2009a, 2009b; Preziosa et al., 2016; Rimkus et al., 2011; Rocca et al., 2014; Ruano et al., 2017; Sacco et al., 2015; Talebi et al., 2022), followed by verbal memory (Conti et al., 2021; Gajofatto et al., 2016; Goretti et al., 2014; Maubeuge et al., 2021; Skorve et al., 2023), and visual memory (Amato et al., 2004; Damasceno et al., 2020; Hulst et al., 2012), with few studies reporting executive function (Rimkus et al., 2019) and visuospatial functioning (Lanzillo et al., 2006) as most affected. This is consistent with other work indicating that attention and processing speed deficits are the most prevalent cognitive difficulty in MS (McNicholas et al., 2018).

There are several caveats to the representativeness of the estimate we obtained. Though we did not place restrictions on the recruitment setting when selecting studies, all included studies which reported this recruited from a clinical setting, and no studies based purely on community samples met inclusion criteria. Thus, our estimate is constrained to people with RRMS who attend clinical services and may not generalize to people with RRMS more generally. In light of this, the current meta-analysis may overestimate the prevalence of CIm, as people with concerns about their cognitive functioning may be more likely to present to a clinical setting, leading to selection bias (Abdelnour et al., 2017; Farias et al., 2009). Future research conducting formal neuropsychological testing in community-based samples is warranted. The scope of our study was also limited to adult-onset RRMS, and specific investigations into CIm prevalence in other MS subpopulations may be informative. Pediatric-onset MS comprises approximately 5% of all MS cases (Harding et al., 2013) and the risk of CIm may be greater than those with adult-onset MS, particularly on measures sensitive to processing speed deficits such as the SDMT and PASAT (McKay et al., 2019; Ruano et al., 2018). Given evidence of greater magnitude of CIm in the progressive disease courses (Johnen et al., 2017; Planche et al., 2016), CIm prevalence among these groups is also likely to differ from that in RRMS. Furthermore, subjective CIm may be an important aspect of patient experience distinct from objective CIm (Hughes et al., 2019; Julian et al., 2007; Kinsinger et al., 2010; Mortensen et al., 2020), which requires future examination.

Our ability to perform meta-regression analyses was also affected by the restricted ranges of obtained scores for some variables, specifically EDSS and study quality. Additionally, the study quality tool used was limited by the heterogeneity of its criteria and could only be used to create a statistical model-free count score rather than a model-based scale (Scherer & Emslander, 2024). We were thus required to generate somewhat crude binary classifications to make these variables suitable for subgroup analyses. Neither study quality nor EDSS were significant moderators in the present study. Future work which estimates CIm prevalence could improve in quality by inclusion of information regarding the possibility of recruitment bias, such as the number of people who were approached but did not participate in the study and reasons for non-participation. Potential relationships between neurological disability and CIm may also be more effectively captured by reporting separate EDSS scores for subgroups of participants with impaired or preserved cognition, allowing for between-group comparisons to be extracted meta-analytically. Otherwise, future meta-analytic work in this area may be done using an individual participant data (IPD) approach, where original participant data is collected rather than aggregate data (Tierney et al., 2023). This approach allows for more rigorous examination of moderators and has been considered a ‘gold standard’ of systematic review.

It was notable that none of the selected studies included an estimate of premorbid function as their reference point for impaired cognition. Comparing participants’ test scores to those of other groups without consideration of decline for the individual may impact the CIm prevalence rates reported (Douglas et al., 2018; Tran et al., 2021). For instance, cognitively high-functioning individuals who experience a clinically significant decline from their premorbid abilities, but whose performances on testing do not fall below the chosen normative cut-off, will fail to be classified as having CIm (Sumowski et al., 2018). Conversely, those with a low level of premorbid cognitive functioning may experience relatively mild levels of decline or even no decline, but perform below the cut-off range on testing are deemed cognitively impaired. Use of premorbid estimates may improve the accuracy of prevalence estimates in future studies.

This work has several implications. Our finding that a third of adults with RRMS have CIm is lower than previous ranges cited in the literature for people with MS, such as the 40–65% range previously reported (Amato et al., 2006). This updated, rigorous, and narrower prevalence estimate may provide clinicians with increased confidence when communicating with patients regarding the risk of cognitive difficulties, which is often a concern for newly diagnosed people with MS who are typically of working age (Day et al., 2018; Heesen et al., 2008). Importantly, our estimate is specific to those with a relapsing-remitting disease course, who comprise the majority of MS cases, rather than previous estimates which do not distinguish between relapsing-remitting and progressive courses of MS which experience more severe CIm (Johnen et al., 2017). Our finding can also facilitate management of resource allocation. MS services can make provisions with the view that a third of RRMS patients are likely to require more intensive support for cognitive functioning, such as comprehensive assessment and access to cognitive rehabilitation services. This information may help to determine priorities for funding and staffing. Furthermore, this finding establishes a benchmark for future research into risk profiles, such as factors which predict CIm, to further initiatives into prevention and management.

Conclusion

Ultimately, our finding that approximately one third of adults with RRMS experience CIm, performing below the 7th percentile in two or more cognitive domains, is important for clinical practice. People with RRMS who have CIm are more vulnerable to poor functional outcomes than those without CIm (Bruce et al., 2010; Clemens & Langdon, 2018; Goverover et al., 2019; Rao et al., 1991b; Yazgan et al., 2021), and thus are likely to benefit from additional supports and provision of strategies to manage their symptoms. Routine cognitive testing may be appropriate for those with RRMS to identify such individuals, particularly as patient attitudes to this are positive (Mortensen et al., 2020) and it aligns with both patients’ and clinicians’ priorities for treatment (Singer et al., 2021).