Abstract
Cognitive training (CT) is an increasingly popular, non-pharmacological intervention for improving cognitive functioning in neurodegenerative diseases and healthy aging. Although meta-analyses support the efficacy of CT in improving cognitive functioning, the neural mechanisms underlying the effects of CT are still unclear. We performed a systematic review of literature in the PubMed, Embase and PsycINFO databases on controlled CT trials (N > 20) in aging and neurodegenerative diseases with pre- and post-training functional MRI outcomes up to November 23rd 2018 (PROSPERO registration number CRD42019103662). Twenty articles were eligible for our systematic review. We distinguished between multi-domain and single-domain CT. CT induced both increases and decreases in task-related functional activation, possibly indicative of an inverted U-shaped curve association between regional brain activity and task performance. Functional connectivity within ‘cognitive’ brain networks was consistently reported to increase after CT while a minority of studies additionally reported increased segregation of frontoparietal and default mode brain networks. Although we acknowledge the large heterogeneity in type of CT, imaging methodology, in-scanner task paradigm and analysis methods between studies, we propose a working model of the effects of CT on brain activity and connectivity in the context of current knowledge on compensatory mechanisms that are associated with aging and neurodegenerative diseases.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
In recent years, cognitive training (CT) has become increasingly popular as a treatment for cognitive dysfunction and decline. CT is a behavioral, non-pharmacological treatment that has a history of more than a century but has regained interest in the past two decades (Katz, Shah, & Meyer, 2018). Its scope nowadays ranges from alleviating cognitive dysfunction in neurodegenerative diseases, in which pharmacological treatment options have limited efficacy (Tan et al., 2014; Seppi et al., 2011), to improvement of cognitive abilities in cognitively intact individuals (see e.g., Shah, Weinborn, Verdile, Sohrabi, & Martins, 2017). The recent popularity of CT has evoked substantial debate and criticism from the scientific community regarding its efficacy and validity (van Heugten, Ponds, & Kessels, 2016; Orban, Rapport, Friedman, & Kofler, 2014; Goodier, 2009; Rabipour & Raz, 2012), at least partly due to the growth of commercial companies promising to enhance mental fitness or cure cognitive dysfunction in an aging society by offering online ‘brain training’ products without scientific basis.
Meta-analyses of studies in multiple neurodegenerative diseases support the efficacy of CT to improve cognitive function (Leung et al., 2015; Sitzer, Twamley, & Jeste, 2006; Chandler, Parks, Marsiske, Rotblatt, & Smith, 2016; Dardiotis et al., 2018). Even more so, CT has been shown to delay cognitive decline in both healthy adults and patients with Parkinson’s disease (e.g. Petrelli et al., 2014; Rebok et al., 2014; Willis et al., 2006). This indicates that CT may have a neuroprotective effect that counteracts or delays age- and disease-related degeneration of the brain and is reminiscent of the “use it or lose it” principle (Swaab, 1991; Hultsch, Hertzog, Small, & Dixon, 1999).
The aim of CT through ‘process-based’ or ‘drill’ training is, as generally opposed to cognitive strategy training, to improve cognition through repeated engagement of cognitive processes using one or more challenging and preferably adaptive tasks – analogous to physical training. Stimulation of neuroplasticity and thereby enhancement of cognitive reserve is thought to represent the underlying neurobiological mechanism (Park & Bischof, 2013; Raichlen & Alexander, 2017). Neuroplasticity refers to the brain’s ability to undergo structural and functional alterations by altering neurotransmission, synaptogenesis and neurogenesis from birth to old age (Mahncke, Bronstone, & Merzenich, 2006). Research in rodents has indicated that training-induced neuroplasticity entails changes in dendritic spine formation rate (Lai, Franke, & Gan, 2012), cortical spine density (Kuhlman, O'Connor, Fox, & Svoboda, 2014) and synapse potentiation (Xiong, Znamenskiy, & Zador, 2015; for a review see Abraham, 2008) and also neural changes such as hippocampal synaptic connectivity in animal models of neurodegenerative diseases (Stuart et al., 2017).
In humans, non-invasive neuroimaging techniques are an elegant way to map neural alterations in response to CT, but so far there is no agreement on how CT alters the aging brain at this macroscopic level. This review seeks to provide a systematic overview of reported changes in brain activity and connectivity induced by process-based CT, measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). As integration of information in large-scale brain networks is increasingly thought to be essential for successful cognition (Bassett & Sporns, 2017; Bullmore & Sporns, 2009), we aim to describe results in the context of the human brain connectome. This systematic review aims to answer the following questions: what is the influence of the trained domain (e.g., single-domain versus multi-domain, working memory versus attention) on alterations of brain activity and connectivity? Do these alterations specifically involve certain brain networks? Are the mechanisms of effect of CT dependent on the target study population?
Insight into the neural mechanisms of CT across neurodegenerative diseases and healthy elderly, and across training packages is warranted to further optimize the efficacy of CT. We hypothesize that CT-induced changes in brain activity and connectivity are dependent on the type of CT, and that these changes occur predominantly in brain networks important for the specific cognitive domain that is trained. We first summarize the existing literature for multi-domain and single-domain CT separately. In the final section, we evaluate and integrate the reviewed studies, and provide recommendations for future research.
Methods
Study Selection and Screening
We performed a systematic literature review following Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA, http://www.prisma-statement.org/) guidelines on records published in the PubMed, Embase, and PsycINFO databases up to November 23, 2018. We registered the review protocol with the PROSPERO International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews of the University of York (registration number: CRD42019103662). The search terms were defined as a combination of “cognitive training” and related terms and “neuroimaging” and related terms. Additionally, we added exclusion terms based on our eligibility criteria. An overview of the literature search terms is provided in Electronic Supplementary Material 1. We also added studies from reference lists of studies in our literature search (i.e., snowball method). Records were first independently screened (TvB, CV) for eligibility and disagreement was re-evaluated to consensus.
Eligibility criteria were 1) randomized and non-randomized controlled trials on process-based CT, 2) in a human population of patients suffering from neurodegenerative diseases or healthy elderly (defined as a mean age of 60 years or older), 3) with neuroimaging data before and immediately following CT, 4) with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI, i.e., resting-state and/or task-related) as reported outcome measure, and 5) with a minimal total sample size of 20 participants to enhance reliability of single-study results. Eligibility criterion 2 was retrospectively adjusted and therefore deviates from the PROSPERO review protocol: the minimal age for healthy participants was increased from 30 to 60 years in order to include studies specifically in the aging healthy elderly sample. One study that was originally included (Clark, Lawlor-Savage, & Goghari, 2017) was for this reason excluded from the synthesis. Exclusion criteria were combination treatments, such as CT combined with physical activity. As the popularity of process-based CT is increasing, there is also considerable debate on the efficacy and working mechanism, as described in the Introduction. Cognitive strategy training studies were therefore not eligible for this review in order to improve our understanding of –specifically– process-based training, to enhance comparability in a heterogeneous research field.
We only considered reports written in English. Potentially eligible records were screened in full-text based on the aforementioned criteria and excluded records were assigned an exclusion label. Eligible records were included in the systematic review.
While meta-analytic methods such as activation likelihood estimation and seed-based d mapping (Eickhoff, Bzdok, Laird, Kurth, & Fox, 2012; Radua et al., 2012) are highly suitable to define agreement across multiple neuroimaging studies, we did not perform such a meta-analysis as the correspondence in study methods, analysis methods, seed regions or regions-of-interest used, and in-scanner tasks was insufficient and did not adhere to recent neuroimaging meta-analysis recommendations (Muller et al., 2018). Additionally, as there already was significant heterogeneity in fMRI study methodology and to ensure comparability of results, we focused on studies using this neuroimaging modality and did not include other modalities (e.g., positron emission tomography, magnetoencephalography) as well.
Data Extraction and Assessment
We extracted data of participant demographics and intervention characteristics, including sample size, age, domain of CT, control group type (active of passive) and number of hours trained. We classified interventions as multi-domain or single-domain CT. We extracted the specific domain that single-domain interventions focused on (i.e., cognitive control, inhibition, processing speed and working memory). Concerning the fMRI outcome, we extracted the imaging modality (resting-state or task-related fMRI), the analysis method and seed region or region-of-interest (ROI) applied. We extracted the reported regions of CT-related alterations, including directionality of effect, outcome measure, coordinates of the anatomical brain locations (if specified) and correlations with behavioral outcomes (when applicable). Talairach coordinates were converted to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates using Wake Forest University PickAtlas in Statistical Parametric Mapping (Maldjian, Laurienti, & Burdette, 2004; Maldjian, Laurienti, Kraft, & Burdette, 2003). We used BrainNet Viewer to visualize results (Xia, Wang, & He, 2013). Extracted training-induced changes in brain function are relative to a control condition (i.e., the interaction-effect) unless otherwise specified in this paper.
To get an overview of how CT influenced brain network function, we assigned neural network labels to extracted coordinates of neural alterations using the atlases of cerebral, cerebellar and striatal parcellation studies of which the default mode network (DMN), frontoparietal network (FPN), and dorsal and ventral attention network (DAN and VAN) play a large role in facilitating cognitive functions (see Box 1; Power et al., 2011; Fox et al., 2005; Yeo et al., 2011). We used the widely accepted 7-network topology for neocortical areas as described in the Yeo et al. (2011) paper, the Buckner, Krienen, Castellanos, Diaz, and Yeo (2011) paper for cerebellar network organization, the paper by Choi, Yeo, and Buckner (2012) for striatal areas, and classified the hippocampus as a DMN region (Greicius, Krasnow, Reiss, & Menon, 2003; Greicius, Srivastava, Reiss, & Menon, 2004).
Box 1 Resting-state ‘cognitive’ networks
Default mode network (DMN) - Essential regions of the DMN are the medial prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). This network is characterized by activity in the absence of external cognitive demand. The DMN is thought to be involved in integration of memory, self-monitoring and theory of mind (Spreng, Mar, & Kim, 2009; Burianova, McIntosh, & Grady, 2010; Jeong, Chung, & Kim, 2015). Frontoparietal network (FPN) - Important FPN regions are the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and the posterior parietal cortex. This network is also often referred to as the central executive network, and its activity is anti-correlated with DMN activity. The FPN has previously been described as a “multi-demand network” (Duncan, 2010), and its activity is important for goal-directed cognitive tasks, including working memory, planning, judgment and decision-making (Kelly, Uddin, Biswal, Castellanos, & Milham, 2008; Menon, 2011). Relative to other brain networks, the FPN shows strong within- as well as between-network connectivity, reflecting the heterogeneous functions it encompasses (Cole et al., 2013). Dorsal attention network (DAN) & ventral attention network (VAN) - Lastly, two attention networks can be distinguished that follow a dorsal route (DAN) or a ventral route (VAN). The DAN consists of important regions in the intraparietal sulcus and frontal eye field, and it is mainly important for voluntary, goal-directed attention orientation (Fox, Corbetta, Snyder, Vincent, & Raichle, 2006; Fox et al., 2005; Corbetta & Shulman, 2002; Corbetta, Kincade, Ollinger, McAvoy, & Shulman, 2000). The VAN directs attention stimulus-driven – that is, when identifying salient stimuli (Menon & Uddin, 2010; Sridharan, Levitin, & Menon, 2008; Fox et al., 2005, 2006; Corbetta et al., 2000). The VAN is therefore also referred to as the “salience network” (Seeley et al., 2007; Menon & Uddin, 2010). Crucial VAN regions are the anterior insula and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex. |
Quality Assessment
We assessed the quality of individual studies by a National Institutes of Health standardized quality assessment tool of controlled intervention studies (https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools, see Electronic Supplementary Material 2). Criteria involved clear trial description, randomization procedure, allocation concealment, blinding, no baseline differences between groups, low drop-out rate and difference in drop-out between groups, high protocol adherence, similar simultaneous treatments (e.g., treatment-as-usual), outcome measure quality, sufficient power, pre-specified analyses, and intention-to-treat analysis. The neuroimaging analysis quality was additionally assessed by a clear description of neuroimaging protocol and analyses, correction for motion and multiple comparisons correction. We coded trials as ‘Poor’ if they adhered to <10 criteria, ‘Fair’ if they adhered to 10–13 criteria and ‘Good’ if they adhered to >13 criteria. Records were independently assessed by TvB and CV and disagreement was re-evaluated to consensus. As the final sample size was low, we did not exclude studies from this review based on the quality.
Results
Study Selection
The literature screening resulted in 1760 records through PubMed, 1058 records through Embase and 707 records through PsycINFO. Of these, 1300 records were duplicates and five records were non-English. A total of 2220 records were screened of which 98 full-text articles were reviewed. Twenty full-text articles met the inclusion criteria and were included in this systematic review. An overview of the excluded records that underwent full-text review, including the main reason for exclusion, is provided in Electronic Supplementary Material 3. A flow diagram of the screening process according to PRISMA guidelines (www.prisma-statement.org) is provided in Fig. 1.
Study Characteristics
Eight studies included healthy elderly (Cao et al., 2016; De Marco et al., 2016; Luo et al., 2016; Li et al., 2016; Kim, Chey, & Lee, 2017; Kuhn et al., 2017; Lebedev, Nilsson, & Lovden, 2018; Ross et al., 2018), three studies included individuals with mild cognitive impairment (MCI; Suo et al., 2016; Lin et al., 2016; De Marco, Meneghello, Pilosio, Rigon, & Venneri, 2018), one study was performed in Alzheimer’s disease (AD; Huntley, Hampshire, Bor, Owen, & Howard, 2017), six in multiple sclerosis (MS; De Giglio et al., 2016; Bonavita et al., 2015; Parisi et al., 2014; Filippi et al., 2012; Campbell, Langdon, Cercignani, & Rashid, 2016) and one study was performed in Parkinson’s disease (PD; Diez-Cirarda et al., 2016). One study included AD and MCI patients, and healthy elderly (Barban et al., 2017). The sample size varied from twenty (Filippi et al., 2012; Parisi, Rocca, Valsasina, et al., 2014) to 54 participants (Li et al., 2016). Fourteen studies assessed the neural effects of multi-domain training (Barban et al., 2017; Bonavita et al., 2015; Campbell et al., 2016; Cao et al., 2016; De Giglio et al., 2016; De Marco et al., 2016, 2018; Diez-Cirarda et al., 2016; Filippi et al., 2012; Li et al., 2016; Luo et al., 2016; Parisi, Rocca, Valsasina, et al., 2014; Suo et al., 2016) and six studies assessed the effects of single-domain training (Huntley et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2017; Kuhn et al., 2017; Lebedev et al., 2018; Lin et al., 2016; Ross et al., 2018). A description of the interventions and re-coding to single- or multi-domain training is supplied in Electronic Supplementary Material 4. The amount of training hours varied from ten (Ross et al., 2018) to 78 h (Suo et al., 2016). The majority of studies (i.e., n = 13) compared CT effects to an active control group (Barban et al., 2017; Bonavita et al., 2015; Campbell et al., 2016; Cerasa et al., 2013; De Marco et al., 2016, 2018; Diez-Cirarda et al., 2016; Huntley et al., 2017; Lebedev et al., 2018; Li et al., 2016; Lin et al., 2016; Ross et al., 2018; Suo et al., 2016), six studies applied a passive control group (Cao et al., 2016; De Giglio et al., 2016; Filippi et al., 2012; Kim et al., 2017; Luo et al., 2016; Parisi, Rocca, Valsasina, et al., 2014) and one study compared CT to both an active and a passive control group (Kuhn et al., 2017). All studies used randomization except for three studies that were not described to be randomized (Bonavita et al., 2015; De Marco et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2017) and two studies that were unclear on randomization (De Marco et al., 2018; Lin et al., 2016). Detailed study characteristics are shown in Table 1.
Quality Assessment
Table 2 shows the quality assessment results. We classified the Diez-Cirarda et al. (2016) paper after re-evaluation as ‘Good’ as the main RCT aspects were present and clearly reported on, and we classified the Lin et al. (2016) paper after re-evaluation as ‘Fair’ as both the blinding and the randomization procedure were not reported on. A major limitation across most studies was the lack of blinding for participants and/or for assessors. The sample size was generally low without clear power calculations to support the sample size, probably due to the exploratory nature of the studies. In general, the neuroimaging methodology was clearly reported on. Concerning competing interests, in the study by Diez-Cirarda et al. (2016), two co-authors were reported to be copyright holders of the studied intervention. Three studies did not report on competing interests (De Marco et al., 2016; Lebedev et al., 2018; Parisi, Rocca, Valsasina, et al., 2014). The other studies reported to have no competing interests.
Results of Individual Studies
The results of individual studies and methodological details are summarized in Table 1. The results are divided into studies on multi-domain training and single-domain training. A graphical presentation of the findings is shown in Fig. 2.
Multi-Domain Training
In Healthy Elderly
In healthy elderly, multi-domain training increased functional connectivity of the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) with other DMN regions (Cao et al., 2016; De Marco et al., 2016) and within-network connectivity of the FPN and salience network (Cao et al., 2016). Cao et al. (2016) additionally reported an increased training-related anti-correlation between the DMN and FPN. In the same dataset, Luo et al. (2016) showed increased laterality of the left FPN –an increased confinement of the FPN to the left hemisphere– after CT, which was interpreted as an increase in information processing efficiency. Li et al. (2016) assessed brain entropy in this dataset comparing the multi-domain training with an additional single-domain training and an active control condition (for a detailed description of entropy measures, see T. Li et al., 2016). Both the multi-domain and single-domain training counteracted the age-related increase in whole-brain asynchrony and decrease in spontaneous brain activity. Additionally, the multi-domain training significantly reduced the age-related decrease in lateralization of entropy measures, while single-domain training did not.
In Neurodegenerative Diseases
Five studies applied a specific multi-domain training (i.e., ‘RehaCom’) in individuals with MS with variable length (12–36 h). Using independent component analysis on resting-state fMRI scans, RehaCom training increased resting-state functional connectivity within the DMN, mainly in the posterior, parieto-occipital DMN regions (Bonavita et al., 2015), which correlated with a lower post-treatment interference on the Stroop task. Another study showed that RehaCom led to increased or stable resting-state activity fluctuations of salience network, FPN, and DMN areas relative to decreased fluctuations in the control group (Filippi et al., 2012). In this study, task-related activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and PCC during the interference condition of the Stroop task was also increased, correlating with performance on a working memory task (paced auditory serial addition test), but no information was provided on the direction of these specific correlations. On the same dataset, Parisi and colleagues (Parisi, Rocca, Valsasina, et al., 2014) reported increased resting-state functional connectivity between the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and inferior parietal lobe after RehaCom training that was related to improved performance on a working memory task.
RehaCom training in MS induced increased task-related activation during a working memory task of a temporo-parietal region (Campbell et al., 2016), and in the superior parietal and posterior cerebellar lobe; the latter correlated positively with post-training Stroop interference task performance (lobe VI; Cerasa et al., 2013). After a 12-week follow-up period, task-related activity of the temporal-parietal and additional frontal and prefrontal regions was still higher compared with the control group (Campbell et al., 2016).
A different multi-domain training paradigm in MS resulted in increased resting-state functional connectivity between the thalamus seed and the PCC, precuneus and lateral parietal cortex, and decreased connectivity between the thalamus and the left dlPFC, the vermis, and bilateral cerebellar cortical regions (De Giglio et al., 2016).
In patients with amnestic MCI, multi-domain training decreased functional connectivity of the superior frontal gyrus and ACC with the PCC, a core area of the DMN, but increased connectivity of the hippocampus with the superior frontal gyrus (Suo et al., 2016). Another multi-domain training increased the within-DMN connectivity in MCI patients (De Marco et al., 2018) but did not alter connectivity within the FPN or visual network. The study from Barban et al. (2017) in MCI patients reported decreased functional connectivity of the medial superior frontal gyrus with the DMN after multi-domain training and –using network-based statistics analysis (Zalesky, Fornito, & Bullmore, 2010)– decreased connectivity within a subnetwork consisting of subcortical areas, the cerebellum, and temporal and occipital areas. They additionally reported increased betweenness centrality, i.e., the importance of a particular brain area in long-range network communication (Sporns, 2014), of the orbitofrontal cortex, and decreased betweenness centrality of the cerebellar vermis. Conversely, in the same study multi-domain training in AD patients increased the spatial extent of the DMN, increased the functional connectivity of the network-based statistics subnetwork and increased betweenness centrality of the ACC. The authors therefore concluded that multi-domain training had different effects on the brain in MCI and AD, despite similar cognitive improvements. It must be noted, however, that MCI patients improved mainly on memory tasks while AD patients showed an improvement in attention.
In PD patients, an integrative multi-domain training resulted in increased resting-state functional connectivity between the left inferior temporal lobe and bilateral dlPFC and increased activation of the left middle temporal lobe during a memory task (Diez-Cirarda et al., 2016).
Synthesis of Results
Results from multi-domain training studies coherently suggest that it counteracts age-related or disease-related network dysfunction by increasing the within-network connectivity –predominantly reported in the DMN– and the degree of anti-correlation between networks, in particular between the FPN and DMN, which has been related to better cognitive functioning (Kelly et al., 2008; Baggio et al., 2015; Hampson, Driesen, Roth, Gore, & Constable, 2010). Studies on within-DMN connectivity alterations in MCI and AD patients showed mixed results, however, with both increased and decreased connectivity in this resting-state network. CT increased task-related activity across the brain, suggesting an increased neural effort, although this was only studied in MS and PD patients.
Single-Domain Training
In Healthy Elderly
Processing speed/attention training induced decreased activation in the anterior insula and supplementary motor area during an in-scanner ‘useful field of view’ task (Ross et al., 2018) – a visual processing speed and attention task (Wood & Owsley, 2014). This reduction in brain activation was significantly different compared with no-contact controls, while no training-induced activation differences were found relative to an active control group. In this study, resting-state functional connectivity of areas that showed activation during the task at baseline increased significantly after the experimental training compared with both control groups. Kuhn et al. (2017) compared inhibition training to 1) multi-domain training on a mobile application and 2) a passive control condition, and found decreased activation of the right inferior frontal gyrus/anterior insula during an in-scanner stop-signal task after inhibition training only, although this effect did not reach significance in the interaction model (i.e., relative to the control groups). A cognitive control training increased activation of right frontoparietal regions and the left anterior insula during an interference control task (Kim et al., 2017). These results were associated with cognitive performance improvement, mainly on the Stroop color-word interference task.
Working memory training increased brain-network segregation during an n-back and a visuospatial reasoning task as shown by increased whole-brain modularity and reduced connectivity between the FPN and both the DMN and sensorimotor network after training (Lebedev et al., 2018). Interestingly, before training modularity was positively associated with working memory performance, but not with the complex reasoning task. The authors argued that the working memory training potentially induced an increased modular organization that is beneficial for specific abilities such as working memory, but deleterious for complex cognitive tasks.
In Neurodegenerative Diseases
In amnestic MCI patients, a visuospatial speed of processing training resulted in increased resting-state functional connectivity within the DMN (Lin et al., 2016), while increased FPN connectivity did not differ significantly from the active control group. An adaptive WM training in early AD resulted in decreased averaged post-training activity of the right dlPFC and left parietal cortex (Huntley et al., 2017).
Synthesis of Results
Single-domain training paradigms increased functional connectivity within networks and reduced connectivity between networks, similar to connectivity alterations after multi-domain training, but only two studies performed functional connectivity analyses. These paradigms additionally induced reductions in task-related activations in both healthy elderly and an MCI population, which is generally interpreted as an increased efficiency of neural resources needed for the task at hand (Thompson, Waskom, & Gabrieli, 2016; Clark et al., 2017). Reduced activation may additionally be related to the fact that the content of the single-domain CT was identical (Huntley et al., 2017; Ross et al., 2018) or highly similar (Kuhn et al., 2017) to the in-scanner task, thus inducing practice effects. Accordingly, one study that used an in-scanner task that was dissimilar to the CT found increased training-related activity (Kim et al., 2017). The quality rating of this study was, however, poor so these results should be interpreted with caution.
Discussion
This paper systematically reviews studies that investigated the influence of CT through repeated cognitive engagement (i.e., process-based training) on task-related activity and resting-state functional connectivity using fMRI in both healthy populations and neurodegenerative diseases. Our results show that all investigated CT paradigms led to changes in brain activation during task performance and resting-state functional connectivity. There are three main conclusions to our research questions that can be drawn from this literature overview: 1) CT induced both increases and decreases in task-related activity, mostly in fronto-parietal brain areas, without a clear influence of the targeted cognitive domains; 2) multi-domain CT was consistently reported to counteract dysfunctional connectivity patterns in cognitive brain networks that are generally associated with aging or neurodegenerative diseases; 3) methodological heterogeneity between studies limits our ability to statistically compare findings and study disease- or training-specific neural alterations. Below we consider the implications of our findings and critically discuss methodological issues to guide future research.
Task-Related Activation Studies: Training-Induced Improvement in Increased Neural Efficiency, or Increased Effort
The majority of single-domain studies and four multi-domain CT studies assessed training-induced alterations in task-related brain activation. We observed that alterations were most frequently reported in frontoparietal areas, probably driven by the fact that the FPN was a network of interest in most studies (see “Limitations and recommendations for future research” section). Three single-domain studies found decreased task-related activity after CT (Huntley et al., 2017; Kuhn et al., 2017; Ross et al., 2018), while one single-domain study and all multi-domain studies found increased task-related activity (Kim et al., 2017; Diez-Cirarda et al., 2016; Filippi et al., 2012; Cerasa et al., 2013; Campbell et al., 2016). These results of both increased and decreased activity are in line with a meta-analysis in healthy elderly that similarly reported on functional activity alterations predominantly in areas of the FPN (Duda & Sweet, 2019). A review on working memory training in healthy populations reported predominantly post-training regional activity decreases, while increased activity was more scarcely reported (Buschkuehl, Jaeggi, & Jonides, 2012). An interesting distinction was found in one study that the review described; reporting decreased post-CT activity in young adults but increased activity in elderly suggesting different processes of plasticity (Dahlin, Neely, Larsson, Backman, & Nyberg, 2008). Another similar review in healthy populations also reported both increased and decreased brain activity after CT, possibly related to task selectivity, improved efficiency or more automatic processing due to CT (Taya, Sun, Babiloni, Thakor, & Bezerianos, 2015). We hypothesize that either training-induced neural efficiency or neural effort can account for the bi-directionality of the results, although in-scanner task characteristics may also be relevant.
Decreased task-related activity after training suggests an increase in efficiency, i.e., reduced use of neural resources. This result was described only in healthy elderly samples and fits well with the literature on compensatory mechanisms of increased neural resource use to uphold ‘normal’ cognitive task performance in healthy aging (Reuter-Lorenz & Cappell, 2008; Reuter-Lorenz & Park, 2014; Cabeza, 2002; Davis, Dennis, Daselaar, Fleck, & Cabeza, 2008). It is generally accepted that aging-related decline in brain ‘fitness’ due to, for example, decreased within-network connectivity, increased neural noise, or dedifferentiation of task-positive and task-negative neural networks is compensated for by mechanisms such as regional over-activation, a posterior-to-anterior shift in brain activity, and decreased asymmetry of hemispheric activity (Reuter-Lorenz & Park, 2010; Festini, Zahodne, & Reuter-Lorenz, 2018; Cabeza et al., 2018). Our results may therefore indicate that the increased efficiency after single-domain training reverses age-related compensatory mechanisms of increased neural effort while maintaining cognitive performance (illustrated by (1) in Fig. 3, panel a and b).
Four studies in patients with PD or MS reported an increase in post-training activity during task performance. This seems to reflect an increase in neural effort, which is in contrast with the increase in efficiency as described above – but may be related to differences between healthy and non-healthy populations. Theories on compensatory mechanisms with healthy aging assume that significant neural remodeling occurs, while at the behavioral level performance remains relatively unimpaired (Festini et al., 2018). This mechanism has also been described in early stages of neurodegenerative diseases in which task performance is still comparable to age-matched healthy controls (Trujillo et al., 2014; Gerrits et al., 2015; Audoin et al., 2003; Lopez-Gongora et al., 2015). Indeed, in a recent study, working memory CT showed similar task-related activity decrease in healthy adults and a population of early-stage cognitively healthy MS patients (Aguirre et al., 2019). Cognitive performance in MS and PD patients is, however, generally impaired (Bosboom, Stoffers, & Wolters, 2004; Chiaravalloti & DeLuca, 2008) and compensatory mechanisms as described above may at later disease stage no longer be sufficient for these patients. Indeed, task-related neural activity is hypothesized to follow an inverted U-shaped curve both in healthy adults and elderly subjects (Cabeza et al., 2018), in patients suffering from MS (Schoonheim, Geurts, & Barkhof, 2010) and neuropsychiatric disorders (van Velzen, Vriend, de Wit, & van den Heuvel, 2014), so that when task demands become too high, compensatory hyperactivity fails resulting in relative hypo-activity and impaired performance. In the reviewed studies on MS and PD patients, we hypothesize that –as baseline cognitive performance was impaired in these studies– training resulted in a shift of the U-shaped curve to the right thus counteracting the relative hypo-activity at baseline, and improved performance (illustrated as (2) in Fig. 3). There is, however, no information on the baseline cognitive performance of the MS and PD samples relative to healthy controls.
Differences in task demand and familiarity may also have impacted the observed outcomes. In accordance with the literature on compensatory brain processes (Reuter-Lorenz & Cappell, 2008; Cabeza et al., 2018), variation in task load difficulty between task-related fMRI studies may have induced different results with less demanding fMRI tasks such as a digit span task (Huntley et al., 2017) inducing increased efficiency of neural resources, while complex tasks such as the multi-source interference task (Kim et al., 2017) induced increased brain activity. Moreover, some studies using fMRI task paradigms that were also part of the CT paradigm, e.g., in Huntley et al. (2017) and Ross et al. (2018), reported post-training activity decreases, while studies using an fMRI task that was less similar to the tasks being trained (such as in the multi-domain training studies) showed increased post-training activity. In line with theories of neural efficiency (Reuter-Lorenz & Cappell, 2008) and neural scaffolding (Reuter-Lorenz & Park, 2014), this may indicate increased neural efficiency by familiarity of the task paradigm –i.e., having repeatedly performed one specific task– or increased scaffolding by being able to address compensatory neural resources, respectively.
Counteracting Age- or Disease-Related Neural Network Dysfunctions
The brain is organized into several segregated functional and structural networks that facilitate the execution of complex functions (Wig, 2017; Fox et al., 2005; Seeley et al., 2007). Aging and neurodegenerative diseases lead to reductions in the connectivity within networks and decreased segregation of (i.e., increased connectivity between) these networks (Spreng, Sepulcre, Turner, Stevens, & Schacter, 2013; Grady, Sarraf, Saverino, & Campbell, 2016; Joo, Lim, & Lee, 2016; Hohenfeld, Werner, & Reetz, 2018; Damoiseaux, 2017). Our review shows that relative to a control condition, CT consistently induced neural alterations that counteracted these age- and disease-related connectivity patterns. This was particularly evident for studies on multi-domain training. First, CT increased intra-network functional connectivity. This effect was most frequently reported within the DMN, but also for the FPN, DAN/VAN connectivity and in functional connectivity of the hippocampus and thalamus. Additionally, CT enhanced the degree of network segregation, evident from increases in the anti-correlation between task-negative (i.e., DMN) and task-positive networks (FPN or DAN), or a training-induced increase in whole-brain modularity. Enhanced segregation is associated with better cognitive functioning in the network literature (Damoiseaux, 2017).
Two reports specifically addressed age-related neural alterations and described a long-term effect of CT on resting-state fMRI-derived indices of neural network laterality (Luo et al., 2016) and brain entropy (Li et al., 2016), even a year after training. Similarly, a recent study in patients with MCI found increased spontaneous regional brain activity during resting-state (Li et al., 2019). Taken together, the results thus seem to indicate a restorative effect of CT on aging and neurodegeneration-induced changes in neural network organization (see Fig. 3, panel b). This seems mainly applicable to multi-domain training. It should be noted that only a single study enrolled both a healthy and a non-healthy population; it therefore remains speculative whether CT indeed restores brain network connectivity of patients with neurodegenerative diseases to healthy control levels.
Limitations and Recommendations for Future Research
The main shortcoming of the reviewed literature is the heterogeneity in type of training, imaging methodology, in-scanner task paradigm and analysis method, which did not allow us to do statistical comparisons through meta-analyses. The reviewed studies all show that CT induces changes in brain activity and connectivity that are not localized to specific brain regions. Our systematic review therefore does not seem to confirm an earlier reported assumption that neural alterations are specific to a particular type of CT (Buschkuehl et al., 2012) – although there are some general differences between multi- and single-domain training. It remains, however, unclear if any population- or training-specific effects exist. There is a significant body of literature on functional activity and connectivity changes after strategy-based CT that show similar findings as the results from this review. For instance, mnemonic strategy CTs increased activity of fronto-parietal and temporal regions in individuals suffering from MCI and increased functional connectivity within cognitive networks in healthy elderly (Chapman et al., 2015; Chapman, Spence, Aslan, & Keebler, 2017; Hampstead, Stringer, Stilla, Giddens, & Sathian, 2012; Hampstead, Stringer, Stilla, & Sathian, 2019; Simon et al., 2019). These studies were, however, beyond the scope of this review. An additional issue impacting the results of this review is that there is a likelihood of publication bias given the lack of negative results in this review sample. Future research should focus on statistically comparing training packages and types, population-specific CT effects and quantify publication bias. Likewise, our quality assessment identified several low-powered, non-blinded, or non-randomized studies, which substantiates the need for future studies with unified CT paradigms and analysis methods to enhance comparability. The use of healthy control samples could additionally highlight differential effects of CT on healthy and non-healthy populations and shed light on possible restorative effects that CT might induce. One recently published study in a small group of individuals with MS showed for example that hyper-activation relative to healthy controls at baseline was partially normalized after CT (Bonzano et al., 2018).
An important methodological limitation of the reviewed studies is the fact that the majority of studies focused on alterations of the activity or connectivity of a single or only a few brain regions using ROI- or seed-based approaches. As a consequence, the selection of the ROI or seed determines the observed patterns and (slight) shifts in location may already lead to different results (Li, Guo, Nie, Li, & Liu, 2009). Moreover, ROI- or seed-based approaches are based on the assumption that cognitive functions are related to discrete brain regions and thus focus on just a piece of the puzzle (Turk-Browne, 2013). By exclusively focusing on particular ROIs or seed regions, other potentially interesting and meaningful brain activity and connectivity patterns might have been missed. Neural correlates of CT were, for example, reported mainly in cognitive brain networks, but the majority of the ROIs or seed regions were located within these networks which biases the reported effects. As higher-order cognitive functions typically require the integration of multiple brain processes by having large-scale networks interact dynamically, instead of relying on independent, localized processes (Menon, 2011; Bullmore & Sporns, 2009; Bassett & Sporns, 2017), whole-brain, integrative approaches are indispensable in the assessment of neural CT correlates.
Improvements in computational power in neuroimaging and its analyses have led to a network approach that can grasp the complexity of behavior including cognitive functions (Ferguson, Anderson, & Spreng, 2017; Bassett & Sporns, 2017; Stam, 2014). In neuroscience, neural network topology and dynamics are strongly correlated with cognitive functions (Douw et al., 2011; Langer et al., 2012; Camicioli et al., 2009). An example of an integrative approach is offered by a single study in this review that used modularity as a graph theoretical measure to assess neural network segregation after CT (Lebedev et al., 2018). The results of this study confirm the findings from seed-based connectivity studies in this review of CT-induced enhanced task-positive versus task-negative network segregation during task performance. It has further been advocated that to really understand the neural correlates of executive functioning –and by extension the working mechanism of CT– one needs to study the dynamic and flexible engagement of brain networks (Braun et al., 2015). Interactions between brain regions constantly change during task execution, a process called dynamic network reconfiguration, which is dependent on task demands (Braun et al., 2015; Kitzbichler, Henson, Smith, Nathan, & Bullmore, 2011; Bentivoglio, Baldonero, Ricciardi, De Nigris, & Daniele, 2013). Converging evidence of both integrative and targeted approaches may in the end lead to a better understanding of how CT alters brain function.
Lastly, although most studies show that CT is able to induce either clinical cognitive improvement or lead to changes in brain activity and/or connectivity, few studies report on an actual association between CT-induced alterations in brain function and the change in neuropsychological measures. Yet, from a clinical perspective, it is essential to demonstrate these associations between neural alterations and improvement on neuropsychological measures and, even more so, in measures of everyday function. Furthermore, the organization of brain networks may serve as a predictive biomarker for treatment response to facilitate personalized CT programs (i.e., precision medicine). Multiple studies in neurological and psychiatric populations have shown that the individual variation in pre-training brain morphological and/or network characteristics, e.g., brain modularity or cortical volume, is related to the variability in CT-induced cognitive improvement (Arnemann et al., 2015; Engvig et al., 2012; Gallen et al., 2016; Strangman et al., 2010; Verghese, Garner, Mattingley, & Dux, 2016; Vermeij et al., 2016). In addition, neural activation and functional connectivity after CT reportedly predict the persistence of neuropsychological and behavioral improvement at follow-up testing (Subramaniam et al., 2012, 2014; Parisi, Rocca, Mattioli, et al., 2014).
Conclusion
There is convincing evidence that cognitive process-based training alters brain activation and connectivity patterns. CT-induced changes occur mainly in neural networks important for cognitive function and seem to counteract dysfunctional activation and connectivity patterns associated with aging and neurodegenerative diseases, either indicative of a restorative or compensatory process – or a combination thereof. In order to improve our understanding of CT-induced neural alterations and the associated cognitive improvement, we advocate a network view of the brain to better comprehend the complex, dynamic changes in the brain induced by training. It is essential to harmonize the methodology and improve trial quality to increase comparability between studies and ultimately enable quantitative meta-analyses. Knowledge of how CT alters the brain network and how this relates to cognitive improvement may ultimately improve CT efficacy and accelerate individualized cognitive training programs.
Abbreviations
- ACC:
-
Anterior cingulate cortex
- AD:
-
Alzheimer’s disease
- CT:
-
Cognitive training
- DAN:
-
Dorsal attention network
- dlPFC:
-
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
- DMN:
-
Default mode network
- (f) MRI:
-
(functional) MRI
- FPN:
-
Frontoparietal network
- MCI:
-
Mild cognitive impairment
- MNI:
-
Montreal Neurological Institute
- MRI:
-
Magnetic resonance imaging
- MS:
-
Multiple sclerosis
- PCC:
-
Posterior cingulate cortex
- PD:
-
Parkinson’s disease
- ROI:
-
Region-of-interest
- VAN:
-
Ventral attention network
References
Abraham, W. C. (2008). Metaplasticity: Tuning synapses and networks for plasticity. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9(5), 387. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2356
Aguirre, N., Cruz-Gomez, A. J., Miro-Padilla, A., Bueicheku, E., Broseta Torres, R., Avila, C., et al. (2019). Repeated working memory training improves task performance and neural efficiency in multiple sclerosis patients and healthy controls. Multiple Sclerosis International, 2019, 2657902. https://doi.org/10.1155/2019/2657902
Arnemann, K. L., Chen, A. J., Novakovic-Agopian, T., Gratton, C., Nomura, E. M., & D'Esposito, M. (2015). Functional brain network modularity predicts response to cognitive training after brain injury. Neurology, 84(15), 1568–1574. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000001476
Audoin, B., Ibarrola, D., Ranjeva, J. P., Confort-Gouny, S., Malikova, I., Ali-Cherif, A., et al. (2003). Compensatory cortical activation observed by fmri during a cognitive task at the earliest stage of ms. Human Brain Mapping, 20(2), 51–58. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10128
Baggio, H. C., Segura, B., Sala-Llonch, R., Marti, M. J., Valldeoriola, F., Compta, Y., et al. (2015). Cognitive impairment and resting-state network connectivity in parkinson's disease. Human Brain Mapping, 36(1), 199–212. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22622
Barban, F., Mancini, M., Cercignani, M., Adriano, F., Perri, R., Annicchiarico, R., et al. (2017). A pilot study on brain plasticity of functional connectivity modulated by cognitive training in mild alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Brain Sciences, 7(5). https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7050050
Bassett, D. S., & Sporns, O. (2017). Network neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 20(3), 353–364. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4502
Bentivoglio, A. R., Baldonero, E., Ricciardi, L., De Nigris, F., & Daniele, A. (2013). Neuropsychological features of patients with parkinson's disease and impulse control disorders. Neurological Sciences, 34, 1207–1213. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-012-1224-5
Bonavita, S., Sacco, R., Della Corte, M., Esposito, S., Sparaco, M., d'Ambrosio, A., et al. (2015). Computer-aided cognitive rehabilitation improves cognitive performances and induces brain functional connectivity changes in relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis patients: An exploratory study. Journal of Neurology, 262(1), 91–100. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-014-7528-z
Bonzano, L., Pedulla, L., Pardini, M., Tacchino, A., Zaratin, P., Battaglia, M. A., et al. (2018). Brain activity pattern changes after adaptive working memory training in multiple sclerosis. Brain Imaging and Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-018-9984-z
Bosboom, J. L., Stoffers, D., & Wolters, E. (2004). Cognitive dysfunction and dementia in parkinson's disease. Journal of Neural Transmission (Vienna), 111(10–11), 1303–1315. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-004-0168-1
Braun, U., Schafer, A., Walter, H., Erk, S., Romanczuk-Seiferth, N., Haddad, L., et al. (2015). Dynamic reconfiguration of frontal brain networks during executive cognition in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(37), 11678–11683. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1422487112
Buckner, R. L., Krienen, F. M., Castellanos, A., Diaz, J. C., & Yeo, B. T. (2011). The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(5), 2322–2345. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00339.2011
Bullmore, E., & Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 10(3), 186–198. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2575
Burianova, H., McIntosh, A. R., & Grady, C. L. (2010). A common functional brain network for autobiographical, episodic, and semantic memory retrieval. Neuroimage, 49(1), 865–874. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.066
Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S. M., & Jonides, J. (2012). Neuronal effects following working memory training. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(Suppl 1), S167–S179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.10.001
Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The harold model. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 85–100.
Cabeza, R., Albert, M., Belleville, S., Craik, F. I. M., Duarte, A., Grady, C. L., et al. (2018). Maintenance, reserve and compensation: The cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 19(11), 701–710. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2
Camicioli, R., Gee, M., Bouchard, T. P., Fisher, N. J., Hanstock, C. C., Emery, D. J., et al. (2009). Voxel-based morphometry reveals extra-nigral atrophy patterns associated with dopamine refractory cognitive and motor impairment in parkinsonism. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 15(3), 187–195.
Campbell, J., Langdon, D., Cercignani, M., & Rashid, W. (2016). A randomised controlled trial of efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis: A cognitive, behavioural, and mri study. Neural Plasticity, 2016, 4292585. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/4292585
Cao, W., Cao, X., Hou, C., Li, T., Cheng, Y., Jiang, L., et al. (2016). Effects of cognitive training on resting-state functional connectivity of default mode, salience, and central executive networks. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8, 70. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00070
Cerasa, A., Gioia, M. C., Valentino, P., Nistico, R., Chiriaco, C., Pirritano, D., et al. (2013). Computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation of attention deficits for multiple sclerosis: A randomized trial with fmri correlates. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 27(4), 284–295. https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968312465194
Chandler, M. J., Parks, A. C., Marsiske, M., Rotblatt, L. J., & Smith, G. E. (2016). Everyday impact of cognitive interventions in mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology Review, 26(3), 225–251. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-016-9330-4
Chapman, S. B., Aslan, S., Spence, J. S., Hart Jr., J. J., Bartz, E. K., Didehbani, N., et al. (2015). Neural mechanisms of brain plasticity with complex cognitive training in healthy seniors. Cerebral Cortex, 25(2), 396–405. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht234
Chapman, S. B., Spence, J. S., Aslan, S., & Keebler, M. W. (2017). Enhancing innovation and underlying neural mechanisms via cognitive training in healthy older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, 314. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00314
Chiaravalloti, N. D., & DeLuca, J. (2008). Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurology, 7(12), 1139–1151.
Choi, E. Y., Yeo, B. T., & Buckner, R. L. (2012). The organization of the human striatum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108(8), 2242–2263. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00270.2012
Clark, C. M., Lawlor-Savage, L., & Goghari, V. M. (2017). Functional brain activation associated with working memory training and transfer. Behavioural Brain Research, 334, 34–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2017.07.030
Cole, M. W., Reynolds, J. R., Power, J. D., Repovs, G., Anticevic, A., & Braver, T. S. (2013). Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nature Neuroscience, 16(9), 1348–1355. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3470
Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P., & Shulman, G. L. (2000). Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 292–297. https://doi.org/10.1038/73009
Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755
Dahlin, E., Neely, A. S., Larsson, A., Backman, L., & Nyberg, L. (2008). Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science, 320(5882), 1510–1512. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1155466
Damoiseaux, J. S. (2017). Effects of aging on functional and structural brain connectivity. Neuroimage, 160, 32–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.077
Dardiotis, E., Nousia, A., Siokas, V., Tsouris, Z., Andravizou, A., Mentis, A. A., et al. (2018). Efficacy of computer-based cognitive training in neuropsychological performance of patients with multiple sclerosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Multiple Sclerosis and Related Disorders, 20, 58–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2017.12.017
Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A., Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Que pasa? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1201–1209. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm155
De Giglio, L., Tona, F., De Luca, F., Petsas, N., Prosperini, L., Bianchi, V., et al. (2016). Multiple sclerosis: Changes in thalamic resting-state functional connectivity induced by a home-based cognitive rehabilitation program. Radiology, 280(1), 202–211. https://doi.org/10.1148/radiol.2016150710
De Marco, M., Meneghello, F., Duzzi, D., Rigon, J., Pilosio, C., & Venneri, A. (2016). Cognitive stimulation of the default-mode network modulates functional connectivity in healthy aging. Brain Research Bulletin, 121, 26–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2015.12.001
De Marco, M., Meneghello, F., Pilosio, C., Rigon, J., & Venneri, A. (2018). Up-regulation of dmn connectivity in mild cognitive impairment via network-based cognitive training. Current Alzheimer Research, 15(6), 578–589. https://doi.org/10.2174/1567205015666171212103323
Diez-Cirarda, M., Ojeda, N., Pena, J., Cabrera-Zubizarreta, A., Lucas-Jimenez, O., Gomez-Esteban, J. C., et al. (2016). Increased brain connectivity and activation after cognitive rehabilitation in parkinson's disease: A randomized controlled trial. Brain Imaging and Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-016-9639-x
Douw, L., Schoonheim, M. M., Landi, D., van der Meer, M. L., Geurts, J. J., Reijneveld, J. C., et al. (2011). Cognition is related to resting-state small-world network topology: An magnetoencephalographic study. Neuroscience, 175, 169–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.11.039
Duda, B. M., & Sweet, L. H. (2019). Functional brain changes associated with cognitive training in healthy older adults: A preliminary ale meta-analysis. Brain Imaging and Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00080-0
Duncan, J. (2010). The multiple-demand (md) system of the primate brain: Mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(4), 172–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.004
Eickhoff, S. B., Bzdok, D., Laird, A. R., Kurth, F., & Fox, P. T. (2012). Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. Neuroimage, 59(3), 2349–2361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.017
Engvig, A., Fjell, A. M., Westlye, L. T., Skaane, N. V., Sundseth, O., & Walhovd, K. B. (2012). Hippocampal subfield volumes correlate with memory training benefit in subjective memory impairment. Neuroimage, 61(1), 188–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.072
Ferguson, M. A., Anderson, J. S., & Spreng, R. N. (2017). Fluid and flexible minds: Intelligence reflects synchrony in the brain’s intrinsic network architecture. Network Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00010
Festini, S. B., Zahodne, L., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2018). Theoretical perspectives on age differences in brain activation: Harold, pasa, crunch—How do they stac up? Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.400
Filippi, M., Riccitelli, G., Mattioli, F., Capra, R., Stampatori, C., Pagani, E., et al. (2012). Multiple sclerosis: Effects of cognitive rehabilitation on structural and functional mr imaging measures--an explorative study. Radiology, 262(3), 932–940. https://doi.org/10.1148/radiol.11111299
Fox, M. D., Corbetta, M., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2006). Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(26), 10046–10051. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0604187103
Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(27), 9673–9678. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0504136102
Gallen, C. L., Baniqued, P. L., Chapman, S. B., Aslan, S., Keebler, M., Didehbani, N., et al. (2016). Modular brain network organization predicts response to cognitive training in older adults. PLoS One, 11(12), e0169015. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169015
Gerrits, N. J., van der Werf, Y. D., Verhoef, K. M., Veltman, D. J., Groenewegen, H. J., Berendse, H. W., et al. (2015). Compensatory fronto-parietal hyperactivation during set-shifting in unmedicated patients with parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia.
Goodier, R. (2009). Brain training’s unproven hype. Scientific American Mind, 20(4), 8–8.
Grady, C., Sarraf, S., Saverino, C., & Campbell, K. (2016). Age differences in the functional interactions among the default, frontoparietal control, and dorsal attention networks. Neurobiology of Aging, 41, 159–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.02.020
Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(1), 253–258. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0135058100
Greicius, M. D., Srivastava, G., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2004). Default-mode network activity distinguishes alzheimer's disease from healthy aging: Evidence from functional mri. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(13), 4637–4642. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0308627101
Hampson, M., Driesen, N., Roth, J. K., Gore, J. C., & Constable, R. T. (2010). Functional connectivity between task-positive and task-negative brain areas and its relation to working memory performance. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 28(8), 1051–1057. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mri.2010.03.021
Hampstead, B. M., Stringer, A. Y., Stilla, R. F., Giddens, M., & Sathian, K. (2012). Mnemonic strategy training partially restores hippocampal activity in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Hippocampus, 22(8), 1652–1658. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22006
Hampstead, B. M., Stringer, A. Y., Stilla, R. F., & Sathian, K. (2019). Mnemonic strategy training increases neocortical activation in healthy older adults and patients with mild cognitive impairment. International Journal of Psychophysiology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.04.011
Hohenfeld, C., Werner, C. J., & Reetz, K. (2018). Resting-state connectivity in neurodegenerative disorders: Is there potential for an imaging biomarker? Neuroimage Clin, 18, 849–870. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2018.03.013
Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J., & Dixon, R. A. (1999). Use it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychology and Aging, 14(2), 245–263. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.14.2.245
Huntley, J. D., Hampshire, A., Bor, D., Owen, A., & Howard, R. J. (2017). Adaptive working memory strategy training in early alzheimer's disease: Randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 210(1), 61–66. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.116.182048
Jeong, W., Chung, C. K., & Kim, J. S. (2015). Episodic memory in aspects of large-scale brain networks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 454. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00454
Joo, S. H., Lim, H. K., & Lee, C. U. (2016). Three large-scale functional brain networks from resting-state functional mri in subjects with different levels of cognitive impairment. Psychiatry Investigation, 13(1), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.4306/pi.2016.13.1.1
Katz, B., Shah, P., & Meyer, D. E. (2018). How to play 20 questions with nature and lose: Reflections on 100 years of brain-training research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(40), 9897–9904. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1617102114
Kelly, A. M., Uddin, L. Q., Biswal, B. B., Castellanos, F. X., & Milham, M. P. (2008). Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability. Neuroimage, 39(1), 527–537. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.008
Kim, H., Chey, J., & Lee, S. (2017). Effects of multicomponent training of cognitive control on cognitive function and brain activation in older adults. Neuroscience Research, 124, 8–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2017.05.004
Kitzbichler, M. G., Henson, R. N., Smith, M. L., Nathan, P. J., & Bullmore, E. T. (2011). Cognitive effort drives workspace configuration of human brain functional networks. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(22), 8259–8270. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0440-11.2011
Kuhlman, S. J., O'Connor, D. H., Fox, K., & Svoboda, K. (2014). Structural plasticity within the barrel cortex during initial phases of whisker-dependent learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(17), 6078–6083. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4919-12.2014
Kuhn, S., Lorenz, R. C., Weichenberger, M., Becker, M., Haesner, M., O'Sullivan, J., et al. (2017). Taking control! Structural and behavioural plasticity in response to game-based inhibition training in older adults. Neuroimage, 156, 199–206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.026
Lai, C. S., Franke, T. F., & Gan, W. B. (2012). Opposite effects of fear conditioning and extinction on dendritic spine remodelling. Nature, 483(7387), 87–91. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10792
Langer, N., Pedroni, A., Gianotti, L. R., Hanggi, J., Knoch, D., & Jancke, L. (2012). Functional brain network efficiency predicts intelligence. Human Brain Mapping, 33(6), 1393–1406. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21297
Lebedev, A. V., Nilsson, J., & Lovden, M. (2018). Working memory and reasoning benefit from different modes of large-scale brain dynamics in healthy older adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30(7), 1033–1046. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01260
Leung, I. H., Walton, C. C., Hallock, H., Lewis, S. J., Valenzuela, M., & Lampit, A. (2015). Cognitive training in parkinson disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology, 85(21), 1843–1851. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000002145
Li, B. Y., He, N. Y., Qiao, Y., Xu, H. M., Lu, Y. Z., Cui, P. J., et al. (2019). Computerized cognitive training for chinese mild cognitive impairment patients: A neuropsychological and fmri study. Neuroimage Clinical, 22, 101691. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101691
Li, K., Guo, L., Nie, J., Li, G., & Liu, T. (2009). Review of methods for functional brain connectivity detection using fmri. Computerized Medical Imaging and Graphics, 33(2), 131–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compmedimag.2008.10.011
Li, T., Yao, Y., Cheng, Y., Xu, B., Cao, X., Waxman, D., et al. (2016). Cognitive training can reduce the rate of cognitive aging: A neuroimaging cohort study. BMC Geriatrics, 16, 12. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-016-0194-5
Lin, F., Heffner, K. L., Ren, P., Tivarus, M. E., Brasch, J., Chen, D. G., et al. (2016). Cognitive and neural effects of vision-based speed-of-processing training in older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment: A pilot study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 64(6), 1293–1298. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.14132
Lopez-Gongora, M., Escartin, A., Martinez-Horta, S., Fernandez-Bobadilla, R., Querol, L., Romero, S., et al. (2015). Neurophysiological evidence of compensatory brain mechanisms in early-stage multiple sclerosis. PLoS One, 10(8), e0136786. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136786
Luo, C., Zhang, X., Cao, X., Gan, Y., Li, T., Cheng, Y., et al. (2016). The lateralization of intrinsic networks in the aging brain implicates the effects of cognitive training. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8, 32. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00032
Mahncke, H. W., Bronstone, A., & Merzenich, M. M. (2006). Brain plasticity and functional losses in the aged: Scientific bases for a novel intervention. Progress in Brain Research, 157, 81–109. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(06)57006-2
Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., & Burdette, J. H. (2004). Precentral gyrus discrepancy in electronic versions of the talairach atlas. Neuroimage, 21(1), 450–455.
Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A., & Burdette, J. H. (2003). An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fmri data sets. Neuroimage, 19(3), 1233–1239.
Menon, V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(10), 483–506. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003
Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure & Function, 214(5–6), 655–667. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0
Muller, V. I., Cieslik, E. C., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., et al. (2018). Ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 84, 151–161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.012
Orban, S. A., Rapport, M. D., Friedman, L. M., & Kofler, M. J. (2014). Executive function/cognitive training for children with adhd: Do results warrant the hype and cost? The ADHD Report, 22(8), 8–14. https://doi.org/10.1521/adhd.2014.22.8.8
Parisi, L., Rocca, M. A., Mattioli, F., Copetti, M., Capra, R., Valsasina, P., et al. (2014). Changes of brain resting state functional connectivity predict the persistence of cognitive rehabilitation effects in patients with multiple sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis, 20(6), 686–694. https://doi.org/10.1177/1352458513505692
Parisi, L., Rocca, M. A., Valsasina, P., Panicari, L., Mattioli, F., & Filippi, M. (2014). Cognitive rehabilitation correlates with the functional connectivity of the anterior cingulate cortex in patients with multiple sclerosis. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 8(3), 387–393. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-012-9160-9
Park, D. C., & Bischof, G. N. (2013). The aging mind: Neuroplasticity in response to cognitive training. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 15(1), 109–119.
Petrelli, A., Kaesberg, S., Barbe, M. T., Timmermann, L., Rosen, J. B., Fink, G. R., et al. (2014). Cognitive training in parkinson's disease reduces cognitive decline in the long term. European Journal of Neurology. https://doi.org/10.1111/ene.12621
Power, J. D., Cohen, A. L., Nelson, S. M., Wig, G. S., Barnes, K. A., Church, J. A., et al. (2011). Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron, 72(4), 665–678. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006
Rabipour, S., & Raz, A. (2012). Training the brain: Fact and fad in cognitive and behavioral remediation. Brain and Cognition, 79(2), 159–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2012.02.006
Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., Phillips, M. L., El-Hage, W., Kronhaus, D. M., Cardoner, N., et al. (2012). A new meta-analytic method for neuroimaging studies that combines reported peak coordinates and statistical parametric maps. European Psychiatry, 27(8), 605–611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2011.04.001
Raichlen, D. A., & Alexander, G. E. (2017). Adaptive capacity: An evolutionary neuroscience model linking exercise, cognition, and brain health. Trends in Neurosciences, 40(7), 408–421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2017.05.001
Rebok, G. W., Ball, K., Guey, L. T., Jones, R. N., Kim, H. Y., King, J. W., et al. (2014). Ten-year effects of the advanced cognitive training for independent and vital elderly cognitive training trial on cognition and everyday functioning in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 62(1), 16–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.12607
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 177–182.
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Park, D. C. (2010). Human neuroscience and the aging mind: A new look at old problems. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65(4), 405–415. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbq035
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Park, D. C. (2014). How does it stac up? Revisiting the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychology Review, 24(3), 355–370. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-014-9270-9
Ross, L. A., Webb, C. E., Whitaker, C., Hicks, J. M., Schmidt, E. L., Samimy, S., et al. (2018). The effects of useful field of view training on brain activity and connectivity. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby041
Schoonheim, M. M., Geurts, J. J., & Barkhof, F. (2010). The limits of functional reorganization in multiple sclerosis. Neurology, 74(16), 1246–1247. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181db9957
Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(9), 2349–2356. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007
Seppi, K., Weintraub, D., Coelho, M., Perez-Lloret, S., Fox, S. H., Katzenschlager, R., et al. (2011). The movement disorder society evidence-based medicine review update: Treatments for the non-motor symptoms of parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders, 26(Suppl 3), S42–S80. https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.23884
Shah, T. M., Weinborn, M., Verdile, G., Sohrabi, H. R., & Martins, R. N. (2017). Enhancing cognitive functioning in healthly older adults: A systematic review of the clinical significance of commercially available computerized cognitive training in preventing cognitive decline. Neuropsychology Review, 27(1), 62–80. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-016-9338-9
Simon, S. S., Hampstead, B. M., Nucci, M. P., Duran, F. L. S., Fonseca, L. M., Martin, M., et al. (2019). Training gains and transfer effects after mnemonic strategy training in mild cognitive impairment: A fmri study. International Journal of Psychophysiology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.03.014
Sitzer, D. I., Twamley, E. W., & Jeste, D. V. (2006). Cognitive training in alzheimer's disease: A meta-analysis of the literature. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 114(2), 75–90.
Sporns, O. (2014). Contributions and challenges for network models in cognitive neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 17(5), 652–660. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3690
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., & Kim, A. S. (2009). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 489–510. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.21029
Spreng, R. N., Sepulcre, J., Turner, G. R., Stevens, W. D., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Intrinsic architecture underlying the relations among the default, dorsal attention, and frontoparietal control networks of the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(1), 74–86. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00281
Sridharan, D., Levitin, D. J., & Menon, V. (2008). A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(34), 12569–12574. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0800005105
Stam, C. J. (2014). Modern network science of neurological disorders. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 15(10), 683–695. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3801
Strangman, G. E., O'Neil-Pirozzi, T. M., Supelana, C., Goldstein, R., Katz, D. I., & Glenn, M. B. (2010). Regional brain morphometry predicts memory rehabilitation outcome after traumatic brain injury. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 182. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00182
Stuart, K. E., King, A. E., Fernandez-Martos, C. M., Dittmann, J., Summers, M. J., & Vickers, J. C. (2017). Mid-life environmental enrichment increases synaptic density in ca1 in a mouse model of abeta-associated pathology and positively influences synaptic and cognitive health in healthy ageing. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 525(8), 1797–1810. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.24156
Subramaniam, K., Luks, T. L., Fisher, M., Simpson, G. V., Nagarajan, S., & Vinogradov, S. (2012). Computerized cognitive training restores neural activity within the reality monitoring network in schizophrenia. Neuron, 73(4), 842–853. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.024
Subramaniam, K., Luks, T. L., Garrett, C., Chung, C., Fisher, M., Nagarajan, S., et al. (2014). Intensive cognitive training in schizophrenia enhances working memory and associated prefrontal cortical efficiency in a manner that drives long-term functional gains. Neuroimage, 99, 281–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.057
Suo, C., Singh, M. F., Gates, N., Wen, W., Sachdev, P., Brodaty, H., et al. (2016). Therapeutically relevant structural and functional mechanisms triggered by physical and cognitive exercise. Molecular Psychiatry, 21(11), 1633–1642. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2016.57
Swaab, D. F. (1991). Brain aging and alzheimer's disease, "wear and tear" versus "use it or lose it". Neurobiology of Aging, 12(4), 317–324. https://doi.org/10.1016/0197-4580(91)90008-8
Tan, C. C., Yu, J. T., Wang, H. F., Tan, M. S., Meng, X. F., Wang, C., et al. (2014). Efficacy and safety of donepezil, galantamine, rivastigmine, and memantine for the treatment of alzheimer's disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 41(2), 615–631. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-132690
Taya, F., Sun, Y., Babiloni, F., Thakor, N., & Bezerianos, A. (2015). Brain enhancement through cognitive training: A new insight from brain connectome. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 9, 44. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2015.00044
Thompson, T. W., Waskom, M. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2016). Intensive working memory training produces functional changes in large-scale frontoparietal networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(4), 575–588. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00916
Trujillo, J. P., Gerrits, N. J. H. M., Veltman, D. J., Berendse, H. W., Van der Werf, Y. D., & Van den Heuvel, O. A. (2014). Reduced neural connectivity but increased task-related activity during working memory in de novo parkinson patients (in press). Human Brain Mapping.
Turk-Browne, N. B. (2013). Functional interactions as big data in the human brain. Science, 342(6158), 580–584. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1238409
van Heugten, C. M., Ponds, R. W., & Kessels, R. P. (2016). Brain training: Hype or hope? Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 26(5–6), 639–644. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2016.1186101
van Velzen, L. S., Vriend, C., de Wit, S. J., & van den Heuvel, O. A. (2014). Response inhibition and interference control in obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 419. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00419
Verghese, A., Garner, K. G., Mattingley, J. B., & Dux, P. E. (2016). Prefrontal cortex structure predicts training-induced improvements in multitasking performance. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36(9), 2638–2645. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3410-15.2016
Vermeij, A., Kessels, R. P., Heskamp, L., Simons, E. M., Dautzenberg, P. L., & Claassen, J. A. (2016). Prefrontal activation may predict working-memory training gain in normal aging and mild cognitive impairment. Brain Imaging and Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-016-9508-7
Wig, G. S. (2017). Segregated systems of human brain networks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(12), 981–996. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.09.006
Willis, S. L., Tennstedt, S. L., Marsiske, M., Ball, K., Elias, J., Koepke, K. M., et al. (2006). Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. JAMA, 296(23), 2805–2814. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.296.23.2805
Wood, J. M., & Owsley, C. (2014). Useful field of view test. Gerontology, 60(4), 315–318. https://doi.org/10.1159/000356753
Xia, M., Wang, J., & He, Y. (2013). Brainnet viewer: A network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS One, 8(7), e68910. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068910
Xiong, Q., Znamenskiy, P., & Zador, A. M. (2015). Selective corticostriatal plasticity during acquisition of an auditory discrimination task. Nature, 521(7552), 348–351. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature14225
Yeo, B. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., et al. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(3), 1125–1165. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00338.2011
Zalesky, A., Fornito, A., & Bullmore, E. T. (2010). Network-based statistic: Identifying differences in brain networks. Neuroimage, 53(4), 1197–1207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041
Funding
This research was supported by Dutch Parkinson’s Disease Association (‘Parkinson Vereniging’ fellowship 19–2015/MvhH; k€ 40 to van den Heuvel / Vriend) and the Brain Foundation of the Netherlands (‘Hersenstichting’ fellowship HA-2017-00227; k€ 300 to Vriend / van den Heuvel).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
All authors contributed to the study conception. Literature search and evaluation were performed by Tim D. van Balkom and Chris Vriend. The first draft of the manuscript was written by Tim D. van Balkom and Chris Vriend and all authors critically revised the work. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Ethical Approval
This article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals performed by any of the authors.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
ESM 1
(PDF 363 kb)
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
van Balkom, T.D., van den Heuvel, O.A., Berendse, H.W. et al. The Effects of Cognitive Training on Brain Network Activity and Connectivity in Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases: a Systematic Review. Neuropsychol Rev 30, 267–286 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-020-09440-w
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-020-09440-w