Skip to main content
Log in

Reorganization of the Association between Intelligence and the Characteristics of Attention and Memory on Aging

  • Published:
Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

The association between intelligence and measures of the functions of the systems underlying attention and the recognition of verbal and image stimuli presented for memorization, as well as the lateral characteristics of speech memory were studied in members of an elderly age group (64.5 ± 6.3 years, n = 83; 43 women) and a young age group (22.0 ± 4.5 years, n = 133; 83 women). The rate of selection of information in conditions of conflict served as a predictor of the level of intelligence, independently of age. In elderly subjects, a higher level of intelligence corresponded to shorter executive control times, while no significant link between intelligence and functional measures of the attention system was seen in young people. Analysis of the properties of memory showed that reproduction of words addressed to the left hemisphere made a positive contribution to intelligence; in young people verbal memory for words addressed to the right hemisphere also made a contribution, while in elderly people there was a contribution from the efficiency of recognizing remembered verbal and image stimuli. Gender-linked features were seen in the age-related reorganization of the contribution of attention and memory to intelligence, with more marked changes in men.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Albinet, C. T., Boucard, G., Bouquet, C. A., and Audiffren, M., “Processing speed and executive functions in cognitive aging: How to disentangle their mutual relationship?” Brain Cognit., 79, 1–11 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., “Working memory: looking back and looking forward,” Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 4, No. 10, 829–839 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., and Lindenberger, U., “Life span psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning,” Annu. Rev. Psychol., 50, 471–507 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Batty, C. D., Kivimaki, M., and Deary, I. J., “Intelligence, education, and mortality,” Br. Med. J., 340, c563 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batty, G. D., Deary, I. J., and Gottfredson, L. S., “Premorbid (early life) IQ and later mortality risk: Systematic review,” Ann. Epidemiol., 17, No. 4, 278–288 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borghesani, P. R., Madhyastha, T. M., Aylward, E. H., et al., “The association between higher order abilities, processing speed, and age are variably mediated by white matter integrity during typical aging,” Neuropsychologia, 51, No. 8, 1435–1444 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, R. L., “Memory and executive function review in aging and AD: Multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate,” Neuron, 44, 195–208 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cicerone, K. D., Langenbahn, D. M., Braden, C., et al., “Evidence based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 2003 through 2008,” Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil., 92, No. 4, 519–530 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colom, R., Privado, J., Garcna, L. F., et al., “Fluid intelligence and working memory capacity: Is the time for working on intelligence problems relevant for explaining their large relationship?” Pers. Individ. Differ., 79, 75–80 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Patel, G., and Shulman, G. L., “The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind,” Neuron, 58, No. 3, 306–324 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Daselaar, S. and Cabeza, R., “Age-related changes in hemispheric organization,” in: Cognitive Neuroscience of Aging: Linking Cognitive and Cerebral Aging, Cabeza, R. et al. (eds.), Oxford Univ. Press, New York (2010), pp. 325–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deary, I. J., Johnson, W., and Starr, J., “Are processing speed tasks biomarkers of cognitive aging?” Psychol. Aging, 25, No. 1, 219–228 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deary, I., “Why do intelligent people live longer?” Nature, 456, 175–176 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dolcos, F., Rice, H. J., and Cabeza, R., “Hemispheric asymmetry and aging: right hemisphere decline or asymmetry reduction,” Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 26, No. 7, 819–825 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunst, B., Benedek, M., Koschutnig, K., et al., “Sex differences in the IQwhite matter microstructure relationship: A DTI study,” Brain Cogn., 91, 71–78 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Eckert, M. A., “Slowing down: age-related neurobiological predictors of processing speed,” Front. Neurosci. (2011), doi https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2011.00025.

  • Eng, H. Y., Chen, D. Y., and Jiang, Y. H., “Visual working memory for simple and complex visual stimuli,” Psychon. Bull. Rev, 12, 1127–1133 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J., Check Your Own IQ [Russian translation], Mir, Moscow (1972).

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H., “Intelligence: a new view,” Vopr. Psikihol., No. 1, 111–131 (1995).

  • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., et al., “Testing the effi ciency and independence of attentional networks,” J. Cogn. Neurosci., 14, No. 3, 340–347 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferreira, L., Santos, R. F. G., Ferri, C. P., and Galduróz, J. C. F., “Rate of cognitive decline in relation to sex after 60 years-of-age: A systematic review,” Geriatr. Gerontol. Int., 14, 23–31 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Finkel, D., Reynolds, C. A., McArdle, J. J., and Pedersen, N. L., “Age changes in processing speed as a leading indicator of cognitive aging,” Psychol. Aging, 22, No. 3, 558–568 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Floyd, M. and Scogin, F., “Effects of memory training on the subjective memory functioning and mental health of older adults: a meta-analysis,” Psychol. Aging, 12, 150–161 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fry, A. F. and Hale, S., “Relationships among processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence in children,” Biol. Psychol., 54, 1–34 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Groth, K. E. and Allen, P. A., “Visual attention and aging,” Front. Biosci., 5, d284–297 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haier, R. J., Jung, R. E., Yeo, R. A., et al., “The neuroanatomy of general intelligence: sex matters,” Neuroimage, 25, No. 1, 320–327 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, D. F., Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities, New York (2000). Hemmingsson, T., Melin, B., Allebeck, P., and Lundberg, I., “The association between cognitive ability measured at ages 18–20 and mortality during 30 years of follow-up – a prospective observational study among Swedish males born 1949–1951,” Int. J. Epidemiol., 35, 665–670 (2006).

  • Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., and Perrig, W. J., “Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 105, 6829–6833 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Juan-Espinosa, M., García, L. F., Escorial, S., et al., “Age dedifferentiation hypothesis: Evidence from the WAIS III,” Intelligence, 30, 395–408 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanazawa S., “General intelligence as a domain-specific adaptation,” Psychol. Rev., 111, No. 2, 512–523 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., “WAIS-III IQs, Horn’s theory, and generational changes from young adulthood to old age,” Intelligence, 29, 131–167 (2001).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luks, T. L., Oliveira, M., Possin, K. L., et al., “Atrophy in two attention networks is associated with performance on a Flanker task in neurodegenerative disease,” Neuropsychologia, 48, 165–170 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Madden, D. J., “Adult age differences in attentional selectivity and capacity,” Eur. J. Cognit. Psychol., 2, 229–252 (1990).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manard, M., Carabin, D., Jaspar, M., and Collette, F., “Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed,” BMC Neurosci., 15, 7 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Matsuyoshi, D., Osaka, M., and Osaka, N., “Age and individual differences in visual working memory defi cit induced by overload,” Front. Psychol., 5, No. 384 (2014), doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.00384 (2014).

  • Miller, L. J., Myers A, Prinzi, L., and Mittenberg, W., “Changes in intellectual functioning associated with normal aging,” Arch. Clin. Neuropsychology, 24, No. 7, 681–688 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papp, K. V., Walsh, S. J., and Snyder, P. J., “Immediate and delayed effects of cognitive interventions in healthy elderly: A review of current literature and future directions,” Alz. Dement., 5, 50–60 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peich, M.-C., Husain, M., and Bays, P. M., “Age-related decline of precision and binding in visual working memory,” Psychol. Aging, 28, No. 3, 729–743 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. and Petersen, S. E., “The attention system of the human brain,” Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 13, 25–42 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Qiu, C., Kivipelto, M., and von Strauss, E., “Epidemiology of Alzheimer’s disease: occurrence, determinants, and strategies toward intervention,” Dialog. Clin. Neurosci., 11, No. 2, 111–128 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rammsayer, T. H. and Troche, S. J., “Effects of age and the relationship between response time measures and psychometric intelligence in younger adults,” Pers. Individ. Differ., 48, 49–53 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Razumnikova, O. M., Patterns of aging of the brain and means of activating its compensatory resources,” Usp. Fiziol. Nauk., 46, No. 2, 3–16 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., et al., “No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study,” J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 142, No. 2, 359–379 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., et al., “Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET,” J. Cogn. Neuroscience, 12, 174–187 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, J. J., Sattler, J. M., and Lopez, A. J., “Age effects on Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale III subtests,” Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol., 15, 311–317 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T. A. and Ferrer-Caja, E., “What needs to be explained to account for age-related effects on multiple cognitive variables?” Psychol. Aging, 18, No. 1, 91–110 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T. A., “The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition,” Psychol. Rev., 103, 403–428 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K. W. and Willis, S. L., “Age difference patterns of psychometric intelligence in adulthood: generalizability within and across ability domains,” Psychol. Aging, 8, No. 1, 44–55 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheppard, L. D. and Vernon, P. A., “Intelligence and speed of information processing: A review of 50 years of research,” Pers. Individ. Differ., 44, 535–551 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., and Engle, R. W., “Is working memory training effective?” Psychol. Bull., 138, No. 4, 628–654 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slagter, H. A., Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., et al., “Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources,” PLoS Biol., 5, e138 (2007), doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050138

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart-Hamilton, I., The Psychology of Ageing [Russian translation], Piter, St. Petersburg (2010), 4th ed.

  • Wager, T. D. and Smith, E. E., “Neuroimaging studies of working memory: a meta-analysis,” Neuroimage, 3, 255–274 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wahlund, L. O., Almkvist, O., Basun, H., and Julin, P., “MRI in successful aging, a 5-year follow-up study from the eighth to ninth decade of life,” Magn. Reson. Imaging, 14, No. 6, 601–608 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • West, R. L., “An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging,” Psychol. Bull., 120, No. 2, 272–292 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, S., Fan, J., Lee, T. M. C., et al., “Age-related differences in attentional networks of alerting and executive control in young, middle-aged, and older Chinese adults,” Brain Cognit., 75, 205–210 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to O. M. Razumnikova.

Additional information

Translated from Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel’nosti imeni I. P. Pavlova, Vol. 67, No. 1, pp. 55–67, January–February, 2017.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Razumnikova, O.M., Vol’f, N.V. Reorganization of the Association between Intelligence and the Characteristics of Attention and Memory on Aging. Neurosci Behav Physi 48, 453–462 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11055-018-0586-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11055-018-0586-4

Keywords

Navigation