Skip to main content
Log in

The common core of relativization in Georgian

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Notes

  1. The following abbreviations are used: 1,2,3=person markers; acc=accusative; add=additive particle, aor=aorist; comp=complementizer, cop=copula; dat=dative; dem=demonstrative; erg=ergative; fut=future; gen=genitive; ins=instrumental; nom=nominative; nonact=non-active; obj=object marker; past.imperf=past imperfective; pl=plural; pres=present; prev=perfectivizing preverb; sbjv=subjunctive; sg=singular; subj=subject marker. Note that the morphological decomposition of Georgian verbs is simplified in this work to ease the presentation. The Georgian data unless otherwise indicated represents the intuition of one of the authors. Most examples have been further confirmed with several native speakers in Tbilisi.

  2. Using different terminology, the profusion of relativization strategies used by Georgian is already noted in Harris (1992) and (1994).

  3. A note on our terminology: an externally headed relative structure consists of an NP “head” and the relative clause. An internally headed relative clause will lack an external NP “head” and will instead have an internal NP “head.” Correlative structures will consist of a correlative clause and a main clause with a correlate DP. The correlative clause itself may contain an internal head or not. We will refer to correlative clauses without an overt internal head as gapped relative clauses. Often the distinction between externally headed/internally head/correlative clauses will not be relevant and we will just refer to them as rom relative clauses. The term head is semantically overloaded—in addition to the “head” of a relative clause structure, we also have the phrase structure notion of head versus phrase and the movement notion of the head versus tail of a chain. All three notions are commonly used and we will continue to do so but to keep the distinctions clear we will consistently provide disambiguating language. The relative clause “head” will always be the NP head/head NP/internal head/external head, the phrase structure “head” will always be the head of XP/Xhead, and the movement “head” will be the head of a chain.

  4. The non-initiality requirement applies to adverbial clauses also.

    1. (i)
      figure d

    We do not discuss further the non-initiality requirement on rom in relativization structures but offer a comment from a reviewer who notes that this parallel with relative clauses is not unexpected given that many adverbial clauses are structurally relative clauses, involving movement dependences similar to other relative clauses; see Foley (2013) for a synchronic syntactic analysis. Harris (1994) provides a diachronic explanation for the non-initiality requirement on rom. She takes relative clauses that use the relative pronoun (e.g. romeli ‘which’) as historically prior to rom relatives. The relative pronoun was the first element in its clause. At some point the external head underwent inverse case attraction, i.e. it started appearing with relative clause internal case and was reanalyzed as a relative clause internal constituent. The relative pronoun itself was forced into second position and eventually reanalyzed as an invariant complementizer head. In terms that we will introduce later, the externally headed relative structure was reanalyzed as an internally headed relative. Further developments involving raising of the NP head and a generalization of pre-rom scrambling would take us to the contemporary externally headed rom relative.

  5. We assume that nominals when they combine with predicates always involve a DP projection. Following Eren (2015) and Öztürk and Eren (2021), who discuss the closely related language Pazar Laz, we assume that D’s are always silent in Georgian; we briefly address the question of their semantic content in Sect. 6. The assumption that nominals always involve a DP projection is, however, not crucial to our analysis. We discuss why and outline a D-less alternative at the end of Sect. 6.

  6. Modulo pronunciation, this structure is identical to the one that Kayne (1994) proposed for his raising treatment of externally headed relative clauses. We do not, however, adopt this structure for externally headed relative clauses as that would make the wrong predictions concerning case marking on the NP head; see (30) and (31).

  7. If an IHRC only consists of intransitive verb and the subject is linearly adjacent to –ze as in (i), the sentence is marginally acceptable.

    1. (i)
      figure ag

    The minimally different (ii) is entirely unacceptable.

    1. (ii)
      figure ah

    The difference between (i) and (ii) is merely that (ii) contains an additional DP. We speculate that linear adjacency between the head and the verb makes available an alternate head external parse with a prenominal relative clause (i.e. [[....V] NP]-P) but at this point we don’t know why linear adjacency combined with absence of other relative-clause internal nominals is significant in this manner. A reviewer suggests that this state of affairs could arise from the prenominal verb functioning here as an adjective/participle, which modifies the head noun, yielding a DP which explains why a P head can follow the noun.

  8. A reviewer notes that since there can be only one hanging topic in a sentence, our analysis predicts that there should be at most one hanging topic correlative clause in one sentence. This prediction is borne out.

    1. (i)
      figure au

    However, this seems to be part of wider restriction against having multiple relative clause structures clause-initially. (i), which involves two hanging topics, is out but so are two initial gapped rom relatives and most surprisingly even two initial externally headed relatives. Neither involve hanging topics according to our analysis. At this point, we do not understand the source of this broader restriction.

  9. We thank Alice Harris for suggesting this direction to us.

  10. This is because the correlate NP c-commands the Dem and the correlative clause which contains the internal head is dominated by Dem.

  11. Our proposal is inspired by the late merge based treatment of extraposition in Fox and Nissenbaum (1999) and Fox (2002) and extensions in Bhatt and Pancheva (2004) and Takahashi and Hulsey (2009) though given the complexity of demonstrative semantics, our proposal is not formally identical to the preceding ones. We believe that this complexity is an artifact of our partial understanding of the syntax of demonstrative phrases. The literature on the structure of demonstrative phrases reflects a more articulated structure than the one we are assuming in this paper, see for example Kayne (2006).

  12. See for example Safir’s (2019) Peak Novelty Condition. The precise restriction we need departs slightly from that delivered by the Peak Novelty Condition, which only allows Merge to the root node or nodes immediately dominated by the root node. It thus does not permit the generally assumed derivation for sentences like [Which [novel [that Hemingway wrote]]] did he most admire? as here the relative clause is merged with the NP novel, which is not immediately dominated by the root node. We need to allow derivations like these—see (64)—but block other instances of deep countercyclic merge.

References

  • Belyaev, Oleg, and Dag Haug. 2020. The genesis and typology of correlatives. Language 96: 874–907.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhatt, Rajesh. 2003. Locality in correlativization. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 21(3): 485–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhatt, Rajesh, and Roumyana Pancheva. 2004. Late merge of degree clauses. Linguistic Inquiry 34(3): 1–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caponigro, Ivano, and Maria Polinsky. 2011. Relative embeddings: A Circassian puzzle for the syntax/semantics interface. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 29: 71–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, Gregory. 1977. Reference to kinds in English. PhD diss., University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1977. On wh-movement. In Formal syntax, eds. Peter Culicover, Thomas Wasow, and Adrian Akmajian, 71–132. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cinque, Guglielmo. 1977. The movement nature of left dislocation. Linguistic Inquiry 8: 397–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cinque, Guglielmo. 1990. Types of A’-dependencies. Linguistic inquiry monographs. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cinque, Guglielmo. 2020. The Syntax of Relative Clauses. Vol. 166 of Cambridge Studies in Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, Peter. 1987. The structure of internally headed relative clauses. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 5(2): 277–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, Chris. 1998. A note on extraction from conditionals. In Cornell working papers in linguistics, eds. Niken Adisasmito-Smith and Tobey Doeleman. Vol. 16, 57–66. Cornell University, Ithaca: CLC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dayal, Veneeta. 1996. Locality in wh-quantification: Questions and relative clauses in Hindi. Studies in linguistics and philosophy. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Demirok, Ömer, and Balkız Öztürk. 2021. A typology for correlatives: A view from the languages of the Caucasus. In Handbook of languages of the Caucasus, Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbourne, Paul. 2008. Demonstratives as individual concepts. Linguistics and Philosophy 31: 409–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eren, Ömer. 2015. An articleless DP language: Pazar Laz. In Proceedings of Chicago linguistic society (CLS) 51, eds. Ksenia Ershova, Joshua Falk, Jeffrey Geiger, Zachary Hebert, Robert E. Jr. Lewis, Patrick Munoz, Jacob B. Phillips, and Betsy Pillion, 137–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foley, Steven. 2013. The syntax of Georgian relative clauses. NYU Senior Honors Thesis.

  • Fox, Danny. 2002. Antecedent contained deletion and the copy theory of movement. Linguistic Inquiry 34(1): 63–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, Danny, and Jon Nissenbaum. 1999. Extraposition and scope: A case for overt QR. In Proceedings of the 18th west coast conference on formal linguistics, 132–144. Somerville: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, Alice. 1992. Changes in relativization strategies: Georgian and language universals. In Caucasologie et mythologie comparée, ed. Catherine Paris, 391–403. Paris: Peeters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, Alice. 1994. On the history of relative clauses in Georgian. In Non-Slavic languages of the USSR: Papers from the fourth conference, ed. Howard I. Aronson, 130–142. Bloomington: Slavica Publishers Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Havers, Wilhelm. 1926. Der sog. ‘Nominativus pendens’. Indogermanische Forschungen 43: 207–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewitt, George B. 1987. The typology of subordination in Georgian and Abkhaz. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hewitt, George B. 1995. Georgian: A structural reference grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hulsey, Sarah, and Uli Sauerland. 2006. Sorting out relative clauses. Natural Language Semantics 14(2): 111–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izvorski, Roumyana. 1996. The syntax and semantics of correlative proforms. In Proceedings of NELS 26, ed. Kiyomi Kusumoto, 133–147. Amherst: GLSA

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard S. 1994. The antisymmetry of syntax. Linguistic inquiry monographs. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard S. 2006. On parameters and on principles of pronunciation. In Organizing grammar: Linguistic studies in honor of Henk van Riemsdijk, eds. Hans Broekhuis, Norbert Corver, Riny Huybregts, Ursula Kleinhenz, and Jan Koster, 289–299. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, Edward. 1985. Relative clauses. In Language typology and syntactic description, ed. Timothy Shopen. Vol. 2, 141–170. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, Christopher. 2002. Comparative deletion and optimality in syntax. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 20(3): 553–621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipták, Anikó, ed. 2009. Correlatives cross-linguistically, Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nash, Léa. 2017. Simple correlatives in Georgian. Paper presented at Correlative Workshop, QMUL.

  • Nash, Léa. 2022. Correlatives and other relatives in Georgian. In Issues in Kartvelian studies, ed. Tamar Makharoblidze. Wilmington, Delaware: Vernon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Öztürk, Balkız, and Ömer Eren. 2021. The nominal domain in languages of the Caucasus. In The Oxford handbook of languages of the Caucasus, ed. Maria Polinsky. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Safir, Ken. 2019. The A/A-bar distinction as an epiphenomenon. Linguistic Inquiry 50(2): 285–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sauerland, Uli. 1998. The meaning of chains. PhD diss., Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Shimoyama, Junko. 1999. Internally headed relative clauses in Japanese and e-type anaphora. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 8: 147–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Srivastav, Veneeta. 1991. The syntax and semantics of correlatives. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 9: 637–686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, Shoichi, and Sarah Hulsey. 2009. Wholesale late merger: Beyond the A/A’-distinction. Linguistic Inquiry 40(3): 387–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vergnaud, Jean-Roger. 1974. French relative clauses. PhD diss., Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Williamson, Janice. 1987. An indefinite restriction on relative clauses in Lakhota. In The linguistic representation of (in)definiteness, eds. Eric Reuland and Alice ter Meulen. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Our greatest thanks go to our three anonymous reviewers whose contributions have had an enormous impact on this paper. We are very grateful and amazed by the depth of their engagement with our ideas. Many thanks also to our editor Hedde Zeijlstra. Next we would like to thank our native speaker consultants: Nino Amiridze, Tato Antadze, Natuka Bidzinashvili, Inesa Gelashvili, and Tamara Kalkhitashvili. Our paper benefitted from audiences at TripleA 5 in Konstanz, GLEAMS at Michigan State 2018, South Caucasian Chalk Circle 2 in Tbilisi, RALFe 2018 in Paris, FASAL 9 at Reed College, GLOW 42 in Oslo, the 2019 RNC workshop in Toronto, and Rutgers (Spring 2021). We gratefully acknowledge stimulating discussions on this topic with Carlo Cecchetto, Amy Rose Deal, Caterina Donati, Daniel Harbour, Emily Hainink, Troy Messick, and Ken Safir. Special thanks to David Erschler and Alice Harris for written comments on an early draft.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rajesh Bhatt.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bhatt, R., Nash, L. The common core of relativization in Georgian. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 41, 501–546 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-022-09547-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-022-09547-0

Keywords

Navigation