Skip to main content
Log in

Applicatives without verbs

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper, we argue that Applicatives can occur without verbs, more specifically, internal to nouns and adjectives, as previously proposed by Ingason (2015, 2016). We extend Ingason’s theory and separate the noun-internal Root-Selecting Event Applicative of the Icelandic type (Applexp) from another Root-Selecting Applicative attested in Basque (referred to here as Applgoal). Evidence supporting this hypothesis comes from two empirical domains: (i) result/process nominals taking datives attested in the syntax of headlines, where no v is projected at all; and (ii) predicative configurations headed by adjectives and process nouns taking internal datives. On the basis of distributive and morphological evidence, as well as constituency tests, we argue that the presence of these internal datives is independent from v. On the other hand, we depart from Ingason’s analysis in that the Root-selecting Applicative of the Basque type, although related to an event, does not strictly correspond to Pylkkänen’s High Appl projection, but rather resembles the Low one in that it introduces goal/source datives. Finally, we show that the Appl projection introducing experiencer datives in Basque is in fact dependent on the projection of v.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. As an anonymous reviewer notes, to state that a nominal head is causative might be problematic for the long-established connection between the verbal category and causativity (Chomsky 1970). We will not pursue this issue further, since we are not assuming the presence of a nominal head with causative flavor in our analysis.

  2. In this paper, we will address mostly DPs interpreted as goals, as these instances are the most common and extended in this empirical context in Basque. Nevertheless, there are some cases where the noun/adjective-internal dative DPs can be also interpreted as sources, as in alergia salizitatoei ‘allergy to salicylate’. In this paper, we will restrict our analysis to goals, but we consider it can be similarly extended to sources. On the other hand, although most data gathered for our study show animate goals, inanimate goals are also attested. Here we provide the reader with more examples including inanimate goals:

    1. (i)
      figure c
  3. This restriction also holds for PPs. Actually, nP-internal PPs are ungrammatical outside headlines (if not selected by the relational postposition -ko, see Sect. 3.3). As pointed out by de Rijk (1998: 378), the restriction of nP-internal PPs to headlines is also attested inlanguages such as Quechua, Japanese and Turkish.

  4. The example from Fernández and Sarasola (2010) was furnished by Josu Landa by means of the syntactic browser Corpus Arakatzaile Sintaktikoa (CAS), which is available at http://corsyntax.ametza.com/cas/, accessed 19 November 2018.

  5. This approach cannot be extended to all verbless headlines involving datives. For instance, in some cases (e.g. (i)) the dative seems to be the goal of a silent verb, presumably give, and not of the noun itself.

    1. (i)
      figure i

    Although instances like these are similar to the ones we have analyzed in the paper, unlike the headlines we are discussing they are compatible with temporal adverbs such as aurrerantzean ‘from now on’ or manner adverbs such as apurka-apurka ‘little by little’. As we show in example (9b), the headlines we are analyzing are incompatible with these adverbs.

  6. Non-eventive nouns that are somehow related to an event can be grammatical in this context. For instance, in the Basque journal Argia ‘the light’, there is a section called Zuzendariari gutuna ‘a letter to the editor’ lit. ‘a letter to the director’. In this particular case, the noun does not name an event, but it denotes an entity that is nevertheless the result of a writing event. Not all nouns of this kind seem to be grammatical, since liburua irakurzaleari / irakurzaleari liburua ‘a book to the reader’ is not acceptable. Hence, the grammaticality of this example needs further explanation. Nevertheless, we think that the main distinction applies, as other non-eventive/concrete nouns are completely unattested and seem to be out (ex. (10)a). We thank an anonymous reviewer for bringing this to our attention.

  7. The verb may not be nominalized immediately above vP. Actually, several works have proposed that -tze infinitives involve more clausal projections, like Infl/T (see, among others, Ortiz de Urbina 1989; Zabala and Odriozola 1996; Duguine 2013). For instance, these clausal projections have been argued to also be responsible for assigning the subject ergative case (e.g. Ortiz de Urbina 1989; Rezac et al. 2014). Since this is not strictly relevant for our analysis, we will assume for simplicity that nP is projected on top of vP.

  8. We are abstracting away from the discussion on the separation between Voice and v, and we presume, for the sake of simplicity, that v is both the verbalizer and the head assigning absolutive case.

  9. As a reviewer notes, analysing nres and nproc as different heads is reminiscent of Moulton’s (2014) analysis of complex event nominals, simple event nominals and result nominals, where n is associated to different semantics in each case.

  10. Chomsky’s (2001:13) PIC1 is slightly different:

    1. (i)

      The domain of H is not accessible to operations outside HP; only H and its edge are accessible to such operations.

    Following Embick (2010) and related work, we adopt the second version of the Phase Impenetrability Condition. In well-known scenarios like T agreement with the quirky nominative object in Icelandic (Chomsky 2001) and root-determined allomorphy in the English past tense (Embick 2010), it is necessary that the domain of H stays accessible until the next phase-head (i.e. Z) is merged, something that is possible with the PIC2 but not with the PIC1. More specifically, in these two cases, T (a non-phase head projected between v and C) must be able to access the material in the domain of v.

  11. In the list of derived verbs (26), both Romance loan (errespetu/errespetatu) and non-loan (gorroto/gorrotatu, aipu/aipatu) pairs are included. As can be seen, the process of lowering the final vowel to /a/ takes place in both cases. It must be noted, though, that in Spanish (one of the two Romance languages in contact with Basque), the counterpart to errespetatu is respetar, with the /a/ theme vowel attached to the verbal Root (i.e. respeta-r). In the nominal environment, the Root ends in /o/, i.e. respeto ‘respect’. On the basis of this, it could be argued that there is no lowering phonological process affecting the final vowel in Basque; rather, the Root (in the verbal context) is directly borrowed from Spanish with a final vowel /a/. The same could be argued for the pair abisu ‘warning’ and abisatu ‘to warn’, which in Spanish have the counterparts aviso ‘warning’ and avisar‘to warn’.

    This does not affect our analysis, since we argue that the vowel change is a context-dependent and lexically restricted process. The /a/ ending lexical item is the exponent of the Root in the context of the verbalizer. It does not matter whether it is listed as a phonological process affecting only certain Roots in certain derivational environments, or whether it involves Root allomorphs for some Romance-loan Roots.

  12. Our analysis is similarly compatible with a view of dative arguments as DPs, instead of KPs.

  13. Similar instances have been obtained from technical texts and exhaustively analyzed by Makazaga (1999) and Zabala (1999).

  14. We assume that dative agreement markers on the auxiliary are really dative clitics, in line with Rezac et al. (2014), Odria (2017) and references therein. In any case, nothing in our analysis hinges on this view, as our analysis is similarly compatible with the view of dative greement markers (instead of clitics).

  15. The evidence of the optional dative cliticization exemplified in (40) and (41) for the adjective-internal dative analysis is relevant for Central and Western varieties of Basque, but not for Eastern ones. In fact, Eastern varieties show optional dative cliticization in instances similar to those in (41) (Etxepare and Oyharçabal 2009, 2013; Fernández and Landa 2009; Fernández et al. 2009). As a consequence, the lack of dative cliticization in Eastern varieties can be argued to be related to the independent dative clitic drop phenomenon attested in these varieties, and not to the adjective-internal nature of the dative. Nevertheless, we would still argue that, in these predicative configurations under discussion, the dative is introduced internally to the adjective/noun also in Eastern dialects. With regards to optional dative cliticization, see also fn. 30.

  16. As a consequence, the absolutive DP in these predicative configurations is not subject to the Person Case Constraint, as pointed out by Rezac (2003, 2008, 2009 and references therein).

  17. In the case of iraun ‘remain’ in (46), the presence of a lexical verb seems to block the raising of the dative clitic first to [spec, a] and then to [spec, vP]. If it cannot rise, it will be sent to Spell Out once v is merged into the structure, together with the whole ApplP (according to PIC2). At this moment, we cannot find a theoretical explanation for this analysis. Nevertheless, we believe that the data are significant per se, since they show that dative arguments of adjectives are not necessarily cross-referenced in the finite verbal form, an aspect that is remarkable for the Basque inflectional pattern.

  18. The analysis of adverbs as including a silent P (a relational element) and a noun (a non-relational element) was already proposed in Mateu (2002) and Mateu and Rigau (2002). In particular, they propose that adjectives and adverbs must be regarded as relational elements to which a non-relational head has been incorporated or conflated.

  19. In fact, this is a fully productive process in Basque. Aspectual verbs like egon ‘(stage level) be’, ibili ‘walk’, or ari ‘be engaged in’ can take postpositional complements with a noun naming an event: lan-ean [work-INE] ‘in work’, meaning ‘working’, igeri-an [swim-INE] ‘in swim’, meaning ‘swimming’, surf-ean [surf-INE] ‘in surf”. These event-naming nouns do not have matching verbal roots. Either they need egin ‘do’ in order to be used as verbs or the Root is just different: lan egin ‘to work’ but *lan-du ‘to work’, igeri egin ‘to swim’ but *igeri-tu ‘to swim’, surf egin / surfeatu ‘to surf’ but *surf-(a)-tu etc.

  20. Note, however, that examples with a silent P do not admit adjectives. Thus, a variant of Mitxelena’s example in (64) with a silent P *Begiramen horreneske lotsagabenator is out, whereas a similar one without the adjective is good: Begiramen horreneskenator. On the other hand, it is worth noting that configurations with an overt P also admit dative goal arguments.

    1. (i)
      figure bb
  21. For a related phenomenon, see high-lowapplicatives in Wood’s (2015:216–220) typology of Icelandic applicatives. Wood’s high-low applicatives are based on Marantz (2009a, 2009b), apud Wood (2015:217), that argues that English benefactives share many properties with high applicatives and thus, involve a kind of High-Low applicative. Wood extends this idea to some Icelandic dative-nominative constructions and claims that in configurations involving change-of-state or creation verbs, some direct object DPs can be interpreted as eventualities (states or events). As Wood claims, in these configurations, the applicative takes this eventive DP as a complement, and it is therefore semantically interpreted as a High Applicative. However, this very same applicative is syntactically low as it takes a DP complement and applies a second argument to that eventive complement.

  22. A Root is not a verb but belongs to the semantic type <s,t>, i.e. it denotes a set of events. A related approach to Roots has been adopted in Levinson (2010) for pseudo-relative configurations such as She braided her hair tight, where the Root braid is proposed to denote a set of individuals before being verbalized, and as such, it can be modified by the adjective tight.

  23. The semantic denotation of the nominal head of process is not considered here, since we are going to deal with it in the analysis of predicate configurations headed by nouns (see the definitions in (81) below).

  24. It is worth noting that some psych nouns such as laket or atsegin ‘pleasure/pleasant’ can also be attested in the temere-type configuration, with an ergative marked experiencer and an absolutive stimulus (see Etxepare 2003; Fernández and Ortiz de Urbina 2010, and references therein). In this paper, we will limit our analysis to the dative-absolutive configuration of these predicates. Furthermore, other psych nous like beldur ‘fear’ and gorroto ‘hatred’ can also take dative arguments (beldurra iluntasunari [fear the darkdat] ‘fear of the dark’). Nevertheless, these dative arguments are not experiencers like those analyzed in this section but rather stimulus arguments. We leave the study of this type of psych noun for future research.

  25. It is worth mentioning that the examples in (85) have verbal counterparts that allow dative experiencers. See for instance example (i) which involves a dative experiencer and the psych noun/adjective atsegin ‘pleasure, pleasant’ in a verbal predicate. Similar instances will be presented and analysed in Sect. 6.2.

    1. (i)
      figure bl
  26. Our approach therefore diverges from other analyses of experiencers such as Landau (2010), which fail to account for the asymmetries between experiencer and goal datives in Basque.

  27. Roughly speaking, the agreement contrast in Eastern dialects is as follows: goal datives of ditransitives (e.g. eman ‘to give’), bivalent unergatives (behatu ‘to look at’), and bivalent unaccusatives (jarraiki ‘to follow’) show optional dative cliticization; by contrast, dative experiencers (ahantzi ‘to forget’) and possessors, and ethical datives never drop the dative clitic (Etxepare and Oyharçabal 2009, 2013; Fernández and Landa 2009; Fernández et al. 2009).

  28. Note that, in (98), the ungrammaticality involves the projection of the dative KP. There is nothing wrong with having a SC headed by atsegin ‘pleasure/pleasant’ in the complement position of iruditu.

    1. (i)
      figure ca

References

  • Abney, Steven P. 1987. The English noun phrase in its sentential aspect. B.A., Indiana University.

  • Adger, David, and Gilliam Ramchand. 2006. Psych nouns and predication. In North East Linguistic Society (NELS) 36, 89–103. GLSA: Amherst.

    Google Scholar 

  • Albizu, Pablo. 2001. Datibo sintagmen izaera sintaktikoaren inguruan. In Kasu eta komunztaduraren gainean [On case and agreement], eds. Beatriz Fernández and Pablo Albizu, 49–63. Bilbo: UPV/EHU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexiadou, Artemis. 2001. Functional structure in nominals: Nominalisation and ergativity. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexiadou, Artemis. 2011. Plural mass nouns and the morpho-syntax of number. In West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (WCCFL) 28, eds. Mary Byram Washburn et al., 33–41. Somerville: Cascadilla.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anagnastopoulou, Elena, and Yota Samioti. 2014. Domains within words and their meanings: A case study. In The syntax of roots and the roots of syntax, eds. Artemis Alexidou, Hagit Borer, and Florian Schäfer. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arad, Maya. 2003. Locality constraints on the interpretation of Roots: The case of Hebrew denominal verbs. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 21: 737–778.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arad, Maya. 2005. Roots and patterns: Hebrew morpho-syntax. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Artiagoitia, Xabier. 2004. The case of an enlightening, provoking and admirable Basque derivational suffix with implications for the theory of argument structure. Supplements of ASJU 46: 147–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, Mark C. 1988. Incorporation: A theory of grammatical function changing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belleti, Adriana, and Luiggi Rizzi. 1988. Psych-verbs and θ-theory. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 6: 291–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berro, Ane. 2015. Breaking verbs. From event structure to syntactic categories in Basque. Ph.D. diss., UPV/EHU and Université Bordeaux Montaigne (UBM).

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1970. Remarks on nominalization. In Readings in English transformational grammar, eds. Roderick Jacobs and Peter S. Rosenbaum, 184–221. Boston: Ginn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The Minimalist program. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In Step by step: Essays on minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik, eds. Roger Martin, David Michaels, and Juan Uriagereka. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Ken Hale: A life in language, ed. Michael Kenstowicz, 1–52. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuervo, M. Cristina. 2003. Datives at large. Ph.D. diss., MIT.

  • Demirdache, Hamida, and Myriam Uribe-Etxebarria. 2000. The primitives of temporal relations. In Step by step: Essays on minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik, eds. Roger Martin, David Michaels, and Juan Uriagereka, 157–186. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duguine, Maia. 2013. Null arguments and linguistic variation: A minimalist analysis of pro drop. Ph.D. diss., UPV/EHU and Université de Nantes.

  • Elordieta, Arantzazu. 2001. Verb movement and constituent permutation in Basque. Utrecht: LOT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Embick, David. 2010. Localism versus globalism in morphology and phonology. Linguistic Inquiry Monographs. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Embick, David. 2013. Morphemes and morphophonological loci. In Distributed morphology today: Morphemes for Morris Halle, eds. Alec Marantz and Ora Matushansky, 151–166. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Etxepare, Ricardo. 2003. Valency and argument structure in the Basque Verb. In A grammar of Basque, eds. José I. Hualde and Jon Ortiz de Urbina, 363–425. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Etxepare, Ricardo, and Bernard Oyharçabal. 2009. Bi datibo egitura ifar-ekialdeko zenbait hizkeratan. Lapurdum 13: 145–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Etxepare, Ricardo, and Bernard Oyharçabal. 2013. Datives and adpositions in North-Eastern Basque. In Variation in datives: A micro-comparative perspective, eds. Beatriz Fernández and Ricardo Etxepare, 50–95. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Euskaltzaindia [Royal Academy of the Basque Language]. 1985. Euskal gramatika: Lehen Urratsak I. Iruñea: Institución Príncipe de Viana and Euskaltzaindia.

  • Fernández, Beatriz. 1997. Egiturazko kasuaren erkaketa euskaraz. Bilbo: UPV/EHU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández, Beatriz, and Josu Landa. 2009. Datibo komunztadura beti zaindu, inoiz zaindu ez eta batzuetan baino zaintzen ez denean. Hiru ahoko aldagaia, datu iturri bi eta erreminta bat: Corsintax. Lapurdum 13: 159–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández, Beatriz, Jon Ortiz de Urbina, and Josu Landa. 2009. Komunztadurarik gabeko datiboen gakoez. In Beñat Oihartzabali gorazarre [Supplements of the International Journal of Basque Linguistics and Philology (ASJU) 43: 1–2], eds. Ricardo Etxepare, Ricardo Gómez, and Joseba A. Lakarra, 159–181. Bilbo: UPV/EHU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández, Beatriz, and Jon Ortiz de Urbina. 2010. Datiboa hiztegian. Bilbo: UPV/EHU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández, Beatriz, and Jon Ortiz de Urbina. 2012. Dative (first) complements in Basque. In Syntactic microvariation in Westmost European Languages. Journal of Portuguese Linguistics (Special Issue) 11(1): 83–98. eds. Ernestina Carrilho and Beatriz Fernández, Univ. de Lisboa: Ediçoes Colibri.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández, Beatriz, and Ibon Sarasola. 2010. Marinelei abisua: izen ondoko datibo sintagmak izenburuen sintaxian. Lapurdum 14: 55–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folli, Rafaella, and Heidi Harley. 2005. Flavors of v. In Aspectual inquiries, eds. Paula Kempchinsky and Roumyana Slabakova, 95–120. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goenaga, Patxi. 1978. Gramatika bideetan. Donostia: Erein.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goenaga, Patxi. 1985. Complementación y nominalización en euskara. ASJU 29(2): 493–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimshaw, Jane. 1990. Argument structure. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hale, Ken, and Samuel J. Keyser. 1993. On the argument structure and the lexical expression of syntactic relations. In The view from building 20: Essays in linguistics in honor of Sylvain Bromberger, eds. Ken Hale and Samuel J. Keyser, 53–109. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harley, Heidi. 2005. How do verbs get their names? Denominal verbs, manner incorporation and the ontology of Roots in English. In The syntax of aspect, eds. Nomi Erteschik-Shir and Tova Rapoport, 42–64. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hualde, José Ignacio. 1991. Basque phonology. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingason, Anton Karl. 2015. Applicatives in the noun phrase. Ms., University of Iceland.

  • Ingason, Anton Karl. 2016. Realizing morphemes in the Icelandic noun phrase. Ph.D. diss., University of Pennsylvania.

  • Kayne, Richard S. 1975. French syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laka, Itziar. 1993. Unergatives that assign ergative, unaccusatives that assign accusative. In Papers on case and agreement I: MIT Working Paper in Linguistics 14, eds. Jonathan D. Bobaljik and Colin Phillips, 149–172. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laka, Itziar. 2004. Ari progresiboaz: euskararen kasu markak. In Euskal Gramatika XXI. mendearen atarian: arazo zaharrak, azterbide berriak, eds. Pablo Albizu and Beatriz Fernández. Bilbao: UPV/EHU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laka, Itziar. 2006. Deriving split-ergativity in the progressive: The case of Basque. In Ergativity: Emerging issues, eds. Alana Johns, Diana Massam, and Juvenal Ndayiragije, 173–195. Dodrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, Idan. 2010. The locative syntax of experiencers. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legate, Julie A. 2002. Walrpiri: Theoretical implications. Ph.D. diss., MIT.

  • Legate, Julie A. 2008. Morphological and abstract case. Linguistic Inquiry 39(1): 55–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, Lisa. 2010. Arguments for pseudo-resultative predicates. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 28(1): 135–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Makazaga Jesus Maria. 1999. -kiko atzizki konposatuaz. In Zenbait gai euskara teknikoaren inguruan, ed. Juan Carlos Odriozola, 15–54. Bilbo: UPV/EHU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, Alec. 1997. No escape from syntax. In University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, Vol. 4.2, Penn Linguistics Colloquium (PLC) 21, eds. Alexis Dimitriadis, Laura Siegel et al., 201–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, Alec. 2001. Words. Handout presented at West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (WCCFL) 20, USC.

  • Marantz, Alec. 2007. Phases and words. In Phases in the theory of grammar, ed. Sook-Hee Choe, 191–222. Seoul: Dong-In Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, Alec. 2009a. Resultatives and re-resultatives: Direct objects may construct events by themselves. Philadelphia. Paper presented at the University of Pennsylvania Linguistics Speaker Series, Philadelphia.

  • Marantz, Alec. 2009b. Roots, re-, and affected agents: Can roots pull the agent under little v? Talk given at Roots workshop, Universität Stuttgart.

  • Mateu, Jaume, and Laia Amadas. 1999. Extended argument structure: Progressive as unaccusative. CatWPL 7: 159–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mateu, Jaume. 2002. Argument structure: Relational construal at the syntax-semantics interface. Ph.D. diss., Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona.

  • Mateu, Jaume, and Gemma Rigau. 2002. A minimalist account of conflation processes: Parametric variation at the lexicon-syntax interface. In Theoretical approaches to universals, ed. Artemis Alexiadou, 211–236. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matushansky, Ora. 2006. Head movement in linguistic theory. Linguistic Inquiry 37: 69–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGinnis, Martha. 2001. Phases and the syntax of applicatives. In North East Linguistic Society (NELS) 31, eds. Minjoo Kim and Uri Strauss.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGinnis, Martha, and Donna Gerdts. 2004. A phase-theoretic analysis of Kinyarwanda multiple applicatives. In 2003 CLA Annual Conference, eds. Sophie Burelle and Stana Somesfalean, 154–165. Département de linguistique et de didactique des langues, Université du Québec à Monreal.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGinnis, Martha. 2005. UTAH at merge: Evidence from multiple applicatives. In MITWPL 49: Perspectives on phases, eds. Martha McGinnis and Norvin Richards, 183–200. Cambridge: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merchant, Jason. 2004. Fragments and ellipsis. Linguistics and Philosophy 27: 661–738.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moulton, Keir. 2014. Simple event nominalizations. In Cross-linguistic investigations of nominalization patterns [Linguistik Aktuel / Linguistics Today 210], ed. Ileana Paul, 119–144. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Odria, Ane. 2017. Differential object marking and datives in Basque. Ph.D. diss., UPV/EHU.

  • Ortiz de Urbina, Jon. 1989. Parameters in the grammar of Basque. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortiz de Urbina, Jon and Beatriz Fernández. 2016. Datives in Basque bivalent unergatives. In Microparameters in the grammar of Basque, eds. Beatriz Fernández and Jon Ortiz de Urbina, 67–93. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyharçabal, Bernard. 2010. Basque ditransitives. In Argument structure and syntactic relations, eds. Maia Duguine, Susana Huidobro, and Nerea Madariaga. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pineda, Anna. 2014. Les fronteres de la (in)transitivitat. Estudi dels aplicatius en llenggües romàniques i basc. Ph.D. diss., Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona.

  • Preminger, Omer. 2012. The absence of an implicit object in unergatives: New and old evidence from Basque. Lingua 122(3): 278–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylkkänen, Liina. 2008. Introducing arguments. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rezac, Milan. 2003. The fine structure of cyclic Agree. Syntax 6: 156–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rezac, Milan. 2008. The syntax of eccentric agreement: The person case constraint and absolutive displacement in Basque. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 26: 61–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rezac, Milan. 2009. Person restrictions in Basque intransitives. Lapurdum 13: 305–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rezac, Milan. 2011. Phi-features and the modular architecture of language. Studies in Language and Linguistic Theory, Vol. 81. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rezac, Milan, Pablo Albizu, and Ricardo Etxepare. 2014. The structural ergative of Basque and the theory of Case. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 32: 1273–1330.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Rijk, Rudolf P. G. 1998. Basque Hospitality and the suffix -ko. In Generative studies in Basque Linguistics, eds. José I. Hualde and Jon Ortiz de Urbina, 145–162. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Rijk, Rudolf P. G. 2008. Standard Basque: A progressive grammar. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • San Martin, Itziar. 2009. Derived nominals from the nominal perspective. ASJU 43: 831–846.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torrego, Esther. 1988. Pronouns and determiners: A DP analysis of Spanish nominals. Ms., University of Massachusetts.

  • Uriagereka, Juan. 1995. Aspects of the syntax of clitic placement in Western Romance. Linguistic Inquiry 26: 79–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, Jim. 2015. Icelandic morphosyntax and argument structure. Vol. 90 of Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zabala, Igone. 1999. Izen-sintagma konplexuak: adjektiboen segidak. In Zenbait gai euskara teknikoaren inguruan, ed. Juan Carlos Odriozola, 107–158. Bilbo: UPV/EHU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zabala, Igone, and Juan Carlos Odriozola. 1996. On the relation between DP and TP: The structure of Basque infinitivals. Catalan Journal of Linguistics 5: 231–281.

    Google Scholar 

Corpora

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the three anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments on a previous version of this paper. We would also like to thank Jon Ortiz de Urbina for discussion and valuable comments and José Ignacio Hualde for feedback. The authors also acknowledge the research funding received from the Basque Government (IT665-13), the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (FFI2014-51878-P) and Agence National de la Recherche (ANR-17-CE27-0011-01). The research leading to these results has also received funding from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Program for research, technological development and demonstration under grant agreement no. 613465.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Beatriz Fernández.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Berro, A., Fernández, B. Applicatives without verbs. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 37, 1273–1317 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-018-09437-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-018-09437-4

Keywords

Navigation