Introduction

Positive affirmations have invaded the mainstream. The practice of affirming positive statements via journaling, meditation, or verbal repetition is intended to challenge negative self-beliefs and engender a positive self-image. While this practice is partly rooted in the New Thought and New Age movements (e.gs., Byrne, 2006; Carnegie, 1936; Haye, 1984; Hicks & Hicks, 2006; Vincent Peale, 1952) that gained popularity in the second half of the twentieth century, positive affirmations are now widely established via social media and performed by ‘Gen Z’ and ‘Millennials’. For instance, the TikTok trend, “Lucky Girl Syndrome”, went viral on TikTok in January 2023. The trend encouraged users to recite daily affirmations such as “I am so lucky” and “Everything always works out for me” with the promise that repeating these positive statements will bring good fortune. Despite the widespread popularity of affirmations, the science behind them (and the lack thereof) has been heavily questioned (e.g. Gormon, 2023). While the material benefits of “Lucky Girl Syndrome” might be slightly beyond the scope of evidence-based psychological research, self-affirmation theory (Steele, 1988) and related research have extensively demonstrated the psychological benefits of affirming the self in response to threats over the last 30 years (Cohen & Sherman, 2014; McQueen & Klein, 2006).

The frequent practice of repeating affirmations would suggest that people can develop a propensity to self-affirm in response to threats and challenges over time. Recently, researchers have begun to study individual inclinations to self-affirm to capture such habits (Harris et al., 2019). Indeed, the tendency to respond to psychological threats with self-affirming cognitions (Harris et al., 2019; Pietersma & Dijkstra, 2012) may offer ample psychological benefits. Boredom is a common and negative experience, accompanied by low feelings of meaning, that can be highly problematic for well-being in its chronic form (i.e. boredom proneness; Fahlman et al., 2009; Goldberg et al., 2011; Pfattheicher et al., 2021; Van Tilburg et al., 2019b). In the current research, we proposed that spontaneous self-affirmation may serve as a psychological resource to buffer against boredom by increasing perceptions of meaning in life.

Boredom: A psychological threat

Boredom is an unpleasant emotion involving feeling unchallenged (Van Tilburg & Igou, 2012), meaningless (Van Tilburg & Igou, 2011), unengaged (Eastwood et al., 2012), and like time is passing slowly (Danckert & Allman, 2005). Momentary experiences of boredom occur quite frequently (Chin et al., 2017) and can provoke both undesirable (e.g. non-suicidal self-injury, Nederkoorn et al., 2016) and desirable (e.g. prosocial intentions, Van Tilburg & Igou, 2017b) outcomes. Meanwhile, being susceptible to boredom, typically referred to as boredom proneness, comes with dire consequences for well-being (e.gs., Fahlman et al., 2009; Goldberg et al., 2011). Boredom proneness is a trait-like construct, reflecting individual differences in the frequency of being bored, the intensity of boredom, and general perceptions of life being boring (Tam et al., 2021; Van Tilburg et al., 2024).

Research consistently demonstrates the negative consequences of boredom proneness for psychological and physical well-being. For instance, boredom proneness is associated with increased depression and anxiety symptoms (Fahlman et al., 2009; Goldberg et al., 2011), aggressive tendencies (Pfattheicher et al., 2021; Van Tilburg et al., 2019b), loneliness (Skues et al., 2016), lower levels of intrinsic motivation (Pekrun et al., 2010), and a lack of physical exercise (Wolff et al., 2021). Boredom proneness is also linked to problematic behaviors with negative societal implications, such as risk-taking (Kılıç et al., 2020), impulsivity (Cao & An, 2020; Moynihan et al., 2017), binge drinking (Biolcati et al., 2016), and online trolling (Thacker & Griffiths, 2012). In context, boredom proneness can lead to poorer supervisor ratings of job performance in the workplace (Watt & Hargis, 2010), predict more cheating behaviors (Blais et al., 2023) and negatively impact student engagement and performance in educational settings (Sharp et al., 2020). Evidently, boredom proneness is a problematic dispositional tendency. It appears to impede healthy psychological functioning and is associated with many societal harms.

While research has demonstrated the negative consequences of boredom proneness, little is known about how proneness to boredom can be reduced and prevented. Our perspective starts with the notion that boredom is a psychological threat, as it calls into question our sense of personal meaning and significance (Van Tilburg & Igou, 2012). Perceiving one’s life to be meaningful is a core psychological need (Heine et al., 2006; Igou & Van Tilburg, 2021; Moynihan et al., 2021). Thus, threats to meaning can be threats to our self-integrity. Understanding this essential facet of boredom proneness is crucial for the conceptualization of psychological processes that are suitable for containing boredom proneness. We propose that individual differences to self-affirm in response to threats are particularly suited for inhibiting boredom proneness. People are proposed to self-affirm in response to a threat to their self-integrity (i.e. a psychological threat). This psychological threat arises from the perception of an external challenge to the adequacy of the self (Cohen & Sherman, 2014; Steele, 1988). As we outline below, self-affirmation is an effective resource for alleviating the impact of psychologically threatening information, thus we propose that it is aptly matched to protect against the psychological threat that boredom evokes.

Self-affirmation: A psychological resource

According to self-affirmation theory, people have a fundamental need to maintain a positive self-image and protect their self-integrity (Steele, 1988). Self-affirmation procedures originated as an experimental method developed by Steele and colleagues (Steele, 1988; Steele & Liu, 1983). The most used self-affirmation manipulation involves affirming an important value to the individual and explaining why it is important to them (Cohen & Sherman, 2014). This procedure counteracts the negative impact of threats to the perceived integrity of the self by demonstrating one’s adequacy (Steele, 1988). Subsequent work shows that individuals can also self-affirm by reflecting on their personal strengths (McQueen & Klein, 2006) and positive social relationships (e.g. Cai et al., 2013). This line of research suggests that self-affirmation manipulations have important psychological implications, such as buffering against threats and reliably reducing defensive responses to psychologically threatening information (for reviews, see McQueen & Klein, 2006; Sherman & Cohen, 2006). There are also psychological gains of self-affirmation. For example, self-affirmed participants appear to experience less stress and worry than their non-affirmed counterparts (Sherman et al., 2009).

The mechanisms underlying the benefits of self-affirmation have been disputed. Self-affirmation theory suggests that writing about important values bolsters the self, leading to these psychological benefits. However, value-based affirmation does not seem to increase self-esteem (Crocker et al., 2008; Schmeichel & Martens, 2005). Rather, researchers have proposed that affirming important values may enable self-transcendence by reminding individuals about what they care about beyond themselves, which consequently increases feelings of love and connection (Burson et al., 2012; Crocker et al., 2008; Lindsay & Creswell, 2014). Consistently, self-affirmation promotes higher levels of construal, allowing individuals to take an abstract, broader perspective of a challenging event or stimulus (Schmeichel & Vohs, 2009).

Much self-affirmation research to date has focused on experimental manipulations. However, some individuals are also likely to be naturally inclined to self-affirm in the face of psychologically threatening information. To address this gap, researchers have begun to investigate individual differences in the propensity to self-affirm in everyday life (Harris et al., 2019; Pietersma & Dijkstra, 2012). These individual differences to self-affirm in response to psychological threats have been referred to as cognitive self-affirmation inclination (Pietersma & Dijkstra, 2012) and, more recently, spontaneous self-affirmation (Harris et al., 2019). We adopt the latter term. By spontaneous self-affirmation, Harris et al. (2019) characterize self-affirmation as an individual’s naturally occurring response to a perceived psychological threat. They proposed the Spontaneous Self-Affirmation Measure (SSAM) to measure this construct. The scale represents three sources people use to self-affirm, reflecting on personal strengths and attributes, important values, and social relationships. When their self-integrity feels threatened, individuals may spontaneously remind themselves of these aspects of their life that affirm a positive view of the self.

Given the plethora of benefits associated with self-affirmation inductions, a dispositional tendency to self-affirm is likely to be a valuable and adaptive psychological trait. Emerging research suggests that this indeed might be the case. Spontaneous self-affirmation, like induced self-affirmation, is associated with positive psychological outcomes and can promote self-esteem, habitual positive self-thought, well-being, adaptive coping, and less depression and anxiety (Harris et al., 2019, 2022; Jessop et al., 2022). However, given the recent development of a psychometric measurement of this construct, much remains to be discovered about the correlates and potential implications of this individual difference and if the benefits of induced self-affirmation also extend to spontaneous self-affirmation.

Spontaneous self-affirmation versus boredom proneness: The role of meaning in life

Perceiving that one’s life is meaningful is a central human need (Baumeister, 1991; Heine et al., 2006) and is considered vital for well-being (e.g. Zika & Chamberlain, 1992). Despite definitional ambiguity surrounding the construct of meaning in life, researchers have generally arrived at the consensus that a meaningful life involves feeling that one’s life makes sense (coherence), is imbued with purpose, and has significance (Costin & Vignoles, 2020; King et al., 2006; Steger, 2012). Self-affirmation manipulations typically involve affirming what values are most meaningful in people’s lives (e.g. Schmeichel & Martens, 2005). Thus, self-affirmation and meaning perceptions are closely related. Pursuing goals that align with personal values leads to greater meaning in life (McGregor & Little, 1998); thus, being reminded of meaningful values should naturally boost perceptions of meaning in life. Indeed, a two-week self-affirmation intervention that focused on values increased perceptions of meaning in life (Nelson et al., 2014). Similarly, affirming individual strengths and social relationships will boost meaning in life through self-esteem and belongingness needs, according to the Meaning Maintenance Model (Heine et al., 2006). We propose that individuals who are inclined to self-affirm will also perceive their lives to be more meaningful.

There is initial evidence to suggest that spontaneous self-affirmation will increase perceptions of meaning in life. Spontaneous self-affirmation predicts eudaimonic well-being, cross-sectionally and longitudinally (Jessop et al., 2022), and eudaimonic well-being focuses on the experience of a meaningful life (Ryan & Deci, 2001). However, while self-affirmation theory is centered around affirming what is meaningful to the individual, the relationship between spontaneous self-affirmation and meaning in life has not yet been empirically demonstrated. We addressed this void as part of our larger investigation.

Boredom proneness is consistently associated with lower perceptions of meaning in life, and meaninglessness is a distinctive characteristic of boredom (e.g. Chan et al., 2018; Igou & Van Tilburg, 2021; O'Dea et al., 2022; Van Tilburg & Igou, 2017a). Thus, boredom serves as a psychological threat by evoking feelings of meaninglessness. Emerging research has begun to demonstrate how individual differences that support increased perceptions of meaning in life predict lower boredom proneness (Coughlan et al., 2019; O'Dea et al., 2022; Van Tilburg et al., 2019a). For example, O'Dea et al., 2022 found that individuals with high levels of self-compassion (compassion towards one’s own suffering) are less prone to boredom, and this relationship was mediated by greater levels of perceived meaning in life. Notably, self-affirmation has been shown to increase feelings of self-compassion (Lindsay & Creswell, 2014). As previously mentioned, the Meaning Maintenance Model (Heine et al., 2006) proposes that strengthening meaning through psychological resources, like self-affirmation, can buffer against threats to the self. Self-affirmation reliably buffers against psychological threats and promotes meaning sources (Harris et al., 2022; Lindsay & Creswell, 2014; McQueen & Klein, 2006; Sherman & Cohen, 2006). We thus propose that spontaneous self-affirmation will predict less boredom proneness via higher levels of meaning in life.

Overview

Boredom proneness is a trait-like psychological tendency associated with a myriad of negative well-being outcomes (Tam et al., 2021; Van Tilburg et al., in press). Understanding the predictors and mechanisms associated with the experience is vital to conceptualise how this trait-like experience can be reduced and prevented. Boredom proneness is accompanied by perceptions of low meaning (O'Dea et al., 2022; Van Tilburg & Igou, 2012), meanwhile self-affirmation is a method of bolstering meaning (Heine et al., 2006; Nelson et al., 2014; Schmeichel & Martens, 2005). Thus, we hypothesise that spontaneous self-affirmation, an adaptive psychological tendency (Harris et al., 2022), will predict lower levels of boredom proneness via greater perceptions of meaning in life. We proposed and tested the notion that an inclination to self-affirm will be positively associated with meaning in life (Study 1a) and negatively related to boredom proneness (Study 1b). In Studies 2a and 2b, we tested our central hypothesis, that the relationship between spontaneous self-affirmation and boredom proneness would be mediated by greater perceptions of meaning in life. We conducted four studies to systematically test our hypotheses across different procedures and samples. All studies received ethical approval from the Education and Health Sciences Research Ethics Committee at the University of Limerick and were programmed via Qualtrics. Data and materials are openly available at https://osf.io/7r4b2/?view_only=319f664876aa464e87e12711e21937da.

Studies 1a and 1b

First, we set out to establish the direct relationship between our variables of interest. In Study 1a, we hypothesised that spontaneous self-affirmation would be positively associated with perceptions of meaning in life. In Study 1b, we hypothesised that spontaneous self-affirmation would be negatively associated with boredom proneness.

Methods

Study 1a

Participants and design

There were 166 participants recruited on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk; https://www.mturk.com/) to complete the study in return for payment. We required 109 participants to have a power of (1–β) = 0.90 (Faul et al., 2007) assuming a correlation of r = 0.30, adopting a Type-I error of α = 0.05 (two-tailed). We exceeded this sample size to account for online dropouts. Ten participants were automatically removed from the study after failing the initial attention check, leaving 156 participants in the final sample (81 women, 75 men; Mage = 23.19, SDage = 12.46). Most of the sample were US American (96%; 1 Asian, 1 Canadian, 1 Dominican, 1 Italian, 2 Native American, 1 Russian).

Procedure and materials

After giving informed consent, participants completed an attention check where they were required to indicate what they were asked to do in the study. Participants who answered incorrectly (“Read an article about the impact of nature on fitness”) were excluded from the analysis. Demographic details regarding age, gender, and nationality were collected. The tendency to self-affirm was measured using the Spontaneous Self-Affirmation Measure (SSAM; Harris et al., 2019), a 13-item scale with three subscales reflecting strengths (4 items), values (4 items), and social relations (5 items). Participants responded to these items on a scale of 1 (disagree completely) to 7 (agree completely; ω = 0.95; e.g. “When I find myself feeling threatened or anxious by people or events, I find myself thinking about my strengths”). Presence of meaning in life was measured using the 5-item Presence of Meaning in Life subscale from the Meaning in Life Questionnaire (MP-MLQ; Steger et al., 2006), e.g. “I understand my life’s meaning”, with one reverse-scored item “My life has no clear purpose” (1 = absolutely untrue, 7 = absolutely true; ω = 0.93). Participants were then thanked, debriefed, and rewarded for their participation.

Study 1b

Participants and design

Again, we required 109 participants to have a power of (1–β) = 0.90 (Faul et al., 2007), assuming a correlation of r = 0.30, adopting a Type-I error of α = 0.05 (two-tailed). We recruited 170 participants on MTurk for the study; however, 16 failed the initial attention check and were excluded, resulting in a final sample of 154 (72 women, 82 men; Mage = 24.06, SDage = 13.55). The majority of the sample were US American in nationality (95%, 1 Asian, 1 Middle Eastern, 2 Native American, 1 Russian, 1 Spanish, 1 Turkish).

Procedure and materials

Participants gave informed consent, completed the attention check, and reported demographic details as in Study 1a. The tendency to self-affirm was measured using the same measure as Study 1a (ω = 0.95). Boredom proneness was measured using the Short Boredom Proneness Scale (BPS Short Form; Farmer & Sundberg, 1986; Struk et al., 2017; ω = 0.93), an 8-item scale (e.g. “I often find myself at “loose ends”, not knowing what to do”; 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). Once the measures were completed, participants were thanked, debriefed, and rewarded for their participation.

Results and discussion

Study 1a

Spontaneous self-affirmation was positively correlated with meaning in life (Table 1). This finding supports our hypothesis that individuals who are inclined to self-affirm in response to psychological threats experience greater meaning in life. Next, we examined the relationship between spontaneous self-affirmation and boredom proneness.

Table 1 Correlation Matrix, Means, and Standard Deviations (SD) of Measures in Studies 1a &1b

Study 1b

The correlation between boredom proneness and the inclination to self-affirm in response to threats was negative and significant (Table 1). This result supports our hypothesis that individuals inclined to self-affirm are less prone to boredom.

After Studies 1a and 1b confirmed that spontaneous self-affirmation is linked to both meaning in life and boredom, we developed a test of our mediational model, namely that spontaneous self-affirmation predicts lower levels of boredom through a greater sense of meaning in life, in Studies 2a and 2b.

Studies 2a and 2b

Studies 2a and 2b investigated the mediating role of meaning in life from spontaneous self-affirmation to boredom proneness. Specifically, we hypothesised that spontaneous self-affirmation would be negatively associated with boredom proneness via perceptions of meaning in life. To replicate our findings, we conducted a study identical in method to Study 2a with Irish undergraduate students (Study 2b).

Method

Study 2a

Participants and design

We required 136 participants to have a power of (1–β) = 0.90 to detect indirect effects with a serial mediator (Schoemann et al., 2017), assuming correlations of r = 0.40, estimated with 1000 replications using 20,000 Monte-Carlo draws and assuming a Type-I error of α = 0.05 (two-tailed). We exceeded that sample size in accounting for potential dropouts associated with online studies. There were 267 participants initially recruited on MTurk, but 53 were automatically excluded after the attention check. Thus, there were 214 participants in the final sample (101 women, 114 men; Mage = 23.51, SDage = 11.12; 98% US American; 1 Asian, 1 Malaysian, 2 Native American, 1 Pakistani).

Procedure and materials

As in Study 1a and Study 1b, participants offered informed consent, completed the attention check, and reported demographic details. They then completed the same measures of spontaneous self-affirmation (ω = 0.96), presence of meaning in life (ω = 0.85), and boredom proneness (ω = 0.94) used in the studies above. Participants also completed another measure of boredom proneness, the Harthouse Boredom Proclivity Scale (HBPS; Van Tilburg et al., 2019a; ω = 0.95), a four-item scale (e.g. “How prone are you to feeling bored?”; 1 = never, 7 = all the time). The final item differed in response by asking “Specifically, how often do you feel bored?” (1 = once or twice a year, 7 = at least once a day). This was to ensure that the relationship between self-affirmation and boredom was robust across different boredom measures (O’Dea et al., 2022), especially since the boredom proneness scale faces some challenges regarding its validity (Van Tilburg et al., 2024). The measures were presented in random order to control for potential order effects. Once the study was complete, participants were thanked, debriefed, and rewarded for their participation.

Study 2b

Participant and design

We recruited 120 undergraduate students from the University of Limerick in return for course credit. We recruited as many participants as possible from this limited participant pool of students, but we aimed for at least 100. After failing the attention check, 15 students were automatically excluded, resulting in an effective sample of 105 participants, yielding a power of (1–β) = 0.86 to detect indirect effects with a serial mediator (Schoeman et al., 2017), assuming correlations of r = 0.40, estimated with 1000 replications using 20,000 Monte-Carlo draws and assuming a Type-I error of α = 0.05 (two-tailed). The sample consisted of 36 males, 68 females, and one ‘other’ who were between 18 and 47 years of age (Mage = 19.90, SDage = 3.44). The majority of the participants were Irish (85.7%; 3 US American, 2 English; 2 French 1 Brazilian, 1 Emirati; 1 Polish; 1 Saudi; 1 Swiss).

Procedure and materials

The procedure was identical to Study 2a. Spontaneous self-affirmation (ω = 0.93), presence of meaning in life (ω = 0.91), and boredom proneness (BPS Short Form, ω = 0.90, and HBPS, ω = 0.93) were measured in the same manner as Study 2a. After successfully completing the survey, participants were awarded course credit via SONA, the university’s research participation website.

Results and discussion

Study 2a

The HBPS and BPS Short Form were combined to form one boredom proneness index (BPI) given their high correlation, r = 0.78, p < 0.001. As can be seen in Table 2, there was a positive relationship between spontaneous self-affirmation and meaning in life, replicating Study 1a. There was a negative relationship between perceptions of meaning in life and boredom proneness, as expected. However, there was a positive relationship between an inclination to self-affirm and boredom proneness, contradicting the findings of Study 1b.

Table 2 Correlation matrix, means, and standard deviations (SD) of measures in studies 2a & 2b

To test our mediational model, we used PROCESS (Hayes, 2021; Model 4) with robust standard errors (HC3) and 10,000 bootstraps. We entered spontaneous self-affirmation as the predictor, presence of meaning in life as mediator, and boredom proneness as the outcome variable. Spontaneous self-affirmation positively predicted meaning in life, B = 0.555, SE = 0.081, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.394, 0.715]. Meaning presence was significantly negatively associated with boredom proneness, B = − 0.596, SE = 0.104, p < 0.001, 95% CI [− 0.801, − 0.391]. The total effect of spontaneous self-affirmation on boredom proneness was significant and positive, B = 0.292, SE = 0.078, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.138, 0.445]. It was comprised of a significant direct effect of spontaneous self-affirmation on boredom proneness, B = 0.622, SE = 0.090, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.446, 0.799], and, crucially, a significant negative indirect effect through meaning presence, B = − 0.331, SE = 0.082, 95% CI [− 0.510, − 0.189] (see Fig. 1). This supports our hypothesis that individuals inclined to self-affirm are less prone to boredom as they experience greater meaning in life.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Path Model Study 2a. Spontaneous self-affirmation, presence of meaning in life, and boredom proneness. Indirect effect: B = − 0.331, SE = 0.082, 95% CI [− 0.510, − 0.189]. *p ≤ .05, **p ≤ .01, ***p ≤ .001

Study 2b

Akin to Study 2a, the HBPS and BPS Short Form were highly correlated, r = 0.75, p < 0.001, and combined to form a BPI index. As can be seen in Table 2, spontaneous self-affirmation correlates positively with perceptions of meaning in life and negatively with boredom proneness, consistent with Study 1b but contradicting Study 2a. Again, we find a negative relationship between perceptions of meaning in life and boredom proneness.

Using PROCESS Model 4 (Hayes, 2021) with robust standard errors (HC3) and 10,000 bootstraps, we tested the mediational model as in Study 2a. Spontaneous self-affirmation significantly positively predicted meaning in life, B = 0.400, SE = 0.107, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.188, 0.613], while meaning presence was significantly negatively associated with boredom proneness, B = − 0.282, SE = 0.082, p < 0.001, 95% CI [− 0.444, − 0.119]. The total effect of spontaneous self-affirmation on boredom proneness was significant and negative, B = − 0.247, SE = 0.082, p = 0.003, 95% CI [− 0.410, − 0.084]. The direct effect of spontaneous self-affirmation on boredom proneness was not significant, B = − 0.134, SE = 0.077, p = 0.08, 95% CI [− 0.287, 0.018]. Importantly, there was a significant negative indirect effect through meaning presence, B = − 0.123, SE = 0.045, 95% CI [− 0.214, − 0.037] (see Fig. 2). This adds further support to our hypothesis that individuals inclined to self-affirm are less prone to boredom as they experience greater meaning in life.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Path Model Study 2b. Spontaneous self-affirmation, presence of meaning in life, and boredom proneness. Indirect effect: B = − 0.123, SE = 0.045, 95% CI [− 0.214, − 0.037]. *p ≤ .05, **p ≤ .01, ***p ≤ .001

General discussion

Boredom represents a psychological threat as it features an appraised lack of meaning in one’s situation or life in general (Barbalet, 1999; Chan et al., 2018; Van Tilburg & Igou, 2012, 2017a). Spontaneous self-affirmation is an individual difference that buffers against psychological threats and is likely to increase perceptions of meaning (Harris et al., 2019; Jessop et al., 2022). We hypothesized and found that spontaneous self-affirmation is associated with greater perceptions of meaning in life and, consequently, lower boredom proneness. These four studies provide evidence for the prediction that individuals inclined to self-affirm in response to perceived threat have greater perceptions of meaning in life (Study 1a, Study 2a, & Study 2b) and, consequently, are less prone to boredom (Studies 2a and 2b).

In Study 1a, Study 2a, and Study 2b, spontaneous self-affirmation consistently predicted greater levels of meaning in life, supporting our hypotheses. This suggests that individuals inclined to self-affirm in the face of threats via their values, strengths, or social relationships, perceive their lives to be more meaningful. Given that perceiving one’s life to be meaningful is considered vital for well-being (Zika & Chamberlain, 1992), this positive association is consistent with the emerging research demonstrating the psychological benefits of spontaneous self-affirmation (Harris et al., 2019; Jessop et al., 2022). This relationship also supports the extended self-affirmation literature, which finds that affirming important values can bolster meaning and buffer against psychological threats (e.g. Hales et al., 2016; Nelson et al., 2014; Schmeichel & Martens, 2005).

Crucially, Study 2a and 2b confirm our hypothesis that spontaneous self-affirmation predicts lower levels of boredom proneness via greater perceptions of meaning in life. That is, we found a significant negative indirect effect of spontaneous self-affirmation on boredom proneness through meaning in life in both studies. These results confirm that spontaneous self-affirmation is associated with greater perceptions of meaning in life which in turns predicts lower levels of boredom proneness. Individuals with a tendency to self-affirm in response to a threat have elevated levels of meaning in life, which makes them less vulnerable to boredom. This finding further supports the notion that spontaneous self-affirmation is an adaptive individual difference that can buffer against psychological threats. In addition, this research strengthens the theoretical proposition that perceptions of meaning in life are central to regulating boredom (Chan et al., 2018; Van Tilburg & Igou, 2011, 2012).

The link between boredom and low perceptions of meaning is well-established. More recently, research has demonstrated how augmenting meaning through various meaning sources can buffer against boredom experiences (Coughlan et al., 2019; O’Dea et al., 2022; Van Tilburg et al., 2019a). Increasing meaning appears to be an effective means of ameliorating boredom and its negative consequences. Particularly germane to the present research, Coughlan et al. (2019) found that participants who affirmed heroes experienced higher levels of meaning in life and lower levels of boredom proneness. We found that a trait inclination to affirm the self has similar relationships with meaning and boredom. Evidently, meaning in life is a crucial variable to incorporate when trying to understand and regulate boredom experiences. As aforementioned, boredom proneness is associated with detrimental well-being (e.g. elevated depression symptoms; Goldberg et al., 2011) and social outcomes (e.g. sadistic aggression; Pfattheicher et al., 2021). While empirical research has extensively demonstrated these negative implications, relatively little is known about how we can avoid chronic boredom. Given the prevalence of boredom, this research has broad practical implications for individuals in a range of settings, including education and the workplace. By fostering perceptions of meaning in life, spontaneous self-affirmation may be an adaptive psychological quality for reducing and preventing chronic boredom.

Study 1b and Study 2b confirmed a negative relationship between spontaneous self-affirmation and boredom proneness. Harris et al. (2019) raised the possibility that those higher in spontaneous self-affirmation may experience the world as less threatening, which would support a negative relationship between boredom proneness and spontaneous self-affirmation. However, Study 2a found a positive zero-order correlation between the two variables. This was surprising given that chronic boredom negatively impacts well-being (e.g. Fahlman et al., 2009), while spontaneous self-affirmation has been proposed to contribute to well-being (Jessop et al., 2022). It may be that individuals who are prone to boredom, accompanied by lower perceptions of meaning (Igou & Van Tilburg, 2021; Moynihan et al., 2021), have, over time, become inclined to self-affirm in response to this threat as an adaptive strategy, lending to a positive correlation. The difference may also be, in part, attributed to the characteristics of the samples. Descriptive analyses suggest that the US sample (Study 2a) had better psychological well-being (higher meaning and self-affirmation levels and lower boredom proneness) than the Irish undergraduate sample (Study 2b). Cultural or age differences may have impacted these findings, which would be an interesting topic for further investigation. We found predominant evidence (Study 1b and Study 2b) to suggest that the relationship between these variables is, in general, more likely to be negative. However, the relationship between these constructs is likely complex, and several factors may be underlying their relation not addressed in the current investigation.

Consistent with the self-transcendence perspective on the mechanisms underlying self-affirmation (e.g. Crocker et al., 2008), spontaneous self-affirmation may also activate positive other-directed feelings like love and connection, strong sources of meaning in life (Baumeister, 1991; King & Hicks, 2021). As aforementioned, Lindsay and Creswell (2014) find that a self-affirmation manipulation activates feelings of increased self-compassion. Moreover, they find that self-affirmation specifically enhances self-compassion for individuals with low levels of trait self-compassion, consistent with the idea that self-affirmation can boost deficient psychological resources. Spontaneous self-affirmation may be a means through which these positive emotions are activated. Future research should investigate if spontaneous self-affirmation promotes these emotions, as a state or trait, and consequently, bolsters meaning in life. These variables may further explain the relationship between self-affirmation and boredom proneness and the conflicting correlations we find between Study 1b, Study 2a, and Study 2b.

The presented research has some limitations. Notably, the findings presented in this article are based on cross-sectional, correlational evidence and are limited to individual differences in self-affirmation, meaning in life, and boredom proneness. Causal claims about their relation cannot be made and the mediation model is affected similarly. These findings provide a basis for future research to examine the causal effects of self-affirmation on boredom. For instance, Nelson et al. (2014) found that a brief self-affirmation manipulation can boost perceptions of meaning in daily life, and self-affirmation interventions have been demonstrated to boost feelings of connectedness and belonging (Crocker et al., 2008; Hales et al., 2016), core aspects of what makes life meaningful (Baumeister, 1991; Hicks & King, 2009). Perceptions of meaning in life may be a causal mechanism through which self-affirmation promotes ample positive psychological outcomes such as less boredom.

Self-affirmation interventions may be an efficacious strategy for combatting both chronic and state boredom. Individuals can experience momentary instances of boredom without necessarily being prone to boredom (Elpidorou, 2014). Therefore, the utility of a self-affirmation manipulation for combatting state boredom also has practical relevance. Experimental, experience sampling, and longitudinal studies are needed to establish causality towards the development of interventions. There are likely several effective strategies to address chronic boredom (e.g. addressing attentional failures, Eastwood et al., 2012); addressing meaning perceptions is one such theoretically and empirically supported strategy. Drawing on the control-value theory of achievement emotions (Pekrun, 2006), affirming the perceived value of an activity for the self is likely to prevent and reduce boredom (Pekrun et al., 2010). Tailored value-affirmation interventions may be an effective tool for combatting boredom in applied settings. An investment into boredom mitigation strategies is essential for the well-being of both the individual and society.

In addition, the studies presented here rely on samples predominantly composed of US American participants. While we aspired to address this shortcoming by replicating our findings with an Irish sample (Study 2b), further testing of the hypotheses in non-Western samples is necessary to generalise our findings to other cultures and nationalities. Given the apparent novelty of the empirical study of spontaneous self-affirmation, cultural differences in the tendency to self-affirm have not yet been examined and may be present (as discussed above). We may expect that collective and individualistic cultures will experience psychological threats differently. For instance, in collectivist cultures, internal attributes are less integrated to one’s self-integrity than the individuals’ social roles and relationships (Heine & Lehman, 1997). Thus, cultural differences may moderate the way one spontaneously self-affirms. Lastly, future research may wish to use a common factor model with latent variables to further test the validity of the relationship between the variables.

Conclusion

Boredom proneness is linked to various psychological, physical, and behavioral issues, including low perceptions of meaning in life. Spontaneous self-affirmation has the potential to foster adaptive outcomes and defend against everyday psychological threats. Consistently, our results suggest that spontaneous self-affirmation is associated with an increase in perceptions of meaning in life and a decrease in boredom proneness. These findings have important research and practical implications. Individuals who are inclined to self-affirm through various sources (strengths, values, social relationships) benefit from feelings of meaning and less boredom. Spontaneous self-affirmation may prove a useful individual difference in coping with chronic boredom, although specific experimental and intervention research is needed to confirm this notion. While spontaneous self-affirmation cannot guarantee the physical manifestations of all your desires (as “Lucky Girl Syndrome” may suggest), our findings, in line with prior research, confirm that it can offer valuable psychological benefits.