Skip to main content
Log in

Difficulty of refusal to assist the outgroup nonmonotonically affects the intensity of prejudiced affect

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Building on emotional intensity theory (Brehm in Pers Soc Psychol Rev 3:2–22, 1999), we propose that difficulty of engaging in prejudiced behavior should nonmonotonically influence prejudiced affect. In two experiments, we informed anti-gay participants about a gay and lesbian student organization’s need for assistance. We operationalized refusal to help the organization as a behavioral tendency motivated by the experienced prejudiced affect. To manipulate difficulty of refusing to help, in Study 1, participants were offered an opportunity to help by volunteering either 6 h (easy to refuse to help), 2 h (moderately difficult to refuse), or ½ h (very difficult to refuse) per week. In Study 2, we used the same manipulation except that the participants in the very difficult to refuse condition were asked to volunteer ½ h every other week. In both experiments, participants in the control condition were asked to help but no amount of time was specified. As predicted, prejudiced affect was a cubic function of difficulty of refusal to help: affect decreased from the control to the easy condition, increased from the easy to the moderately difficult condition, and, in Study 2, decreased from the moderate to the very difficult condition. Implications of the findings and future directions for research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Sarah Seaman and Natalie Peters competently ran the participants for Study 1. The third author ran the majority of the participants for Study 2. Cassandra Tate also assisted with data collection.

  2. Given that we had directional hypotheses, 1-tailed tests rendered significant results for the targeted contrast, 1 −1 1 −1, for all dependent measures in Study 1, all ps < .04, 1-tailed, except for a marginally significant effect for the first prejudiced affect measure, p = .075, 1-tailed.

  3. We thank Jack Brehm for pointing out this potential problem.

  4. The mediation analyses followed the procedures outlined by Preacher and Hayes (2008), which test the significance of a mediated effect by using a bootstrapping procedure. The bootstrapping procedure creates bias-corrected confidence intervals. When the intervals do not contain the value of zero, an indirect effect is significant. Prejudiced affect mediated the effect of the difficulty manipulation on the strength of the justifications for not helping; CI95% = (.02; .14) when prejudiced affect toward the organization was the tested mediator, and CI95% = (.06; .30) when prejudiced affect against gays and lesbians was the mediator. In contrast, the strength of the justifications for not helping did not fully mediate the effect of the difficulty manipulation on prejudiced affect; CI95% = (.09; .1.17) when prejudiced affect toward the organization was the predicted variable and CI95% = (.15; 1.18) when prejudiced affect against gays and lesbians was the predicted outcome.

References

  • Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanchard, F. A., Crandall, C. S., Brigham, J. C., & Vaughn, L. A. (1994). Condemning and condoning racism: A social context approach to interracial settings. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 79, 993–997. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.79.6.993.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogardus, E. S. (1925). Measuring social distances. Journal of Applied Sociology, 9, 299–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W. (1999). The intensity of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 2–22. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0301_1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (1981). Psychological reactance: A theory of freedom and control. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W., Brummett, B. H., & Harvey, L. (1999). Paradoxical sadness. Motivation and Emotion, 23, 31–44. doi:10.1023/A:1021379317763.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W., & Miron, A. M. (2006). Can the simultaneous experience of opposing emotions really occur? Motivation and Emotion, 30, 13–30. doi:10.1007/s11031-006-9007-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W., Miron, A. M., & Miller, K. (2009). Affect as a motivational state. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1069–1089. doi:10.1080/02699930802323642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W., & Self, E. A. (1989). The intensity of motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 109–131. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.000545.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W., Wright, R. A., Solomon, S., Silka, L., & Greenberg, J. (1983). Perceived difficulty, energization, and the magnitude of goal valence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 21–48. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(83)90003-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conley, T. D., Evett, S. R., & Devine, P. G. (2007). Attitudes, subjective experiences, and behaviors in imagined and actual encounters between gay and heterosexual people. Journal of Homosexuality, 53, 35–63. doi:10.1300/J082v53n03_04.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crandall, C. S., & Eshleman, A. (2003). A justification-suppression model of the expression and experience of prejudice. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 414–446. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.414.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crandall, C. S., Eshleman, A., & O’Brien, L. (2002). Social norms and the expression and suppression of prejudice: The struggle for internalization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 359–378. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.82.3.359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Czopp, A. M., Monteith, M. J., & Mark, A. Y. (2006). Standing up for a change: Reducing bias through interpersonal confrontation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 784–803. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.784.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Augelli, A. R. (1992). Lesbian and gay male undergraduates’ experiences of harassment and fear on campus. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7, 383–395. doi:10.1177/088626092007003007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, N., & Rivera, L. M. (2008). When social context matters: The influence of long-term contact and short-term exposure to admired outgroup members on implicit attitudes and behavioral intentions. Social Cognition, 26, 112–123. doi:10.1521/soco.2008.26.1.112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Esses, V. M., Beach, K. R., & Gaertner, S. (2002). The role of affect in determining intergroup behavior: The case of willingness to engage in intergroup contact. In D. M. Mackie & E. R. Smith (Eds.), From prejudice to intergroup emotions: Differentiated reactions to social groups (pp. 153–171). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1964). Conflict, decision, and dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, A. H., Manstead, A. S. R., & Zaalberg, R. (2003). Social influences on the emotion process. European Review of Social Psychology, 14, 171–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878–902. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (2006). The laws of emotion. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuegen, K., & Brehm, J. W. (2004). The intensity of affect and resistance to social influence. In E. S. Knowles & J. A. Linn (Eds.), Resistance and persuasion (pp. 39–63). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 61–89). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2005). Understanding and addressing contemporary racism: From aversive racism to the common ingroup identity model. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 615–639. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00424.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gailliot, M. T., Peruche, B. M., Plant, E. A., & Baumeister, R. F. (2009). Stereotypes and prejudice in the blood: Sucrose drinks reduce prejudice and stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 288–290. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.09.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Richter, M. (2006). Ego-involvement and the difficulty law of motivation: Effects on performance-related cardiovascular response. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1188–1203. doi:10.1177/0146167206288945.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Wright, R. A. (2005). Motivation in social settings: Studies of effort-related cardiovascular arousal. In J. P. Forgas, K. Williams, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), Social motivation (pp. 71–90). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glynn, L. M., Christenfeld, N., & Gerin, W. (2002). The role of rumination in recovery from reactivity: Cardiovascular consequences of emotional states. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 714–726. doi:10.1097/01.PSY.0000031574.42041.23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hebl, M. R., Foster, J. B., Mannix, L. M., & Dovidio, J. F. (2002). Formal and interpersonal discrimination: A field study of bias toward homosexual applicants. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 815–825. doi:10.1177/0146167202289010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herek, G. M. (2000). The psychology of sexual prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 19–22. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herek, G. M. (2009). Sexual prejudice. In T. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice (pp. 439–465). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewstone, M., Hopkins, N., & Routh, D. A. (1992). Cognitive models of stereotype change (1): Generalization and subtyping in young people’s view of the police. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 219–234. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420220303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, M. E., & Augoustinos, M. (2001). Stereotype change and prejudice reduction: Short- and long-term evaluation of a cross-cultural awareness programme. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 11, 243–262. doi:10.1002/casp.629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jussim, L., Nelson, T. E., Manis, M., & Soffin, S. (1995). Prejudice, stereotypes, and labeling effects: Sources of bias in person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 228–246. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.2.228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review, 82, 1–25. doi:10.1037/h0076171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science: Selected papers. New York: Harper & Brothers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowery, B. S., Hardin, C. D., & Sinclair, S. (2001). Social influence on automatic racial prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 842–855. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.842.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mackie, D. M., Maitner, A. T., & Smith, E. R. (2009). Intergroup emotions theory. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 285–308). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2003). From prejudice to intergroup emotions. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Milne, A. B. (1998). Saying ‘no’ to unwanted thoughts: Self-focus and the regulation of mental life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 578–589. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.578.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., & Jetten, J. (1994). Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 808–817. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.5.808.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maitner, A. T., Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2006). Evidence for the regulatory function of intergroup emotion: Emotional consequences of implemented or impeded intergroup action tendencies. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 720–728. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.08.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masser, B., & Moffat, K. B. (2006). With friends like these…the role of prejudice and situational norms on discriminatory helping behavior. Journal of Homosexuality, 51, 121–138. doi:10.1300/J082v51n02_07.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miron, A. M., & Brehm, J. W. (2006). Reactance theory—40 years later. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 37, 9–18. doi:10.1024/0044-3514.37.1.9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miron, A. M., & Brehm, J. W. (in press). The emotional intensity theory and its cardiovascular implications for emotional states. In R. A. Wright & G. H. E. Gendolla (Eds.), How motivation affects cardiovascular response: Mechanisms and applications. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  • Miron, A. M., Brummett, B. H., Ruggles, R., & Brehm, J. W. (2008). Deterring anger and anger-motivated behaviors. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 30, 326–338. doi:10.1080/01973530802502259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miron, A. M., Knepfel, D., & Parkinson, S. K. (2009). The surprising effect of partner flaws and qualities on romantic affect. Motivation and Emotion, 33, 261–276. doi:10.1007/sl1031-009-9138-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miron, A. M., & Pantaleo, G. (2010). The paradoxical effect of deterrence on emotional intensity: Why less can be better than more. Manuscript submitted for publication, University of Wisconsin Oshkosh.

  • Miron, A. M., Parkinson, S. K., & Brehm, J. W. (2007). Does happiness have a motivational function? Cognition and Emotion, 21, 248–267. doi:10.1080/02699930600551493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monteith, M. J., Sherman, J., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Suppression as a stereotype control strategy. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 63–82. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0201_4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myrseth, K. O., Fishbach, A., & Trope, Y. (2009). Counteractive self-control: When making temptation available makes temptation less tempting. Psychological Science, 20, 159–163. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02268.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. D. (Ed.). (2009). Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pantaleo, G., Rossi, A., Veneziani, C., Mondini, D., & Selvaggio, F. (2008). On the intensity of intentions: The paradoxical role of “deterrents.” Presentation given at the Department of Psychology Colloquium, University of Geneva, Switzerland.

  • Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (2001). Responses to other-imposed pro-Black pressure: Acceptance or backlash? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 486–501. doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2003). When prejudice does not pay: Effects of interracial contact on executive function. Psychological Science, 14, 287–290. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.03437.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, M., Friedrich, A., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2008). Task difficulty effects on cardiac activity. Psychophysiology, 45, 869–875. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00688.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., Ashmore, R. D., & Gary, M. L. (2001). “Unlearning” automatic biases: The malleability of implicit prejudice and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 856–868. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.856.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sears, D. O, & Henry, P. J. (2005). Over thirty years later: A contemporary look at symbolic racism and its critics. In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 37, pp. 95–150). New York: Academic Press.

  • Silvia, P. J., & Brehm, J. W. (2001). Exploring alternative deterrents to emotional intensity: Anticipated happiness, distraction, and sadness. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 575–592. doi:10.1080/02699930143000040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2008). Intergroup emotions. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. Feldman Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 428–439). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stangor, C., Sullivan, L. A., & Ford, T. E. (1991). Affective and cognitive determinants of prejudice. Social Cognition, 9, 359–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1985). Intergroup anxiety. Journal of Social Issues, 41, 157–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swim, J. K., Ferguson, M. J., & Hyers, L. L. (1999). Avoiding stigma by association: Subtle prejudice against lesbians in the form of social distancing. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 61–68. doi:10.1207/15324839951036560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. C. (2005). Explaining the nature of power: A three-process theory. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 1–22. doi:10.1002/ejsp.244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, R. N., Crisp, R. J., & Lambert, E. (2007). Imagining intergroup contact can improve intergroup attitudes. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 10, 427–441. doi:10.1177/1368430207081533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.1.34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wicklund, R. A., & Brehm, J. W. (1976). Perspectives on cognitive dissonance. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, H. F. (1937). The influence of barriers upon strength of motivation. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A. (2008). Refining the prediction of effort: Brehm’s distinction between potential motivation and motivation intensity. Social and Personality Psychology Compass: Motivation and Emotion, 2, 682–701. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00093.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Schulz, R. (2003). The importance of goal disengagement in adaptive self-regulation: When giving up is beneficial. Self and Identity, 2, 1–20. doi:10.1080/15298860309021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Faculty Development Grant from the University of Wisconsin Oshkosh awarded to the first author. Portions of this research were presented at the Midwestern Psychological Association meeting in May 2009.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anca M. Miron.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Miron, A.M., Ferguson, M.A. & Peterson, A. Difficulty of refusal to assist the outgroup nonmonotonically affects the intensity of prejudiced affect. Motiv Emot 35, 484–498 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9220-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9220-2

Keywords

Navigation